Skip to main content
Research Article

Exaggeration is Harder Than Understatement, but Practice Makes Perfect!

Faking Success in the IAT

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000114

Previous research on the fakeability of the Implicit Association Test (IAT) yielded inconsistent results. The present study simultaneously analyses several relevant factors: faking direction, type of instructions, and practice. Furthermore, it takes baseline individual differences into account. After a baseline assessment in a self-esteem IAT without faking instructions (t0), participants in the faking conditions then (t1) faked high or low scores without being provided with recommended strategies on how to do so (i.e., individual strategies). At t2 and t3, they were asked to fake the IAT after having received information on recommended faking strategies. At t4, faking direction was reversed. Without the recommended strategies, faking high scores was not possible, but faking low scores was. With the recommended strategies, participants needed additional practice to fake high scores. When faking directions were reversed, participants were successful without additional practice, suggesting a transfer in faking skills. In most of the faking attempts, faking success was moderated by individual differences in baseline implicit self-esteem. This suggests that the complex interplay of factors influencing faking success should be taken into account when considering the issue of fakeability of the IAT.

References

  • Aiken, L. , West, S. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Retrieved from PsycINFO database. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Asendorpf, J. , Banse, R. , Mücke, D. (2002). Double dissociation between implicit and explicit personality self-concept: The case of shy behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 380–393. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.380. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Banse, R. , Seise, J. , Zerbes, N. (2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality: Reliability, validity, and controllability of the IAT. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 145–160. doi: 10.1026/0949-3946.48.2.145. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Boon, J. , Gozna, L. , Hall, S. (2008). Detecting “faking bad” on the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 263–272. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2007.08.005. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Bühner, M. , Ziegler, M. (2009). Statistik für Psychologen und Sozialwissenschaftler. [Statistics for Psychologists and Social Scientists] München, Germany: Pearson. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Cohen, J. , Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analyses for behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Conrey, F. R. , Sherman, J. W. , Gawronski, B. , Hugenberg, K. , Groom, C. J. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The Quad Model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 469–487. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.469. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Dalen, L. , Stanton, N. , Roberts, A. (2001). Faking personality questionnaires in personnel selection. Journal of Management Development, 20, 729–742. doi: 10.1108/02621710110401428. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them. In R. W. Wiers, A. W. Stacy (Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11–28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. , Beckers, T. , Moors, A. (2007). Novel attitudes can be faked on the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 972–978. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2006.10.007. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • De Houwer, J. , Moors, A. (2007). How to define and examine the implicitness of implicit measures. In B. Wittenbrink, N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes: Procedures and controversies (pp. 179–194). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Retrieved from PsycINFO database. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Egloff, B. , Schmukle, S. (2002). Predictive validity of an Implicit Association Test for assessing anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1441–1455. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1441. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Fiedler, K. , Bluemke, M. (2005). Faking the IAT: Aided and unaided response control on the Implicit Association Tests. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 307–316. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp2704_3. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Gawronski, B. (2002). What does the Implicit Association Test measure? A test of the convergent and discriminant validity of prejudice-related IATs. Experimental Psychology, 49, 171–180. doi: 10.1026/1618-3169.49.3.171. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. , Farnham, S. (2000). Using the Implicit Association Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1022–1038. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. , McGhee, D. , Schwartz, J. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. , Nosek, B. , Banaji, M. (2003a). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A. , Nosek, B. , Banaji, M. (2003b). “Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm”: Correction to Greenwald et al. (2003). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 481. doi: 10.1037/h0087889. First citation in articleMedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Kim, D. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 83–96. doi: 10.2307/3090143. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Lowery, B. S. , Hardin, C. D. , Sinclair, S. (2001). Social influence effects on automatic racial prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 842–855. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.842. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Marcus, B. (2004). Von sozialer Erwünschtheit als Problem, als Phänomen und als Chance der Eignungsdiagnostik. [Social desirability as a problem, a phenomenon, and a chance in the personnel selection].Zeitschrift für Personalpsychologie, 3, 122–127. doi: 10.1026/1617-6391.3.3.122. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • McDaniel, M. , Beier, M. , Perkins, A. , Goggin, S. , Frankel, B. (2009). An assessment of the fakeability of self-report and implicit personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 682–685. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2009.01.011. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • McFarland, L. , Ryan, A. (2000). Variance in faking across noncognitive measures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 812–821. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.85.5.812. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Mierke, J. , Klauer, K. (2001). Implicit association measurement with the IAT: Evidence for effects of executive control processes. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 107–122. Retrieved from PSYNDEX: Literature and Audiovisual Media with PSYNDEX Tests database. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Mueller-Hanson, R. , Heggestad, E. , Thornton, G. (2006). Individual differences in impression management: An exploration of the psychological processes underlying faking. Psychology Science, 48, 288–312. Retrieved from PsycINFO database. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Musch, J. , Brockhaus, R. , Bröder, A. (2002). Ein Inventar zur Erfassung von zwei Faktoren sozialer Erwünschtheit. [An inventory for the assessment of two factors of social desirability]. Diagnostica, 48, 121–129. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.48.3.121. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Nosek, B. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565–584. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.134.4.565. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Paulhus, D. , Fridhandler, B. , Hayes, S. (1997). Psychological defense: Contemporary theory and research. In J. Johnson, R. Hogan, S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 543–579). New York, NY: Academic Press. doi: 10.1016/B978-012134645-4/50023-8. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rothermund, K. , Wentura, D. (2001). Figure-ground asymmetries in the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 94–106. doi: 10.1026/0949-3946.48.2.94. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Rudolph, A. , Schröder, M. , Schütz, A. (2006). Ein Impliziter Assoziationstest zur Erfassung von Selbstwertschätzung. In T. M. Ortner, R. T. Proyer, K. D. Kubinger (Eds.), [An Implicit Association Test of self-esteem] Theorie und Praxis Objektiver Persönlichkeitstests (pp. 153–163). Bern, Switzerland: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Rudolph, A. , Schröder-Abé, M. , Schütz, A. , Gregg, A. , Sedikides, C. (2008). Through a glass, less darkly? Reassessing convergent and discriminant validity in measures of implicit self-esteem. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 273–281. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.24.4.273. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt-Atzert, L. , Bühner, M. , Rischen, S. , Warkentin, V. (2004). Erkennen von Simulation und Dissimulation im Test d2. [Detecting simulation and dissimulation in the d2]. Diagnostica, 50, 124–133. doi: 10.1026/0012-1924.50.3.124. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schmit, M. , Ryan, A. (1993). The Big Five in personnel selection: Factor structure in applicant and nonapplicant populations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 966–974. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.78.6.966. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schnabel, K. , Asendorpf, J. , Greenwald, A. (2008). Using Implicit Association Tests for the assessment of implicit personality self-concept. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment, Vol 2: Personality measurement and testing (pp. 508–528). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Retrieved from PsycINFO database. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steffens, M. (2004). Is the Implicit Association Test immune to faking? Experimental Psychology, 51, 165–179. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.51.3.165. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Steffens, M. , Plewe, I. (2001). Items’ cross-category associations as a confounding factor in the Implicit Association Test. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 123–134. doi: 10.1026/0949-3946.48.2.123. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Strack, F. , Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1. First citation in articleCrossref MedlineGoogle Scholar

  • Tulbure, B. (2006). Dissimulating anxiety in front of the Implicit Association Test (IAT). Cogniţie Creier Comportament, 10, 559–579. Retrieved from PsycINFO database. University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • von Collani, G. , Herzberg, P. (2003). Eine revidierte Fassung der deutschsprachigen Skala zum Selbstwertgefühl von Rosenberg. [A revised version of the German Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle und Diagnostische Psychologie, 24, 3–7. doi: 10.1024/0170-1789.24.1.3. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Wallaert, M. , Ward, A. , Mann, T. (2010). Explicit control of implicit responses. Simple directives can alter IAT performance. Social Psychology, 41, 152–157. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000022. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Weiner, I. B. , Freedheim, D. K. , Schinka, J. A. , Velicer, W. F. (2003). Handbook of psychology: Research methods in psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar