Skip to main content

Unintentional non-adherence to chronic prescription medications: How unintentional is it really?

Abstract

Background

Unintentional non-adherence has been characterized as passively inconsistent medication-taking behavior (forgetfulness or carelessness). Our objectives were to: (1) study the prevalence and predictors of unintentional non-adherence; and (2) explore the interrelationship between intentional and unintentional non-adherence in relation to patients’ medication beliefs.

Methods

We conducted a cross-sectional survey of adults with asthma, hypertension, diabetes, hyperlipidemia, osteoporosis, or depression from the Harris Interactive Chronic Illness Panel. The analytic sample for this study included 24,017 adults who self-identified themselves as persistent to prescription medications for their index disease. They answered three questions on unintentional non-adherence (forgot, ran out, being careless), 11 questions on intentional non-adherence, and three multi-item scales assessing perceived need for medication (k = 10), perceived medication concerns (k = 6), and perceived medication affordability (k = 4). Logistic regression was used to model predictors of each unintentional non-adherence behavior. Baron and Kenny’s regression approach was used to test the mediational effect of unintentional non-adherence on the relationship between medication beliefs and intentional non-adherence. Bootstrapping was employed to confirm the statistical significance of these results.

Results

For the index disease, 62% forgot to take a medication, 37% had run out of the medication, and 23% were careless about taking the medication. Common multivariate predictors (p < .001) of the three behaviors were: (1) lower perceived need for medications; (2) more medication affordability problems; (3) worse self-rated health; (4) diabetes or osteoporosis (relative to hypertension); and (5) younger age. Unique statistically-significant predictors of the three behaviors were: (a) ‘forgot to take medications’ - greater concerns about the index medication and male gender; (b) ‘run out of medications’ - non-white race, asthma, and higher number of total prescription medications; (c) ‘being careless’ - greater medication concerns. Mediational tests confirmed the hypothesis that the effect of medication beliefs (perceived need, concerns, and affordability) on intentional non-adherence is mediated through unintentional non-adherence.

Conclusions

For our study sample, unintentional non-adherence does not appear to be random and is predicted by medication beliefs, chronic disease, and sociodemographics. The data suggests that the importance of unintentional non-adherence may lie in its potential prognostic significance for future intentional non-adherence. Health care providers may consider routinely inquiring about unintentional non-adherence in order to proactively address patients’ suboptimal medication beliefs before they choose to discontinue therapy all together.

Peer Review reports

Background

Medication adherence refers to the extent of conformity to treatment recommendations with respect to the timing, dosage, frequency, and duration of a prescribed medication [1]. Non-adherence to chronic medications is an Achilles’ heel of evidence-based medicine and is one of the major reasons why patients fail to reach their clinical goals, resulting in suboptimal health outcomes [2, 3]. In an effort to better understand the underlying causes of patients’ medication-taking behaviors, researchers have differentiated between two types of non-adherence. Intentional non-adherence is an active decision on the part of patients to forego prescribed therapy [4, 5]. Unintentional non-adherence, on the other hand, is a passive process whereby patients fail to adhere to prescribing instructions through forgetfulness, carelessness, or circumstances out of their control (e.g., health literacy) [6, 7]. Patients can, and often do, exhibit both types of non-adherent behaviors [810]. Intentional non-adherence may be demonstrated through non-fulfillment of a new prescription or through discontinuation of an existing medication therapy without the advice of a provider. Approximately 15% of patients do not fill a new prescription [11]; of those who do fill a new prescription, roughly 50% discontinue therapy in the first six months [3, 12, 13]. Estimates of unintentional non-adherence vary considerably and range from 20% to over 50% [7, 8, 10, 1423].

Research over the past 20 years has consistently shown that intentional non-adherence is driven by patient beliefs about their treatment, disease, and prognosis as well as their objective experiences with medications [2427]. Some early research on unintentional non-adherence suggested it was more strongly correlated with demographic characteristics [6, 7, 28, 29] than with medication knowledge or beliefs. Recent work, however, indicates that that there may be more to unintentional non-adherence than pure forgetfulness or carelessness [10, 15, 20, 21, 30, 31]. Patients reporting intentional versus unintentional non-adherence have been found to be similar to one another in terms of their adherence-related knowledge and motivation [31]. Unintentional non-adherence has also been recently linked to perceived need for medications [10, 30], medication concerns [15, 19, 21], and beliefs about treatment efficacy [20], which suggests that interventions focusing on patients’ medication beliefs may be required to address both intentional and unintentional non-adherence.

Although recent studies have begun to clear the veil on unintentional non-adherence, the generalizability of the evidence to date is limited by small samples in niche populations. A majority of these results were reported in samples of about 150 patients or less [9, 10, 15, 20, 30], typically for a single condition [9, 10, 15, 20, 31], and usually with a single-item measure of unintentional non-adherence [15, 20, 30, 31]. The one study that was conducted in multiple chronic diseases with somewhat of a larger sample only targeted Medicare enrollees and excluded uninsured patients or those enrolled primarily in private/employer-sponsored health plans [21]. Finally, although it has been documented that patients exhibit both intentional and unintentional non-adherence, no previous study has explored the interrelationship between unintentional and intentional non-adherence in relation to patients’ medication beliefs. The current study, conducted in a large sample of adults with chronic disease, aimed to: (1) report the prevalence of unintentional non-adherence; (2) explore whether medication beliefs predict unintentional non-adherence; and (3) explore the interrelationship between unintentional and intentional non-adherence in relation to medication beliefs.

Methods

Study population

A cross-sectional survey of U.S. adults with self-reported chronic disease was conducted using the Harris Interactive Chronic Illness Panel (CIP). CIP participants are recruited through a number of avenues, including postal-mail invitations, television advertisements, telephone recruitment for under-represented populations, targeted email solicitations, and text-banner placements on websites (e.g., social-media, news sites, search-engine sites and community portals). In February and March of 2009, randomly-selected members of the CIP were sent an e-mail invitation to participate in the survey. Panel members responding to the e-mail invitation were instructed to read the informed consent form and click on yes if they agreed to participate. The study was approved by the George Mason University Institutional Review Board.

Panel members were eligible for participation if they were aged 40 and older, resided in the U.S., and reported one of six chronic diseases (hypertension, hyperlipidemia, diabetes, asthma, osteoporosis, or depression). Only persons age 40 and older were sampled because they bear a greater burden of chronic disease compared to adults aged 18–39. The six chronic diseases reflect a mix of symptomatic, asymptomatic, and mental-health conditions. They are some of the most highly prevalent conditions in the U.S. [32] and are associated with a significant clinical and economic burden for the U.S. health care system [33].

Requests for survey participation were sent to 204,266 randomly-selected CIP members. Of these invitations, there were 11,945 invalid e-mail addresses. Of the 192,321 invitations with valid e-mail addresses, 17,121 were ineligible due to age less than 40. Of the 175,200 age-eligible persons with a valid e-mail address, 51,774 completed the screener (29.5% survey contact rate per standards recommended by The American Association for Public Opinion Research [34]).

During the screening portion of the survey, panel members’ chronic disease status was reconfirmed and their medication-taking behavior for the index condition was solicited. The screener solicited the number of medications respondents currently took for each disease as well as the length of time they reported continuously taking the medication. These items were used to classify respondents as currently persistent to their medication. To identify respondents as non-persisters, the survey asked if, in the last year, they had stopped taking a prescription medication for one of the six conditions without their providers telling them to do so. To identify respondents as non-fulfillers, the survey asked if, in the last year, they had received, but did not fill, a new prescription from their provider for one of the six target conditions. Only subjects who self-identified themselves as persistent to (i.e., currently on therapy with) prescription medications for their index disease form the analytic sample for this study. The index disease was the disease for which respondents completed the medication-belief items. If self-reported persisters reported more than one of the six target conditions, one was randomly selected as the index disease. Because non-fulfillers never filled the index medication, they were not asked questions about unintentional non-adherence. Self-reported non-persisters were excluded from the analytic sample because their complete discontinuation of therapy cannot, logically, be unintentional.

Survey content

Study participants completed a core set of questions on demographics and self-reported health status. They answered three questions on unintentional non-adherence in reference to the prescription medications for their index disease: During the past six months: (1) did you ever forget to take the prescription medication; (2) did you ever run out of the prescription medication; and (3) were you careless at times about taking the prescription medication? Respondents also answered 11 questions on intentional non-adherence in reference to their medication-taking behavior in the past six months for their index disease. These questions surveyed the respondents on the following behaviors: took less medication than instructed because they felt better/worse; skipped taking medication because they felt better/worse; altered dose of medication to suit own needs; stopped taking medication because they felt better/worse; skipped doses to make medication last longer; took smaller doses to make medication last longer; skipped doses of medication because they did not think it was helping them; and stopped medication because they did not think it was helping them. Both sets of measures of unintentional and intentional non-adherence were adapted from validated measures published in the peer-review literature [3539].

Finally, respondents answered 20 questions assessing their beliefs about the index medication: perceived need for the medication (k = 10), perceived medication concerns (k = 6), and perceived medication affordability (k = 4). As described elsewhere [26], multi-item scales were created by summing raw items into a scale score and linearly transforming each sum to a 0–100 metric, with 100 representing the most favorable belief (highest perceived need, fewest perceived concerns, fewest affordability issues), 0 the least favorable, and scores in between representing the percentage of the total possible score. Each multi-item scale was highly reliable [26]. Included in the 20 items were the three items from the Adherence Estimator®, a brief, three-item adherence screener. One item each from the Adherence Estimator® assessed the domains of perceived need for medications, perceived medication concerns, and perceived medication affordability [26].

Analysis

Logistic regression was used to assess differences between age-eligible CIP members with valid e-mail addresses who did and did not responded to the screener (survey non-contact bias per standards recommended by The American Association for Public Opinion Research [34]). Independent variables were age, gender, race, education, income, geographic region of residence, and chronic disease. Prevalence of unintentional and intentional non-adherence among self-identified persisters was tabulated. Chi-square tests were used to assess for variation of any unintentional non-adherence across key variables. Multivariate logistic regression models were used to predict each of the three unintentional non-adherence behaviors (dichotomized as yes/no) as well as any unintentional non-adherence behavior using the multi-item scales for perceived need, perceived concerns, and perceived affordability as independent variables. To assess whether the relationship between medication beliefs and unintentional non-adherence was linear and monotonic, we divided each multi-item scale into quartiles and modeled them as dummy variables. We also modeled unintentional non-adherence using the Adherence Estimator® total score (0–36) and risk levels for non-adherence (low, medium, high) based on the total score. As described elsewhere [26], respondents were classified as low risk for non-adherence if they had a total score of 0, medium risk if they had a score of 2–7, and high risk for non-adherence if they had a score of 8 or higher. Covariates in the models included age, gender, race, education, income, index disease, employment status, and self-rated health. All analyses were conducted using Stata SE version 10.

We hypothesized that the effect of medication beliefs (perceived need, perceived concerns, and perceived affordability) on intentional non-adherence would be mediated through unintentional non-adherence. In other words, it was hypothesized that medication beliefs would significantly impact unintentional non-adherence which, in turn, would significantly influence their intentional non-adherence (Figure 1).

Figure 1
figure 1

Conceptual Diagram Showing the Mediating Effect of Unintentional Non-Adherence.

The most commonly-used method for testing mediation hypotheses is the causal steps approach by Baron and Kenny [40]. For a variable to be considered a mediator according to this approach, four conditions must be met [40, 41]: (1) the independent variable must be significantly associated with the mediator—path a in Figure 1; (2) the mediator must be significantly associated with the dependent variable—path b in Figure 1; (3) the independent variable must be significantly associated with the dependent variable—path c in Figure 1; and (4) the direct effect of the independent variable on the dependent variable after controlling for the mediator must be less than the total effect without controlling for the mediator—path c’ in Figure 1 should have a smaller coefficient than c. These four conditions were tested using four linear regression models. For this analysis, two separate multi-item scales were created for unintentional and intentional non-adherence by summing raw items (three items for unintentional non-adherence and 11 items for intentional non-adherence) into a scale score and linearly transforming each sum to a 0–100 metric, with 100 representing the highest level of non-adherence (positive response for all items), 0 representing complete adherence (negative response on all items), and scores in between representing the percentage of the total possible non-adherence score.

The regression approach proposed by Baron and Kenny [40] involves an implicit assumption that the estimates of the indirect effect are normally distributed. There are reasons to suspect that this assumption may not always hold when mediation is present as indirect effects are usually positively skewed and kurtotic [4244]. A nonparametric resampling procedure called bootstrapping has gained popularity in research on mediation [4547] because it makes no assumption about the shape of the sampling distribution of the indirect effect. Simulation research has shown that bootstrapping is one of the most valid and powerful methods for testing mediation effects [43, 4852]. With bootstrapping, one draws several thousand samples from the original data (of equal size to the original sample), each of which is obtained by random sampling with replacement from the original sample. As recommended by Hayes and others [41, 46], we conducted a bootstrapping procedure with 5,000 samples. These samples were then used to compute the mediated effect and its standard error. Finally, the probability distribution from all of the resampled estimates was used to calculate bias-corrected confidence intervals and bootstrap-estimated standard errors of the mediated effects [43]. If the confidence interval did not include a zero, then a significant indirect effect was concluded [41]. Adjustments for multiple comparisons were made using the Bonferroni correction approach [48].

Results

Survey non-contact bias

A 29.5% survey contact rate was achieved. Compared to those age-eligible respondents with valid e-mail addresses who were invited but did not respond to the screener, those successfully contacted were more likely to be male, age 50 and older, Caucasian, and of higher income but were less likely to have asthma or depression (data not shown). Sociodemographic characteristics (age, gender, race, education, and income), geographic region, and chronic diseases explained only 3.2% of the variance (Cox and Snell pseudo R-square test) in panel members’ likelihood of completing the screener.

Sample description

Of the respondents, 24,017 subjects (88%) self-identified themselves as persistent to the prescription medication for the index disease. Data on these respondents was included in the current analysis. As shown in Table 1, a majority of the self-reported persisters were white (95%), age 60 years and older, and college educated. Two-thirds reported their health as good, very good, or excellent. Over 42% reported hypertension as their index disease, followed by hyperlipidemia (19%), diabetes (13%), osteoporosis (13%), asthma (7%), and depression (5%). Overall, the respondents had a relatively high perceived need for medications (mean of 81.9), moderate medication concerns (mean of 66.7) and moderate perceived medication affordability (mean of 61.4).

Table 1 Sample Description (N = 24,017)

Prevalence of unintentional and intentional non-adherence

An overwhelming majority (70%) of the self-reported persisters reported at least one of the three unintentional non-adherence behaviors in the past six months (Table 2). Forgetting to take medication (62%) was the most commonly-reported behavior followed by running out of medications (37%) and being careless at times about taking the medication (23%). Just over one-third of the respondents reported at least one of the eleven intentional non-adherence behaviors in the previous six months. Skipping doses to make medication last longer was the most commonly reported intentional non-adherence behavior (18%), followed by taking smaller doses to make medication last longer (15%), and altering dose of medication to suit own needs (14%).

Table 2 Prevalence of Unintentional and Intentional Non-adherence (N = 24,017)

Bivariate associations with any unintentional non-adherence

Table 3 shows results of bivariate tests for variation in any unintentional non-adherence across key independent variables. The proportion of respondents reporting any unintentional non-adherence was the highest among persons age 40–49 and decreased as age increased. White respondents were less likely to report any unintentional non-adherence compared to other races. Compared to respondents with education greater than college degree, respondents with a high school education or less were significantly less likely to report any unintentional non-adherence. Retired respondents reported unintentional non-adherence at the lowest rate, while those actively looking for a job reported unintentional non-adherence at the highest rate. Compared to respondents with income greater than $100,000, respondents with income less than $25,000 were significantly more likely to report any unintentional non-adherence. Reports of unintentional non-adherence decreased as self-rated health increased. Respondents with an index medication for depression had the lowest reports of unintentional non-adherence (63%) while those taking osteoporosis medications had the highest (77%). Compared to the fourth (best) quartile, persons in the first three quartiles for perceived need, concerns, and affordability scales had higher reports of any unintentional non-adherence.

Table 3 Reports of Any Unintentional Non-Adherence across Key Variables (N = 24,017)

Multivariate predictors of unintentional non-adherence

Table 4 presents results for multivariate analyses. Common multivariate predictors of each of the three unintentional non-adherence behaviors included: lower perceived need for medications, lower perceived medication affordability, younger age, worse self-rated health, and diabetes or osteoporosis (relative to hypertension). Unique predictors of ‘forgot to take medications’ were more medication concerns and male gender. Unique predictors of ‘run out of medications’ were non-white race and asthma (relative to hypertension). Unique predictors of ‘being careless’ were more medication concerns, non-white race, education greater than college degree, and asthma (relative to hypertension).

Table 4 Logistic Regression Models for Multivariate Predictors of Unintentional Non-Adherence (N = 24,017)

Respondents with less perceived need for medications, more medication concerns, and less perceived medication affordability were significantly more likely to report any unintentional non-adherence. Compared to respondents in the highest quartile for perceived need for medications, those in the first three quartiles were 33% to 51% more likely to report any unintentional non-adherence. Respondents in the lowest quartile for perceived medication affordability scores were 2.32 times more likely than those in the highest quartile to report any unintentional non-adherence. Compared to respondents in the highest quartile for perceived concerns, those in the first three quartiles were 19% to 27% more likely to report any unintentional non-adherence.

Across the three medication beliefs, perceived medication need and perceived medication affordability were stronger predictors of unintentional non-adherence than perceived medication concerns. Among demographics, age was the strongest predictor of unintentional non-adherence. Respondents in the 40–49 years age category were almost two times more likely to report any unintentional non-adherence compared to those age 70 or older.

The Adherence Estimator® total score was a significant predictor of each of the three unintentional non-adherence behaviors as well as any unintentional non-adherence. Compared to persons classified as low-risk by the Adherence Estimator® those classified as medium and high risk were significantly more likely to report forgetting medications (OR = 1.32, 1.44, respectively), carelessness with medications (OR = 1.43, 1.90, respectively), running out of medications (OR = 1.81, 1.90, respectively), and any unintentional non-adherence (OR = 1.59, 1.69, respectively).

Mediating effect of unintentional non-adherence

It was hypothesized that the effect of medication beliefs (perceived need, perceived concerns, and perceived affordability) on intentional non-adherence is mediated through unintentional non-adherence. The four conditions outlined by Barron and Kenny for a variable to be considered as a mediating variable were satisfied (Table 5). These four conditions, demonstrated by paths a, b, c, and c’ in Figure 1, were tested through four successive linear regressions - models 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively in Table 5. The direct effect of the three medication beliefs on unintentional non-adherence was significant (model 1). Compared to persons in the highest quartile of perceived need, those in quartiles 1, 2, and 3 scored, on average, 9.2, 7.5, and 5.1 points, respectively, higher on the unintentional non-adherence scale (with a higher score indicating a higher level of unintentional non-adherence).

Table 5 Unintentional Non-Adherence as a Mediator on the Direct Effect of Patient Beliefs on Intentional Non-Adherence (N = 24,017)

The direct effect of unintentional non-adherence on intentional non-adherence was significant (model 2), as was the total effect of medication beliefs on intentional non-adherence (model 3). Respondents with less perceived need for medications, more medication concerns, and lower perceived medication affordability had significantly higher scores on the intentional non-adherence scale (higher scores indicating greater intentional non-adherence). Consistent with the Barron and Kenny approach [40], the coefficients in model 3 were reduced after controlling for unintentional non-adherence (model 4).

The statistical significance of the mediational relationship was confirmed using bootstrapping. The bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals for the indirect effect of the three medication beliefs (perceived need, perceived concerns, and perceived affordability) were found to exclude zero. This confirmed the hypothesis that the effect of medication beliefs on intentional non-adherence is mediated through unintentional non-adherence.

Discussion

In a sample of 24,017 adults with chronic disease who self-identified as being medication persisters, 70% reported at least one instance of unintentional non-adherence, and 34% reported at least one instance of intentional non-adherence in the past six months. Forgetfulness was the most common unintentional behavior while skipping doses to make the medication last longer was the most common intentional behavior. Our observed prevalence of unintentional non-adherence is likely higher than that reported in other studies [7, 1416, 22, 23, 29, 5356] because of differences in the recall period as well as the wording of the question (our study asked whether the respondent “ever” demonstrated a certain behavior in the past six months, whereas several other studies asked whether the respondent “sometimes” demonstrated a certain behavior (which might be interpreted as a regular behavioral pattern).

Intentional non-adherence has been described as an active process whereby patients choose to deviate from the prescribed regimen [7]. On the other hand, unintentional non-adherence has been described as a passive process, as inadvertent or accidental medication-taking [57, 58]. If unintentional non-adherence is truly random or inadvertent, then one would expect it to be weakly related to demographic characteristics and medication beliefs.

We found a linear and monotonic association between age and unintentional non-adherence: unintentional non-adherence decreased as age increased, which may possibly be attributed to multiple competing demands of professional and family life among those in their 40’s and 50’s. There is conflicting evidence from past research about the influence of age on unintentional non-adherence with some studies reporting no effect [15,20,21,28-30] and others an inverse effect similar to that reported herein [6, 10]. Unlike previous studies, our large sample size allowed us to explore this relationship across several age categories. Consistent with some past research [7], individuals with lower income were more likely to report running out of medications, perhaps reflecting occasional cost-related medication underuse. Worse self-rated health was also a consistent predictor of unintentional non-adherence, which may possibly be related to a greater burden of overall medication taking across multiple comorbidities. Compared to those with hypertension, having osteoporosis or diabetes was associated with a higher likelihood of each of the three unintentional behaviors. A recent systematic review also reported that primary non-adherence, or medication non-fulfillment, was higher in patients with osteoporosis compared to hypertension [11]. Women may perceive osteoporosis to be a less serious condition compared to other diseases [59] and often do not perceive themselves to be at risk for osteoporosis [59, 60], both of which may make them vulnerable to unintentional non-adherence. Diabetes is frequently accompanied by several other comorbid chronic conditions, thus increasing the pill burden of such patients and possibly making them more vulnerable to unintentional non-adherence.

Patient’s medication beliefs, especially perceived need for medication and perceived medication affordability, were strong predictors of unintentional non-adherence. Patients in the lowest quartile of perceived need for medications were 1.9 times more likely to report being careless at times with medications. Patients in the lowest quartile of perceived medication affordability were 4.65 times more likely to report running out of medications at times. Other research has also reported medication beliefs to be associated with unintentional non-adherence [15, 30]. Overall, we found no evidence of unintentional non-adherence being random or accidental [21].

This study has several strengths compared to previous empirical work on unintentional non-adherence. A large, internet-based panel of adults with chronic disease was accessed. The obtained sample represents persons aged 40 and older with one of six chronic diseases from all 50 U.S. states. Our sample is by far the largest in which predictors of unintentional non-adherence has been assessed. The multi-item scales assessing perceived need for medications, perceived medication concerns, and perceived medication affordability have been previously tested and found to be reliable and valid [26]. Unlike most previous studies, the analyses were conducted in multiple chronic diseases including both physical and psychiatric disorders. Finally, the sophisticated bootstrapping approach was used to test the relationship between intentional and unintentional non-adherence.

Despite these strengths, several limitations must be taken in to account when interpreting the results. This study was conducted over the Internet with 29.5% survey contact rate. The CIP recruits panel members through multiple avenues in an attempt to provide a representative sample of the adult, chronically-ill population and to reach under-represented and hard-to-reach populations of interest. However, some biases were observed in the sample. Specifically, compared to the U.S. adult population, the obtained sample was under-represented by adults with income less than $25,000 annually [61], under-represented by adults with a high school education or less [62], and over-represented by Caucasians [63]. As a result, generalizability to the broader population should be made with caution. Because the obtained sample is under-represented by persons of lower income relative to the U.S. adult population, it is likely that the observed effects of income and perceived medication affordability represent lower-bound estimates. All respondents were self-identified persisters to prescription medications, and we did not confirm this based on pharmacy-claims data or other adherence metrics (such as pill counts). The unintentional non-adherence items were dichotomous (yes/no); we did not inquire about the frequency of each unintentional medication-taking behavior. Finally, this study had a cross-sectional design. Results from this study, especially the relationship between intentional and unintentional non-adherence, should be confirmed using longitudinal data.

Numerous measures assessing unintentional non-adherence exist. For example, the four-item Morisky Scale [35] contains two items on unintentional non-adherence and the eight-item Morisky Scale [64] contains three unintentional items. However, an interpretative problem with most measures of unintentional non-adherence is that they do not reveal the underlying reasons for forgetfulness or carelessness. The analyses reported herein suggest that unintentional non-adherence is influenced by the same drivers as intentional non-adherence—patients’ beliefs about the medications. While unintentional non-adherence may appear to be a rather benign behavior, its importance lies in its potential prognostic significance. Qualitative research has depicted how patients test or experiment with their medications [9, 65, 66]. It is plausible that acts of unintentional non-adherence may be prescient of future intentional non-adherence. Our meditational tests support this hypothesis. With occasional episodes of unintentional non-adherence, patients may be testing a medication’s effectiveness or gauging symptom status without the medication. Others have suggested that reports of forgetfulness in taking medications may be a proxy for reduced motivation [67], having doubts about the prescribed therapy [68], or having low perceived need for the medication [29, 69]. Our analyses support these interpretation as all three medication beliefs scales were significantly associated with reports of forgetfulness, carelessness, and any unintentional non-adherence.

Non-persistence most commonly occurs in the first six months of therapy [7077]. It is during this initial period that both unintentional and intentional non-adherence should be assessed. Because physicians poorly predict patients’ adherence [78, 79], adherence screeners, like the Adherence Estimator® [26], might prove useful in identifying nascent, suboptimal medication beliefs that may lead to forgetfulness, carelessness, and, perhaps eventually, non-persistence all together.

Conclusions

For our study sample, unintentional non-adherence does not appear to be random and is predicted by medication beliefs, chronic disease, and sociodemographics. Our data suggests that the importance of unintentional non-adherence may lie in its potential prognostic significance for future intentional non-adherence. Health care providers may consider routinely inquiring about unintentional non-adherence in order to proactively address patients’ suboptimal medication beliefs before they choose to discontinue therapy all together.

References

  1. Cramer JA, Roy A, Burrell A, Fairchild CJ, Fuldeore MJ, Ollendorf DA, Wong PK: Medication compliance and persistence: terminology and definitions. Value Health. 2008, 11: 44-47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. DiMatteo MR, Giordani PJ, Lepper HS, Croghan TW: Patient adherence and medical treatment outcomes: a meta-analysis. Med Care. 2002, 40: 794-811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Cramer JA, Benedict A, Muszbek N, Keskinaslan A, Khan ZM: The significance of compliance and persistence in the treatment of diabetes, hypertension and dyslipidaemia: a review. Int J Clin Pract. 2008, 62: 76-87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Lehane E, McCarthy G: Intentional and unintentional medication non-adherence: a comprehensive framework for clinical research and practice? A discussion paper. Int J Nurs Stud. 2007, 44: 1468-1477.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Lehane E, McCarthy G: An examination of the intentional and unintentional aspects of medication non-adherence in patients diagnosed with hypertension. J Clin Nurs. 2007, 16: 698-706.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Wroe AL: Intentional and unintentional nonadherence: a study of decision making. J Behav Med. 2002, 25: 355-372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Lowry KP, Dudley TK, Oddone EZ, Bosworth HB: Intentional and unintentional nonadherence to antihypertensive medication. Ann Pharmacother. 2005, 39: 1198-1203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Sewitch MJ, Abrahamowicz M, Barkun A, Bitton A, Wild GE, Cohen A, Dobkin PL: Patient nonadherence to medication in inflammatory bowel disease. Am J Gastroenterol. 2003, 98: 1535-1544.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Eliasson L, Clifford S, Barber N, Marin D: Exploring chronic myeloid leukemia patients’ reasons for not adhering to the oral anticancer drug imatinib as prescribed. Leuk Res. 2011, 35: 626-630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Rees G, Leong O, Crowston JG, Lamoureux EL: Intentional and unintentional nonadherence to ocular hypotensive treatment in patients with glaucoma. Ophthalmology. 2010, 117: 903-908.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Gadkari A, McHorney C: Medication non-fulfillment rates and reasons for non-fulfillment: narrative systematic review. CMRO. 2010, 26: 683-705.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Haynes RB, McDonald HP, Garg AX: Helping patients follow prescribed treatment: clinical applications. JAMA. 2002, 288: 2880-2883.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Osterberg L, Blaschke T: Adherence to medication. N Engl J Med. 2005, 353: 487-497.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Barber N, Parsons J, Clifford S, Darracott R, Horne R: Patients’ problems with new medication for chronic conditions. Qual Saf Health Care. 2004, 13: 172-175.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Wray J, Waters S, Radley-Smith R, Sensky T: Adherence in adolescents and young adults following heart or heart-lung transplantation. Pediatr Transplant. 2006, 10: 694-700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Cerveny P, Bortlik M, Kubena A, Vlcek J, Lakatos PL, Lukas M: Nonadherence in inflammatory bowel disease: results of factor analysis. Inflamm Bowel Dis. 2007, 13: 1244-1249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Julian LJ, Yelin E, Yazdany J, Panopalis P, Trupin L, Criswell LA, Katz P: Depression, medication adherence, and service utilization in systemic lupus erythematosus. Arthritis & Rheumatism-Arthritis Care & Research. 2009, 61: 240-246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Sahm L, Maccurtain A, Hayden J, Roche C, Richards HL: Electronic reminders to improve medication adherence-are they acceptable to the patient?. Pharm World Sci. 2009, 31: 627-629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Daleboudt GM, Broadbent E, McQueen F: Kaptein AA. 2010, Intentional and unintentional treatment non-adherence in patients with systemic lupus erythematosus, Arthritis Care Res, [EPub ahead of print]

    Google Scholar 

  20. Saratsiotou I, Kordoni M, Bakogiannis C, Livadarou E, Skarlos D, Kosmidis PA, Razis E: Treatment adherence of cancer patients to orally administered chemotherapy: insights from a Greek study using a self-reported questionnaire. J Oncol Pharm Pract. 2011, 17: 304-311.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Unni EJ, Farris KB: Unintentional non-adherence and belief in medicines in older adults. Patient Educ Couns. 2011, 83: 265-268.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Kjeldsen L, Bjerrum L, Herborg H, Knudsen P, Rossing C, Sondergaard B: Development of new concepts of non-adherence measurements among users of antihypertensives medicines. Int J Clin Pharm. 2011, 33: 572-656.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Lindquist LA, Go L, Fleisher J, Jain N, Friesema E: Baker DW. 2011, Relationship of health literacy to intentional and unintentional non-adherence of hospital discharge m edications, J Gen Intern Med, [EPub ahead of print]

    Google Scholar 

  24. Benson J, Britten N: Patients’ decisions about whether or not to take antihypertensive drugs: qualitative study. BMJ. 2002, 325: 873.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Elliott RA, Ross-Degnan D, Adams AS, Safran DG, Soumerai SB: Strategies for coping in a complex world: adherence behavior among older adults with chronic illness. J Gen Intern Med. 2007, 22: 805-810.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. McHorney C: The adherence estimator: a brief, proximal screener for patient propensity to adhere to prescription medications for chronic disease. Curr Med Res Opin. 2009, 25: 215-238.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. McHorney C, Gadkari A: Individual patients hold different beliefs to prescription medications to which they persist vs. non-persist and persist vs. non-fulfill. Patient Prefer Adherence. 2010, 4: 187-195.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Kim EY, Han HR, Jeong S, Kim KB, Park H, Kang E, Shin HS: Does knowledge matter? Intentional medication nonadherence among middle-aged Korean Americans with high blood pressure. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2007, 22: 397-404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Clifford S, Barber N, Horne R: Understanding different beliefs held by adherers, unintentional nonadherers, and intentional nonadherers: Application of the necessity-concerns framework. J Psychosom Res. 2008, 64: 41-46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Iihara N, Kurosaki Y, Miyoshi C, Takabatake K, Morita S, Hori K: Comparison of individual perceptions of medication costs and benefits between intentional and unintentional medication non-adherence among Japanese patients. Patient Educ Couns. 2008, 70: 292-299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Norton WE, Amico KR, Fisher WA, Shuper PA, Ferrer RA, Cornman DH, Trayling CA, Redding C, Fisher JD: Information-Motivation-Behavioral Skills barriers associated with intentional versus unintentional ARV non-adherence behavior among HIV-positive patients in clinical care. AIDS Care. 2010, 22: 979-987.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. National Center for Health Statistics: Health, United States, 2010: With Special Feature on Death and Dying. 2011, Department of Health and Human Services, Hyattsville

    Google Scholar 

  33. DeVol R, Bedroussian A, Charuworn A, Chatterjee A, Kim I, Kim S: An Unhealthy America: The Economic Burden of Chronic Disease. 2007, The Milken Institure, Santa Monica, CA

    Google Scholar 

  34. The American Association for Public Opinion Research: Standard Definitions: Final Dispositions of Case Codes and Outcomes Rates for Surveys. 2006, Available at: [last accessed 5 May 2007], [http://www.aapor.org/standards.asp]

    Google Scholar 

  35. Morisky DE, Green LW, Levine DM: Concurrent and predictive validity of a self-reported measure of medication adherence. Med Care. 1986, 24: 67-74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Horne R, Weinman J, Hankins M: The Beliefs about Medicines Questionnaire: The development and evaluation of a new method for assessing the cognitive representation of medication. Psychol Health. 1999, 14: 1-24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Kim MT, Hill MN, Bone LR, Levine DM: Development and testing of the Hill-Bone Compliance to High Blood Pressure Therapy Scale. Prog Cardiovasc Nurs. 2000, 15: 90-96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Blalock SJ, Patel RA: Drug therapy concerns questionnaire: Initial development and refinement. J Am Pharm Assoc. 2005, 45: 160-169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Wilson IB, Schoen C, Neuman P, Strollo MK, Rogers WH, Chang H, Safran DG: Physician-patient communication about prescription medication nonadherence: A 50-state study of America's seniors. J Gen Intern Med. 2007, 22: 6-12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Baron RM, Kenny DA: The moderator mediator variable distinction in social psychological-research - Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986, 51: 1173-1182.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Hayes AF: Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Commun Monogr. 2009, 76: 408-420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. MacKinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Hoffman JM, West SG, Sheets V: A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychol Methods. 2002, 7: 83-104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Shrout PE, Bolger N: Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychol Methods. 2002, 7: 422-445.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Fritz MS, MacKinnon DP: Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol Sci. 2007, 18: 233-239.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Lee YY, Lin JL: The effects of trust in physician on self-efficacy, adherence and diabetes outcomes. Soc Sci Med. 2009, 68: 1060-1068.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Anagnostopoulos F, Slater J, Fitzsimmons D: Intrusive thoughts and psychological adjustment to breast cancer: Exploring the moderating and mediating role of global meaning and emotional expressivity. J Clin Psychol Medical Settings. 2010, 17: 137-149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Rosbrook A, Whittingham K: Autistic traits in the general population: What mediates the link with depressive and anxious symptomatology?. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2010, 4: 415-424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Simes RJ: An improved Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrika. 1986, 73: 751-754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Mackinnon DP, Lockwood CM, Williams J: Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivar Behav Res. 2004, 39: 99-128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Pituch KA, Stapleton LM, Kang JY: A comparison of single sample and bootstrap methods to assess mediation in cluster randomized trials. Multivar Behav Res. 2006, 41: 367-400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Cheung GW, Lau RS: Testing mediation and suppression effects of latent variables - Bootstrapping with structural equation models. Organ Res Methods. 2008, 11: 296-325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Krause MR, Serlin RC, Ward SE, Rony YZ, Ezenwa MO, Naab F: Testing mediation in nursing research Beyond Baron and Kenny. Nurs Res. 2010, 59: 288-294.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Sewitch MJ, Dobkin PL, Bernatsky S, Baron M, Starr M, Cohen M, Fitzcharles MA: Medication non-adherence in women with fibromyalgia. Rheumatology. 2004, 43: 648-654.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Atkins L, Fallowfield L: Intentional and non-intentional non-adherence to medication amongst breast cancer patients. Eur J Cancer. 2006, 42: 2271-2276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Ulrik CS, Backer V, Soes-Petersen U, Lange P, Harving H, Plaschke PP: The patient’s perspective: adherence or non-adherence to asthma controller therapy?. J Asthma. 2006, 43: 701-704.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Miasso AI, Monteschi M, Giacchero KG: Bipolar affective disorder: medication adherence and satisfaction with treatment and guidance by the health team in a mental health service. Rev Lat Am Enfermagem. 2009, 17: 548-556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Gordon K, Smith F, Dhillon S: Effective chronic disease management: Patients' perspectives on medication-related problems. Patient Educ Couns. 2007, 65: 407-415.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Ho PM, Bryson CL, Rumsfeld JS: Medication adherence: its importance in cardiovascular outcomes. Circulation. 2009, 119: 3028-3035.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Hsieh C, Novielli KD, Diamond JJ, Cheruva D: Health beliefs and attitudes toward the prevention of osteoporosis in older women. Menopause. 2001, 8: 372-376.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Gerend MA, Erchull MJ, Aiken LS, Maner JK: Reasons and risk: Factors underlying women's perceptions of susceptibility to osteoporosis. Maturitas. 2006, 55: 227-237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. U.S. Census Bureau: United States - Income in the Past 12 Months. 2009, Available at: [Last accessed 28 December 2010], [http://factfinder.census.gov/servlet/STTable?_bm=y&-geo_id=01000US&-qr_name=ACS_2009_1YR_G00_S1901&-ds_name=ACS_2009_1YR_G00_&-_lang=en&-redoLog=false]

    Google Scholar 

  62. U.S. Census Bureau: United States - Educational Attainment 2009. 2010, Available at: {last accessed 28 December 2010}, [http://factfinder.census.gov/servlet/STTable?_bm=y&-geo_id=01000US&-qr_name=ACS_2009_1YR_G00_S1501&-ds_name=ACS_2009_1YR_G00_&-_lang=en&-redoLog=false]

    Google Scholar 

  63. U.S. Census Bureau: Resident Population by Sex, Race, and Hispanic Origin Status. 2010, Available at: [Last accessed 28 December 2010]., [http://www.census.gov/compendia/statab/cats/population/estimates_and_projections_by_age_sex_raceethnicity.html]

    Google Scholar 

  64. Morisky DE, Ang A, Krousel-Wood M, Ward H: Predictive validity of a medication adherence measure in an outpatient setting. J Clin Hypertens. 2008, 10: 348-354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Conrad P: The meaning of medications: another look at compliance. Soc Sci Med. 1985, 20: 29-37.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Raynor DK, Savage I, Knapp P, Henley J: We are the experts: people with asthma talk about their medicine information needs. Patient Educ Couns. 2004, 53: 167-174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Kim E, Gupta S, Bolge S, Chen CC, Whitehead R, Bates JA: Adherence and outcomes associated with copayment burden in schizophrenia: a cross-sectional survey. J Med Econ. 2010, 13: 185-192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Proulx M, Leduc N, Vandelac L, Gregoire JP, Collin J: Social context, the struggle with uncertainty, and subjective risk as meaning-rich constructs for explaining HBP noncompliance. Patient Educ Couns. 2007, 68: 98-106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Horne R, Weinman J: Patients' beliefs about prescribed medicines and their role in adherence to treatment in chronic physical illness. J Psychosom Res. 1999, 47: 555-567.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Benner JS, Glynn RJ, Mogun H, Neumann PJ, Weinstein MC, Avorn J: Long-term persistence in use of statin therapy in elderly patients. JAMA. 2002, 288: 455-4561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Kothawala P, Badamgarav E, Ryu S, Miller RM, Halbert RJ: Systematic review and meta-analysis of real-world adherence to drug therapy for osteoporosis. Mayo Clin Proc. 2007, 82: 1493-1501.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Jones JK, Gorkin L, Lian JF, Staffa JA, Fletcher AP: Discontinuation of and changes in treatment after start of new courses of antihypertensive drugs: A study of a United Kingdom population. BMJ. 1995, 311: 293-295.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Chapman RH, Benner JS, Petrilla AA, Tierce JC, Collins SR, Battleman DS, Schwartz JS: Predictors of adherence with antihypertensive and lipid-lowering therapy. Arch Intern Med. 2005, 165: 1147-1152.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Perreault S, Lamarre D, Blais L, Dragomir A, Berbiche D, Lalonde L, Laurier C, St-Maurice F, Collin J: Persistence with treatment in newly treated middle-aged patients with essential hypertension. Ann Pharmacother. 2005, 39: 1401-1408.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Agarwal S, Tang SS, Rosenberg N, Pettitt D, McLaughlin T, Joyce A, Schwartz JS: Does synchronizing initiation of therapy affect adherence to concomitant use of antihypertensive and lipid-lowering therapy?. Am J Ther. 2009, 16: 119-126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Serna MC, Cruz I, Real J, Gasco E, Galvan L: Duration and adherence of antidepressant treatment (2003 to 2007) based on prescription database. Eur Psychiatry. 2010, 25: 206-213.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Yeaw J, Benner JS, Walt JG, Sian S, Smith DB: Comparing adherence and persistence across 6 chronic medication classes. J Manag Care Pharm. 2009, 15: 728-740.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Mushlin AI, Appel FA: Diagnosing potential noncompliance. Physicians' ability in a behavioral dimension of medical care. Arch Intern Med. 1977, 137: 318-321.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Caron HS, Roth HP: Patients’ cooperation with a medical regimen. Difficulties in identifying the noncooperator. JAMA. 1968, 203: 922-926.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was funded by Merck & Co., Inc.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Abhijit S Gadkari.

Additional information

Competing interests

Drs. Gadkari and McHorney are full-time employees of and own stock in Merck & Co., Inc.

Authors’ contributions

Both AG and CM contributed to the conception of the manuscript idea. CM led the instrument development and data collection process. AG led the statistical analysis and interpretation of data for the manuscript. Both AG and CM contributed to the original draft of the manuscript as well as to the critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. Both AG and CM have given final approval for the current version of the manuscript to be published.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Gadkari, A.S., McHorney, C.A. Unintentional non-adherence to chronic prescription medications: How unintentional is it really?. BMC Health Serv Res 12, 98 (2012). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1472-6963-12-98

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1472-6963-12-98

Keywords