Skip to main content
Articles

Electrophysiological Indices of Processing Symmetry and Aesthetics

A Result of Judgment Categorization or Judgment Report?

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.21.1.9

Abstract. Evaluative aesthetic judgments and descriptive symmetry judgments were compared. Electrophysiological activity was recorded while participants judged the aesthetic value or the symmetry status of novel graphic black and white patterns. In order to experimentally separate judgment categorization processes and judgment report processes, participants were instructed to misreport their true actual judgment in half of the trials. Three effects found in a previous study were examined: (1) an early frontocentral effect for the evaluation of not-beautiful patterns reflecting an early impression formation, (2) a more pronounced ERP lateralization to the right for the aesthetic judgment task in comparison to the symmetry judgment task reflecting evaluative categorization, and (3) a sustained posterior effect for the visual analysis of symmetric patterns. In this study, (1) and (3) were replicated independent of the validity of the response, but (2) was affected by the validity, i.e., the effect was abolished in the false condition. Thus, results allowed further specification of cognitive processes involved in judgments of symmetry or aesthetics. Given present data, the ERP effects predominantly reflect judgment categorization and not judgment report.

References

  • Allen, J.J. , Iacono, W.G. (1997). A comparison of methods for the analysis of event-related potentials in deception detection. Psychophysiology, 34, 234– 240 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Barcelo, F. , Perianez, J.A. , Knight, R.T. (2002). Think differently: A brain orienting response to task novelty. Neuroreport, 13, 1887– 1892 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berlyne, D.E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value. Perception & Psychophysics, 8(5-A), 279– 286 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Berlyne, D.E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology . East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Berlyne, D.E. (1974). Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation . Oxford: Hemisphere First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Brass, M. , Ullsperger, M. , Knoesche, T.R. , von Cramon, D.Y. , Phillips, N.A. (2005). Who comes first? The role of the prefrontal and parietal cortex in cognitive control. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1367– 1375 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J.T. , Crites, S.L. , Berntson, G.G. , Coles, M.G. (1993). If attitudes affect how stimuli are processed, should they not affect the event-related brain potential?. Psychological Science, 4, 108– 112 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J.T. , Crites, S.L. , Gardner, W.L. (1996). Attitudes to the right: Evaluative processing is associated with lateralized late positive event-related brain potentials. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1205– 1219 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J.T. , Crites, S.L. , Gardner, W.L. , Berntson, G.G. (1994). Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorizations: I. A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of trait negativity and extremity. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 67, 115– 125 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cacioppo, J.T. , Petty, R.E. , Losch, M.W. , Crites, S.L. (1994). Psychophysiological approaches to attitudes: Detecting affective dispositions when people won't say, can't say, or don't even know. In S. Shavitt & T.C. Brock (Eds.),Persuasion: Psychological insights and perspectives (pp. 43-69). New York: Allyn and Bacon First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Christensen, C.A. , Ivkovich, D. , Drake, K.J. (2001). Late positive ERP peaks observed in stimulus-response compatibility tasks tested under speed-accuracy instructions. Psychophysiology, 38, 404– 416 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Crites, S.L. (1995). Event-related brain potentials as a function of evaluative and nonevaluative judgment processes: A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of categorization rather than response operations. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering, 55(10-B), 4640– First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Crites, S.L. , Cacioppo, J.T. (1996). Electrocortical differentiation of evaluative and nonevaluative categorizations. Psychological Science, 7, 318– 321 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Crites, S.L. , Cacioppo, J.T. , Gardner, W.L. , Berntson, G.G. (1995). Bioelectrical echoes from evaluative categorization: II. A late positive brain potential that varies as a function of attitude registration rather than attitude report. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 68, 997– 1013 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cunningham, W.A. , Espinet, S.D. , DeYoung, C.G. , Zelazo, P.D. (2005). Attitudes to the right and left: Frontal ERP asymmetries associated with stimulus valence and processing goals. Neuroimage, 28, 827– 834 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cunningham, W.A. , Johnson, M.K. , Gatenby, J.C. , Gore, J.C. , Banaji, M.R. (2003). Neural components of social evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 639– 649 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cunningham, W.A. , Raye, C.L. , Johnson, M.K. (2004). Implicit and explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of valence, emotional intensity, and control in the processing of attitudes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1717– 1729 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Day, H.Y. (1968). The importance of symmetry and complexity in the evaluation of complexity, interest, and pleasingness. Psychonomic Science, 10, 339– 340 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenman, R. (1967). Complexity-simplicity: I. Preference for symmetry and rejection of complexity. Psychonomic Science, 8, 169– 170 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eisenman, R. , Gellens, H.K. (1968). Preference for complexity-simplicity and symmetry-asymmetry. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 26, 888– 890 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Farrow, T.F. , Reilly, R. , Rahman, T.A. , Herford, A.E. , Woodruff, P.W. , Spence, S.A. (2003). Sex and personality traits influence the difference between time taken to tell the truth or lie. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 97, 451– 460 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Farwell, L.A. , Donchin, E. (1991). The truth will out: Interrogative polygraphy (“lie detection”) with event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology, 28, 531– 547 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Fechner, G.T. (1876). Vorschule der Aesthetik . [Experimental Aesthetics; “Preschool” of aesthetics]. Leipzig: Breitkopf & Härtel First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Greenwald, A.G. , McGhee, D.E. , Schwartz, J.L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464– 1480 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Höfel, L. , Jacobsen, T. (2003). Temporal stability and consistency of aesthetic judgments of beauty of formal graphic patterns. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 96, 30– 32 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Höfel, L. , Jacobsen, T. (in preparation). Electrophysiological indices of processing aesthetics: Spontaneous or intentional processes? . First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ito, T.A. , Cacioppo, J.T. (2000). Electrophysiological evidence of implicit and explicit categorization processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 660– 676 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobsen, T. (2004). Individual and group modeling of aesthetic judgment strategies. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 41– 56 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobsen, T. (2006). Bridging the arts and the sciences: A framework for the psychology of aesthetics. Leonardo, 39, 155– 162 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobsen, T. , Buchta, K. , Köhler, M. , Schröger, E. (2004). A primacy of beauty: The aesthetics of things. Psychological Reports, 94, 1253– 1260 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobsen, T. , Höfel, L. (2002). Aesthetic judgments of novel graphic patterns: Analyses of individual judgments. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 95, 755– 766 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobsen, T. , Höfel, L. (2003). Descriptive and evaluative judgment processes: Behavioral and electrophysiological indices of processing symmetry and aesthetics. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 289– 299 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jacobsen, T. , Schubotz, R. , Höfel, L. , von Cramon, D.Y. (2006). Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty. NeuroImage, 29, 276– 285 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Magliero, A. , Bashore, T.R. , Coles, M.G. , Donchin, E. (1984). On the dependence of P300 latency on stimulus evaluation processes. Psychophysiology, 21, 171– 186 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Novak, G.P. , Ritter, W. , Vaughan, H.G. (1992). Mismatch detection and the latency of temporal judgments. Psychophysiology, 29, 398– 411 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Novak, G.P. , Ritter, W. , Vaughan, H.G. , Wiznitzer, M.L. (1990). Differentiation of negative event-related potentials in an auditory discrimination task. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 75, 255– 275 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97– 113 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Pfefferbaum, A. , Christensen, C. , Ford, J.M. , Kopell, B.S. (1986). Apparent response incompatibility effects on P3 latency depend on the task. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 64, 424– 437 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ritterfeld, U. (2002). Social heuristics in interior design preferences. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 22, 369– 386 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenfeld, J.P. , Biroschak, J.R. , Furedy, J.J. (2006). P300-based detection of concealed autobiographical versus incidentally acquired information in target and nontarget paradigms. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 60, 251– 259 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Rosenfeld, J.P. , Soskins, M. , Bosh, G. , Ryan, A. (2004). Simple, effective countermeasures to P300-based tests of detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology, 41, 205– 219 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schroger, E. (1998). Measurement and interpretation of the mismatch negativity. Behavior Research Methods, 30, 131– 145 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Seymour, T.L. , Seifert, C.M. , Shafto, M.G. , Mosmann, A.L. (2000). Using response time measures to assess “guilty knowledge. rdquo; Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 30– 37 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Tyler, C.W. , Baseler, H.A. , Kontsevich, L.L. , Likova, L.T. , Wade, A.R. , Wandell, B.A. (2005). Predominantly extra-retinotopic cortical response to pattern symmetry. Neuroimage, 24, 306– 314 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • van der Zwan, R. , Leo, E. , Joung, W. , Latimer, C. , Wenderoth, P. (1998). Evidence that both area V1 and extrastriate visual cortex contribute to symmetry perception. Current Biology, 8, 889– 892 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar