Skip to main content
Original Article

Attention Tests in Different Stimulus Presentation Modes

A Facet Model of Performance in Attention Tests

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000085

Tests of attention are frequently used in clinical psychology, neuropsychology, and other applied settings. Depending on the assessment tradition, attention tests are designed and administered differently. Among other aspects, assessment traditions enable testtakers to conduct the tests at either their own pace or at a pace set by the test developer, and apply either rows of stimuli or single stimuli. The current study systematically varied these aspects and examined how these variations affected the structure of attention tests and their correlation with reasoning. A facet model was established that included a facet related to the way attention tests are designed and administered as well as a facet related to the attention ability assessed. Results revealed that both facets explained variance in test performance. Moreover, results revealed that the facet-related test design and administration yielded higher correlations with reasoning than the ability facet. A specific combination of pace and stimulus configuration showed the highest correlations with the applied reasoning tests. Implications for future research and practice are discussed.

References

  • Arbuckle, J. L. (2009). Amos 18.0 [Computer software]. Spring House, PA: Amos Development Corp. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Beauducel, A. , Wittmann, W. W. (2005). Simulation study on fit indexes in CFA based on data with slightly distorted simple structure. Structural Equation Modeling, 12, 41–75. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Brickenkamp, R. (2002). Test d2 – Aufmerksamkeits-Belastungs-Test [The test d2 – A timed test of selective attention]. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Carpenter, P. A. , Just, M. A. , Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: A theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological Review, 97, 404–431. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Colombo, J. (2001). The development of visual attention in infancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 337–367. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Danthiir, V. , Wilhelm, O. , Schulze, R. , Roberts, R. D. (2005). Factor structure and validity of paper-and-pencil measures of mental speed: Evidence for a higher-order model? Intelligence, 33, 491–514. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eid, M. (2000). A multitrait-multimethod model with minimal assumptions. Psychometrika, 65, 241–261. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Embretson, E. (2005). Measuring human intelligence with artificial intelligence: Adaptive item generation. In J. E. Pretz R. J. Sternberg, Eds., Cognition and intelligence: Identifying the mechanisms of the mind (pp. 251–267). New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Flehmig, H. C. , Steinborn, M. B. , Westhoff, K. , Langner, R. (2010). Neuroticism and speed-accuracy tradeoff in serial mental addition and comparison performance. Journal of Individual Differences, 31, 130–137. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Goldhammer, F. , Moosbrugger, H. , Schweizer, K. (2007). On the separability of cognitive abilities related to Posner’s attention components. European Psychologist, 12, 103–118. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Guttman, L. (1954). A new approach to factor analysis: The radex. In P. F. Lazarsfeld, (Ed.), Mathematical thinking in the social sciences (pp. 258–348). Glencoe, IL: Free Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Hagemeister, C. , Westhoff, K. (2011). Konzentrationsdiagnostik [Assessment of concentration]. In L. Hornke M. Amelang M. Kersting, Eds., Enzyklopädie der Psychologie, Psychologische Diagnostik, Band 3: Leistungs-, Intelligenz- und Verhaltensdiagnostik (pp. 51–96). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Horn, J. L. , Noll, J. (1994). System for understanding cognitive capabilities: A theory and the evidence on which it is based. In D. K. Detterman, (Ed.), Current topics in human intelligence: Vol. 4. Theories of intelligence (pp. 151–203). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jensen, A. R. (1982). Reaction time and psychometric g. In H. J. Eysenck, (Ed.), A model for intelligence (pp. 93–132). New York: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Jensen, A. R. (2006). Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences. Oxford: Elsevier. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Jensen, A. R. (2011). The theory of intelligence and its measurement. Intelligence, 39, 171–177. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Krumm, S. , Schmidt-Atzert, L. , Eschert, S. (2008). Investigating the structure of attention: How do test characteristics of paper-pencil sustained attention tests influence their relationship with other attention tests? European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 108–116. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Krumm, S. , Schmidt-Atzert, L. , Michalczyk, K. , Danthiir, V. (2008). Sustained attention and mental speed abilities: The same or different constructs? Journal of Individual Differences, 29, 205–216. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Marschner, G. (1980). Revisions-Test [Revision test]. Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Mirsky, A. F. , Anthony, B. J. , Duncan, C. C. , Ahearn, M. B. , Kellam, S. G. (1991). Analysis of the elements of attention: A neuropsychological approach. Neuropsychology Review, 2, 109–145. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Moosbrugger, H. , Goldhammer, F. , Schweizer, K. (2006). Latent factors underlying individual differences in attention measures: Perceptual and executive attention. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22, 177–188. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Neumann, O. (1996). Theorien der Aufmerksamkeit [Theories of attention]. In O. Neumann A. F. Sanders, Eds., Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Bd. C II 2: Aufmerksamkeit (pp. 559–643). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nussbeck, F. W. , Eid, M. , Geiser, C. , Courvoisier, D. S. , Lischetzke, T. (2009). A CTC(M–1) model for different types of raters. Methodology, 5, 88–98. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Park, N. W. , Ingles, J. L. (2001). Effectiveness of attention rehabilitation after an acquired brain injury: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychology, 15, 199–210. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Posner, M. I. , Rafal, R. D. (1987). Cognitive theories of attention and the rehabilitation of attentional deficits. In M. J. Meier A. L. Benton L. Diller, Eds., Neuropsychological rehabilitation (pp. 182–201). Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Prinzel, M. (2006). Prädiktoren von Ausbildungserfolg am Beispiel von Schulnoten: Intelligenz, Persönlichkeit und Leistungsmotivation [Predictors of trainings success exemplified by school grades: Intelligence, personality, and achievement motivation]. Unpublished diploma thesis, University of Marburg. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Raven, J. C. (1962). Advanced progressive matrices. London: Lewis & Co. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt-Atzert, L. (2007). Objektiver Leistungsmotivations-Test, OLMT [Objective achievement motivation test, OLMT]. Mödling, Austria: Schuhfried. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt-Atzert, L. , Bühner, M. , Enders, P. (2006). Messen Konzentrationstests Konzentration? Eine Analyse von Konzentrationstestleistungen [Do concentration tests assess concentration? Analysis of the components of concentration test performances]. Diagnostica, 52, 33–44. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schweizer, K. (2005). An overview of research into the cognitive basis of intelligence. Journal of Individual Differences, 26, 43–51. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Schweizer, K. (2010). The relationship of attention and intelligence. In A. Gruszka G. Mathews B. Szymura, Eds., Handbook of individual differences in cognition: Attention, memory, and executive control (pp. 247–262). New York: Springer. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schweizer, K. , Moosbrugger, H. (2004). Attention and working memory as predictors of intelligence. Intelligence, 32, 329–347. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Schweizer, K. , Moosbrugger, H. , Goldhammer, F. (2005). The structure of the relationship between attention and intelligence. Intelligence, 33, 589–611. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sheppard, L. D. , Vernon, P. A. (2008). Intelligence and speed of information-processing: A review of 50 years of research. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 535–551. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Stankov, L. (1988). Aging, attention, and intelligence. Psychology and Aging, 3, 59–74. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Steinborn, M. B. , Flehmig, H. C. , Westhoff, K. , Langner, R. (2009). Differential effects of prolonged work on performance measures in self-paced speed tests. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 5, 105–113. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Sturm, W. (2006). Wahrnehmungs- und Aufmerksamkeitsfunktionen [Functions of perception and attention]. Moedling, Austria: Schuhfried. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Sturm, W. , Zimmermann, P. (2000). Aufmerksamkeitsstörungen [Attention deficits]. In W. Sturm, M. Herrmann, C. W. Wallesch, (Hrsg.), Lehrbuch der Klinischen Neuropsychologie: Grundlagen, Methoden, Diagnostik, Therapie (pp. 345–365). Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Styles, E. A. (1997). The psychology of attention. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Westhoff, K. (1995). Aufmerksamkeit und Konzentration [Attention and concentration]. In M. Amelang, (Ed.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Bd. C VIII 2, Verhaltens- und Leistungsunterschiede (pp. 375–402). Göttingen: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Westhoff, K. , Graubner, J. (2003). Konstruktion eines komplexen Konzentrationstests. Diagnostica, 49, 110–119. First citation in articleLinkGoogle Scholar

  • Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomic Science, 3, 159–177. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Zimmermann, P. , Fimm, B. (1993). Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung (TAP) [Testbattery for Attentional Performance, TAP]. Würselen, Germany: Vera Fimm Psychologische Testsysteme. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar