Skip to main content

The role of vocal individuality in conservation

Abstract

Identifying the individuals within a population can generate information on life history parameters, generate input data for conservation models, and highlight behavioural traits that may affect management decisions and error or bias within census methods. Individual animals can be discriminated by features of their vocalisations. This vocal individuality can be utilised as an alternative marking technique in situations where the marks are difficult to detect or animals are sensitive to disturbance. Vocal individuality can also be used in cases were the capture and handling of an animal is either logistically or ethically problematic. Many studies have suggested that vocal individuality can be used to count and monitor populations over time; however, few have explicitly tested the method in this role. In this review we discuss methods for extracting individuality information from vocalisations and techniques for using this to count and monitor populations over time. We present case studies in birds where vocal individuality has been applied to conservation and we discuss its role in mammals.

Review

Introduction

Signals can contain information useful to conservation [1, 2]. Until recently, communication behaviour had a limited role in conservation, being restricted to enhancing captive breeding programs [3] or use in species counts [4]. However, knowledge of how individuals within a population communicate and what they are communicating can generate information ranging from measures of habitat use to genetic fitness [2, 5] that can be applied to conservation and that may neither be possible nor desirable to extract using other methods. In this review we shall concentrate on a subsection of communication behaviour that underlies most attempts to gain useful information from signalling, namely individuality. We discuss the different applications and types of information that can be extracted using vocal individuality. Further, we consider the different methods currently applied and the results gained with different taxonomic groups. Finally, we discuss some of the limitations and future directions that this technique can take. As a consequence of our area of research we have concentrated our discussion on the role of acoustic vocal individuality in conservation, although the principles involved equally apply to other signalling modalities, with the possible exception of chemical signals where the techniques may currently be lacking.

Vocal individuality

A pre-requisite for discrimination and individual recognition is that, in the signals being used, there should be low within-individual variation and high between-individual variation [6, 7]. Many studies have shown the presence of individually distinctive vocal features in a wide range of animal species and it seems that vocal individuality is most likely a feature of all vocally active species and is caused by a series of genetic, developmental and environmental factors [8, 9]. The level of individuality and the difficultly in extracting and using it will differ between species and we discuss these issues later in the review (also see [10–12]).

When discussing the uses of vocal individuality we distinguish between two commonly confused terms; discrimination and identification. Discrimination requires that individuals differ enough at one point in time to be separated. Identification requires that an individual's vocal features remain constant enough to be associated with that individual for periods of time. Identification is harder to demonstrate and has more useful conservation applications than discrimination [2, 13]. Discrimination is limited to census tasks, but identification allows individuals to be monitored over time, which can yield useful life-history information [2, 13]. It is possible to have discrimination without identification but not vice versa and the extent to which individuals can be identified should be carefully considered in any cost/benefit analysis of using vocal individuality as a monitoring tool.

Vocal individuality and conservation

Although several studies have suggested that vocal individuality could have a role as a useful conservation tool [5, 14, 15], it has been rarely used or tested in census or monitoring roles (some examples of use and testing are given below). In the following sections we discuss some of the different aspects of conservation biology where the identification of individuals can either yield useful data or be used as a non-invasive marking technique that may offer an alternative to invasive approaches such as mark-recapture or tagging.

Conservation models and identifiable individuals

Several studies by Sutherland, Goss-Custard and colleagues [16–20] have shown the importance of individual differences in predictive models. Assumptions of individual equality have largely come from difficulties associated with collecting and then modelling the baseline demographic data [21]. In most cases individual identification is required to extract this demographic data.

Population viability analysis (PVA) is the most widely used form of conservation model. These models attempt to predict the potential fate of a population given a set of demographic and environmental parameters. PVA models include demographic stochasticity, that is, fluctuations in population size due to random individual differences [22], as a form of 'sampling error' [23]. Mean expected survival and fecundity are estimated from a limited data set and made equal for all individuals; stochasticity is included as the level of variance around this mean when survival is calculated for the model population [23, 24]. Increasing the variance increases the risk of extinction faced by the population. However, these assumptions are biologically untenable. The inclusion of non-random individual variation acts to reduce the effects of demographic stochasticity, leading to the result that the predicted extinction risk faced by populations may be overly pessimistic [23, 24]. By identifying individuals, it is possible to generate more accurate predictions concerning the nature of individual variation in parameters such as fecundity and survival, which will increase the accuracy of predictions of PVA models [17, 23].

Behavioural traits

As well as affecting large-scale population models, identifying individuals can highlight behavioural traits that may affect the conservation value (in terms of effort and resources) of different subsections of a population [5]. In many species there are large asymmetries in lifetime reproductive success, with only a few individuals contributing to the next generation. For example, 17% of a population of common buzzards (Buteo buteo), accounted for 50% of the following year's fledglings [25]. Other bird species have yielded similar values [26]. Kelly [27] showed that a few (8%) cheetah (Acynonyx jubatus) matrilineages produced over 50% of the population over a 20-year period, and studies in other mammals have yielded equally high differences in reproductive success [28, 29].

Individuals within populations also differ in whether they are residents or floaters. Floating individuals often occupy lower quality habitat, possibly representing 'sink' populations, and generally do not breed or breed with lower success [30–32]. This difference in breeding status has obvious implications for management strategies. Monitoring fluctuations in the proportion of floating individuals may also be a more revealing indicator of habitat quality [5]. Also, if translocation is considered as a management option, floating individuals may be suitable for translocation [33], as they may show reproductive success equal to other residents when moved to suitable habitat [30].

Sampling bias

In most cases it is not possible or desirable to count an entire population; usually, census techniques take samples from a population and use them to estimate the true population size [34, 35]. All sampling techniques make assumptions about the populations they are counting and violation of the assumptions leads to biases in the estimate. Unless accounted for, biases can be pervasive and generate misleading census estimates. Accounting and testing for bias within a census can be difficult without either a complete knowledge of the population or a subset of identified individuals [34]. For example, the use of playback to elicit responses from individuals in acoustic surveys can cause a bias by only attracting certain individuals [36, 37]. Using radio-telemetry, Conway et al [36] found both a seasonal and density-dependent bias in response to playback by Yuma clapper rails (Rallus longirostris yumanensisi). Playback elicited responses from 10–40% of males present. This also caused biases in the identification of preferred habitat for the rails [36]. A similar study with black rails (Laterallus jamaicensis) showed differential responses to playback due to factors such as breeding status, with non-nesting males being the most likely to respond [37]. Ogutu & Dublin [38] found playback to be an affective census tool for lion (Panthera leo) and spotted hyena (Crocuta crocuta) populations; however, placement of playback speakers near range borders instigated territorial disputes and prey abundance was also a significant source of bias.

Floating individuals can bias a census as they are usually more mobile than resident individuals. For example, floating male bitterns (Botaurus stellaris) were repeatedly counted in acoustic surveys causing a population of 15 individuals (nine residents and six floaters) to be counted as 25 [39]. This bias was detected by applying vocal individuality information to the population (further details below).

Sampling techniques that catch and mark individuals within a population are likely to also suffer from biases [5]. For example, the key assumptions of most mark-recapture approaches are that individuals are equally likely to be caught and that marks do not affect their subsequent behaviour and survival [34]. However, several studies have shown that capture techniques often only mark a subsection of the population (e.g. [40]). For example, radio-tracked corncrakes (Crex crex) called on 75–80% of nights during one breeding season and this was used to generate census guidelines for that species [41]. However, a subsequent study found a far lower calling rate (~ 40%) in a different population, which led to under-estimates of the population size [40].

Welfare considerations

Although marking techniques can allow the unequivocal identification of individuals, invasive marking of individuals can have both short-term and long-term effects on those individuals. Short-term effects include direct costs of the capture and handling process itself [5]. Longer-term costs include avoidance of the capture area, stress-related susceptibility to disease, increased susceptibility to predation and loss of subsequent reproductive success (e.g. [42]). Both short-and long-term effects of marking are contentious issues, with often equal numbers of articles showing the presence or absence of an effect. An example of this is the debate surrounding the causes of extinction of the Serengeti population of wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). The rapid spread of disease through the population was correlated with capture and handling [43]. However, recent analysis has shown that this relationship was likely to be non-causal and that the administered vaccines failed to take effect during an outbreak of the disease [44]. Another example comes from studies attempting to show effects of neckbands on survival in goose species [45–48].

It can be difficult to study the effects of capture and handling and the subsequent effects of marks on individuals unless there is an individually identifiable (or non-invasively marked) control group for comparison. For example, Nimon et al [49] used artificial eggs to non-invasively measure heart rates in Adélie penguins (Pygoscelis adeliae) and showed that handling individuals pre-disposed them to extreme reactions to human-induced stress [49, 50]. However, it is safe to assume that the capture and handling of animals can only have deleterious effects, even if they may not be immediately obvious. Careful consideration has to be given to balancing benefits gained by invasive marking against potential adverse effects on the individuals or population. In our view, the default should be non-invasive marking techniques, such as vocal individuality – a view that has increasingly been adopted by journals in their recommendations to authors (e.g. [51]).

Life-history parameters

Techniques that identify individuals are used to generate life-history data, such as habitat use, survival, recruitment, immigration and emigration. This information can then be used to test hypotheses concerning the agents of decline facing a population or test the effects of management strategies [35, 52]. Radio-telemetry is often considered the most productive monitoring technique. However, aside from concerns inherent in catching individuals and biases in data generated, radio-tags have a limited operational life, often of one year or less [53]. If vocalizations remain constant over time, they can provide a long-term monitoring option [53, 54].

Summary

We have reasoned that vocal individuality should be widespread (if not ubiquitous) and can generate useful conservation information. Also, as a non-invasive marking method it may provide less biased data than other marking techniques and have fewer adverse welfare implications. We now discuss methods for extracting vocal individuality and some instances in which it has been applied to conservation questions in birds.

Methods used in vocal individuality

The methods used to collect and analyse vocalizations will, in many cases, determine whether vocal individuality is used as monitoring tool. As the amount of equipment, analysis time and specialist knowledge required to discriminate between individuals increases, the likelihood of the technique being used decreases. Thus, in this section we discuss some general points concerning the collecting, processing and analysis of vocalizations for individuality.

Equipment and analysis

Before any statistical analysis takes place, there are a number of practical considerations concerning the process of converting an animal's vocalization into data points. Although some of these considerations may seem trivial, ignoring them can render the approach useless before it has started. This section highlights some of the points that we have encountered in our experience with sound recording and analysis; many texts (e.g. [56]) provide in-depth discussion of these topics.

The best type of microphone to use is determined by features of the vocal signal, predominantly its frequency range [57, 58]. Shotgun and parabolic microphones are most commonly used for individuality studies and have different characteristics [57]. Parabolic reflectors amplify the signal entering the microphone and are more directional than shotgun microphones, reducing the reverberations in recordings [57]. However, parabolas cannot have a flat frequency response because the parabola diameter determines the extent to which each frequency is amplified and also determines the lowest frequency that can be recorded (e.g. [57, 58]). Species with low frequency vocalizations, e.g. European bitterns (frequency range 100–200 Hz) should be recorded with a shotgun microphone with a flat response in this frequency range.

Similarly, the best type of recorder to use is determined by its ability to capture the sounds of interest with the necessary temporal and frequency resolution. Manufacturers' performance specifications are a starting point, although these may often be optimistic, subject to change with age of the machinery and may differ between individual recorders of the same model (particularly analogue tape recorders). It is therefore advisable to include a calibration sound of known frequency and duration at the beginning of a recording session. Digital recorders (solid-state or DAT) are limited by the sampling rate of their analogue to digital converters, so special care is necessary when recording high frequency sounds. Increasingly, field recordings are being made with devices using data compression to increase storage capacity (e.g. MiniDisc). The data compression algorithms remove parts of the signal that are not perceptible to humans, but they can also remove or distort important biological signal components [57].

When recordings are being made, the record level must be adjusted to prevent the signal becoming overloaded and the consequent introduction of distortion. We recommend that the recordist adjusts levels during recording as automatic gain controls rarely adequately control record levels of animal signals. In many cases it will be advisable to include a high-pass filter between the microphone and the recorder as this has the effect of removing low frequency background noise and signal strength is better represented by the recorder's level meters.

When analogue signals are digitised (either during recording or analysis) the highest frequency of interest must be less than half the sampling frequency (the Nyquist frequency), otherwise the higher frequencies will get mirrored in the new digitised signal [59–61]. Low-pass filtering should be used to remove sound from above the Nyquist frequency and filtering to remove unimportant parts of a recording is a method of minimising the amount of signal processing required [61].

Once a signal has been digitised and filtered, it can be measured. Although complex signal processing, as well as automated measuring algorithms, are becoming increasingly common, most measures are taken from three forms of signal representation; waveform (Fig. 1A), power spectrum (Fig. 1B) and spectrogram (or sonagram, Fig. 1C). The waveform shows changes in amplitude (acoustic pressure) with time and is suitable for taking temporal measures. The power spectrum is generated from the waveform and shows changes in amplitude at different frequencies; this is suitable for measurements of features such as dominant frequency. A spectrogram is a three-dimensional display plotting frequency against time, with amplitude shown as intensity of grey scale or in colour. Spectrograms are very useful for visualising sounds and can be used in qualitative comparisons (e.g. [11, 53]), but should not be used for direct measurements. This is because there is a fundamental trade-off when producing spectrograms between the resolution of time and frequency information; high resolution cannot be achieved in both features [62]. We suggest that waveforms should be used for temporal measures and power spectrum for frequency measures. Reported measurements should also always be accompanied by the minimum difference (the cursor increment) that could be measured in addition to resolution limitations imposed by the sampling frequency of A/D conversion.

Figure 1
figure 1

Three common forms of signal representation. An example of a corncrake call displayed as: (A) a waveform which plots temporal information on the x axis and amplitude on the y axis; (B) a spectrogram which plots temporal information on the x axis, frequency on the y axis and amplitude in the image greyscale; (C) a power spectrum, which plots frequency information on the x axis and sound pressure level on the y axis. The spectrogram was made with a 2 msec time step and a 20 Hz frequency step (Hamming window).

Qualitative assessment of vocal individuality

Qualitative comparisons of sounds can be either visual, through graphic representations such as spectrograms, or aural through recordings or listening in the field. Aural identification is possible and field researchers with extensive experience of their study species can often identify individuals by ear [11]. However, there are few studies that address the use of aural comparisons as a census technique. Gilbert [63] showed that discriminating and identifying individual European bitterns by ear was affected by experience and that only small sample sizes could be discriminated.

More common is the use of visual representations of sounds such as spectrograms to discriminate between and to identify individuals [11, 12, 14, 15, 64–67]. In most cases, qualitative comparison is used as the first level of analysis, for example, in selecting sounds to be used in further quantitative tests [67]. In some cases qualitative comparisons have proved more effective than quantitative approaches [68]. Janik [66] compared several techniques used to discriminate between bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncates) signature whistles and found that visual comparison was the most effective at separating individuals.

The main advantage of qualitative assessment is that observers are capable of complex pattern recognition, perhaps using the overall 'gestalt' of an image in a comparison [2, 65]. Features that may not be adequately represented in quantitative tests are considered in qualitative comparisons. Similarly, changes in small-scale measures can be weighted equally with large-scale features. The use of qualitative assessment contains several potential disadvantages associated with the subjectivity of human decision-making, which can lead to biases or inaccuracies [65, 67, 69–71]. It is possible to address these issues by measuring the repeatability of an observer's classifications; however, an observer with high repeatability may not make reproducible classifications [71]. It is possible to measure the degree of inter-observer reliability and use this to determine whether different observers can be used to classify the sounds (see [67] for discussion of inter-observer reliability scores).

Quantitative assessment of vocal individuality

Multivariate approaches are commonly applied to vocal individuality studies and are particularly suited to classification tasks [72]. They can be broadly divided into those that discriminate between individuals by finding differences between them and those that find similarities between them. We shall discuss each in more detail.

Discriminant function analysis (DFA) is a multivariate difference statistic commonly used to show which variables best discriminate between two or more groups. It does this by combining the variables with weighting coefficients to create a set of functions that can discriminate the groups. Once these functions are established, they can be used to classify new data into one of the pre-existing groups. This corresponds to the three main uses of DFA in vocal individuality; establishing variation, data reduction and classification. Establishing variation and paring down the parameters used are the first steps in any investigation into vocal individuality. Invariably many measures are taken from the vocalizations (often >20) and not all of them will be effective in discriminating individuals. For example, Gilbert et al [53] measured 23 features of bittern boom trains and used DFA to reduce these down to 7 features which allowed effective discrimination. A specialised form of this analysis is called stepwise discriminant analysis (SDFA) and it enters variables, one by one, into the analysis until there is no further increase in discrimination accuracy [73]. This can be a powerful way of extracting an 'optimal' subset of features and reducing further analysis time [72, 73]. Once the discriminant functions have been established, they can be used to classify cases to the pre-existing groups. Measures of classification success (percent cases correctly classified) are often cited and are most often high (>80%). However, this measure does not test the functions' generality [73]. The discriminant functions must be validated with data not used in their creation. Most common statistics packages allow validation by not using some of the cases to produce the discriminant functions, and then classifying these unused data. This validation can either be a jackknife, where half the cases are left ungrouped and then classified, or a leave-one-out test where one case at a time is ungrouped and classified. Classification scores without any validation are almost meaningless in the context of their applicability to vocal individuality. Many studies have used DFA to show vocal individuality in avian [10–13, 74, 7], canid [78, 79] and primate [80] species. Implicit in many of these studies is a potential use of DFA to generate conservation data. However, the question remains whether DFA on its own can generate useful conservation information. In reality the role of DFA classification in conservation is limited [2, 81]. This is because in all published examples of the use of DFA for vocal individuality the type of DFA used can only classify vocalizations to particular individuals if all individuals are known; it cannot accommodate vocalizations from new individuals. This limitation is overcome by a non-parametric form of DFA known as adaptive kernel-based DFA, in which the range of values for inclusion into the kernel of existing groups is defined.

Similarity techniques offer a different approach to discrimination that avoids most of problems experienced when using DFA. They do not require complete knowledge of the population being monitored [2, 81]. When using similarity techniques two cases are compared and if they are within a pre-defined threshold, they are classified as coming from the same individual. If a new individual joins the population, it should be outside the threshold for all known individuals. The two most commonly used approaches are acoustic space, which compares measures taken from vocalizations and cross-correlation which is used to compare sounds directly [2]. When a series of measurements are taken from a sound they can be used as coordinates that define the location of that vocalization in an acoustic space whose dimensions are determined by the number of variables [13, 82]. The Euclidean distance between locations is used as the measure of similarity. This approach has been successfully used to identify individuals within and between seasons in several non-oscine bird species [13, 53, 83]. Cross-correlation is used to compare representations of whole sounds, most often these are spectrograms [59, 84]. Two spectrograms are incrementally passed over each other and at each stage a Pearson correlation coefficient is calculated. The maximum correlation value is used as the similarity measure [84]. Many cross correlation routines move spectrograms only along the time axis and thus two sounds with identical frequency contours but centred on different frequencies would give a low similarity value. This is avoided by routines that move along time and frequency axes. The only conservation application of cross-correlation that we are aware of was to monitor wild turkey (Meleagris gallopavo mexicana) populations [85]. However, it has also been used show individuality [12, 86–88], dialect differences [89], vocal learning and development [69, 90] in several species of songbird. The advantage of cross-correlation is that it considers the entire sound objectively. However, care has to be taken with the particular sound types being compared, the amount of noise included in the signal and with the settings used to create the representations, as these factors will adversely affect the similarity value generated (for further discussion see [81, 84, 91]). Once a similarity measure has been used, the distributions of within-individual and between-individual similarity values can be compared (see Fig. 2). With an ideal similarity measure, the two distributions would not overlap, i.e. there would be no chance of making a false identification or discrimination [13]. However, in reality a certain amount of overlap will always occur and the extent of this overlap can be used to show how effective the technique will be (see Fig. 2 and [2, 81, 92]). Note that it will be impossible to maximise correct identifications and simultaneously minimise false identifications. However, studying the area of overlap can aid the setting of a threshold.

Figure 2
figure 2

Examples of distributions of within-and between-individual similarity values. Distributions of within-individual (black bars) and between-individual (white bars) pair-wise comparisons in a similarity analysis. The ideal case (A) has no overlap between the two distributions; however, in cases of complete overlap (B) the technique becomes useless. The most common situation is one of partial overlap (C). The extent of this overlap can be used as a measure of confidence in the technique.

A number of complex non-linear models used for speech recognition have also been applied to bioacoustic signal analysis, and may represent the future direction for analysis tools as researchers try to compare increasingly complex signals. Models that have been used in bioacoustic studies include dynamic time warping [93–95], artificial neural networks [96–98], hidden Markov models [95] and linear predictive coding. These models can be divided into those that function by modelling sound production (linear predictive coding and hidden Markov models) or sound perception (neural networks). Currently, the most commonly used of these models are artificial neural networks. Originally designed as models of biological neural networks, they contain a network of inter-connected simple processing units that work in parallel to solve complex classification tasks [99, 100]. The connections between the units are weighting coefficients. Neural networks solve classification problems by iteratively adjusting the weighting coefficients and combining them with the parameters until some pre-determined classification error value is achieved. The combination of simple algorithms used to classify highly optimised parameters makes them powerful and versatile tools. Neural networks have several advantages over other techniques. First, they are non-linear and can work with data that cannot be separated with linear classification tools [100, 101]. Second, there is a huge range of different neural network types, and they have been applied to many classification and regression tasks. This provides a large source of reference material to draw on [98]. Third, they can be used to find clusters of similar vocalizations, set up groups based on those clusters and then classify new data to one of these groups or create a new group (e.g. Kohonen networks [102]). Neural networks have successfully identified individuals from their vocalizations in tungara frogs (Physalaemus pustulosus) [103, 104], fallow deer (Dama dama) [96, 105], Gunnison's prairie dog (Cynomys gunnisoni) [106], Stellar sea lions (Eumetopias jubatus) [97] and killer whales (Orcinus orca) [107]. Elsewhere we have shown that different neural network models were able to accurately count and identify individual corncrakes in a series of simulated census and monitoring tasks [98]. These types of model make it possible to create automatic analysis and identification systems [95], which will reduce analysis time, increase the amount of data that can be analysed and make complex classification techniques more available. All of these factors will be important in any conservation application. Note that, as with any statistical tool, they cannot be treated as complete black boxes, and some care has to be taken to adhere to the assumptions or limitations of the models (e.g. neural networks: [101], hidden Markov models: [95]).

Three case studies

Here we detail three cases where vocal individuality has been used to obtain conservation information on bird species or where the potential for generating such information has been investigated. Vocal individuality has been used to correct for census error and bias in bitterns and corncrakes, to follow habitat use in corncrakes and owls, to monitor annual turnover in bitterns and owls and to produce data on which to run survival models in bitterns.

European bittern

The bittern is IUCN red-listed and is strictly protected under the EU Birds and Habitats Directives due to large population declines (>50%) in the last 25 years [108]. Bitterns are found in dense reed-bed habitat and are secretive, making monitoring difficult [11]. The use of leg rings can be a useful method to identify individuals; however, bitterns are rarely seen or caught and the only practical census and monitoring method involves counting calling males [109]. Male bitterns have an individually distinctive long-range vocalization [11, 65] – the boom – and booms are used to census and monitor individuals. The bittern is currently the only species we are aware of that is routinely counted and monitored using vocal individuality. Bitterns have been monitored in the United Kingdom in this way for over 10 years, allowing long-term population data to be extracted [53]. Gilbert et al [53] identified males between years by their booms and used these data as the basis for a survival analysis and to monitor movements between populations for the UK populations (although see [12], for possible differences with Italian populations of bitterns).

Terry [110] developed a semi-automatic method that discriminated between relatively large numbers (>40) of bitterns and could also extract the discriminating measures from poor quality recordings. The main discriminating feature was the dominant frequency of the boom and this was found to vary little over very short periods of time (days). However, recordings made between years of four radio-tagged individuals [53] showed within-individual changes over time in the dominant frequency that sometimes exceeded between-individual differences. Bitterns could be identified from year to year by taking several measures of the boom manually [53]. Studies of Italian bitterns also showed that frequency-based features of bittern booms vary over time, and re-identification can be difficult [12].

In areas where vocal individuality is not used, bitterns have been counted using acoustic surveys of calling individuals. In low-density populations, individuals show high site fidelity [53] therefore simply counting calling individuals may be adequate. However, populations at higher densities, where individuals compete more strongly for resources, are likely to contain a number of non-territorial individuals that as a consequence are mobile and vocally active (floaters). Unless these individuals are accounted for they can cause large over-estimates of population size [39].

Corncrake

Corncrakes are a species of land rail that primarily inhabit hay meadows and silage fields [41]. Although population numbers in the UK have increased in recent years due to increased habitat management (currently estimated at 600–700 calling males [111]), they are listed as a species of global conservation concern (IUCN red-listed, [108]) and are on Annex 1 of the EU Wild Birds Directive [109]. As the species is secretive and nocturnal, the only viable method to census a population is by counting calling males in the breeding season. Currently 90% of the UK habitat for corncrakes is monitored in this manner [109].

The census strategy used for corncrakes was developed from radio-tagging studies [41], which found that males rarely move more than 250 m between nights and that on any particular night around 75% of males would call. Based on these findings, censuses were carried out on two nights and if two calling locations were within 250 m of each other on both nights, they were judged to come from the same individual [41]. Corncrakes have individually distinctive vocalizations [13, 112], which are consistent over years [13]. The census rules present obvious sources of bias and Peake & McGregor [40] monitored corncrakes using vocal individuality to show that males called far less than anticipated (41% of males per night) and this led to the population being underestimated by up to 30%. They were also able to follow individuals throughout the season and showed that habitat quality affected movement, with males in poor quality habitat moving greater distances. Their study also shows that tracking movements using the standard census approach was in most cases accurate [40]. The main role for vocal individuality in this case would be to provide correction factors to refine standard census rules or as a method for monitoring individual movements in relation to small-scale habitat differences.

Owl species

Owls, like most raptors, occur at low densities and are usually difficult to monitor. Most owl species are territorial and show high site fidelity after dispersal. However, monitoring territory occupancy, survival and recruitment can be difficult for these mostly nocturnal and secretive species. However, most owl species are characterised by their long-range vocalizations and these have been shown to be individually distinctive for several species (e.g. barred owls, Strix varia [113]; Northern saw whet owls, Aegolius arcadicus [114]; eagle owls, Bubo bubo [115]; screech owls, Otus asio [116]; Christmas Island hawk owls, Ninox natalis [117]; tawny owls, Strix aluco [75, 118]; pygmy owls, Glaucidium passerinum [76]; African wood owls, Strix woodfordii [77] European scops owls, Otus scops [54] and Seychelles scops owls (Otus insularis [Peake, Currie & McGregor pers comm.]). These vocalizations seem to remain stable during a season and, where studied, were consistent for long periods of time [54, 77, 115].

Vocal individuality has proved to be a useful tool for mapping territories in these species and monitoring habitat use through the breeding season. Galeotti & Sacchi [54] showed a high annual turnover on breeding territories for European scops owls, with 55–78% of territories occupied by different individuals between years (note that vocal individuality was validated by previous studies in this species). They suggest that this high turnover may be related to their migratory habits and possible high winter mortality, which leaves many vacant territories at the beginning of the breeding season [54]. African wood owls are a sedentary species where both sexes call. They form monogamous pairs that maintain stable territories. Delport et al [77] recorded wood owls at different calling locations over a 12-year period to monitor territory occupancy and turnover for each sex. They found a turnover of 19.3% and 13.7% for males and females respectively. This is one of the few examples where both sexes can be monitored with vocal individuality.

In both the previously mentioned cases [54, 77], individuals were not independently marked, and vocal stability is assumed based on previous studies of other owl species [75, 76] and the presence of temporal stability in the call features used [77]. However, this assumption of vocal stability has to be treated with care, as if a vocalization changes between years, it will not be possible to determine whether the individual or the vocalization is different.

Potential and limitations of vocal individuality

We have explained how vocal individuality can have a role in conservation and we have presented case studies in which it has been used to generate useful data. For such a potentially useful technique, vocal individuality is surprisingly under-utilised. Our case studies show a heavy bias towards male territorial signalling in avian species. This bias is a natural consequence of an avian bias in bioacoustics and communication generally and the fact that territorial signals are the most obvious. However, we feel that there are other classes of individual and other taxa that could be represented in vocal individuality studies.

In this section we discuss some of the potential limitations of vocal individuality (concentrating particularly on vocal stability and sex differences in vocal activity), we indicate the sort of information required to assess its potential as a conservation tool and we explore the potential of vocal individuality in mammals.

Some limitations of using vocal individuality

All identification techniques contain a number of potential biases and disadvantages that need to be borne in mind in order to minimise the chance of producing misleading results or limiting their explanatory power. Four main problems are encountered when using vocal individuality. First, establishing the extent and stability of individuality requires intensive study with, preferably, independently marked individuals. Second, this intensive study requires knowledge of sound analysis and the equipment used for recording and analysis. Third, vocal individuality is biased towards the most vocally active sections of the population (and there are similar biases in any method based on acoustics). Many factors can affect which portion of a population is vocally active, for example sex (e.g. it is the males of most temperate bird species that are vocally active), age, time of year, breeding status, territorial status (e.g. great horned owls, Bubo virginianus [55]) or dominance. For vocal individuality to provide useful information, these sub-groups have to be known. Fourth, because vocal individuality uses natural variation, there will always be a level of ambiguity in the identification of an individual [2]. We consider the most important current limitations on the value to conservation of vocal individuality to be vocal stability and monitoring females, so we shall discuss these topics in more detail.

Vocal stability over time

Vocal stability over time is difficult to show because ideally it requires independent identification of individuals. The case studies involving corncrakes and bitterns (see above) both used a sample of radio-tagged males to collect recordings over time [12, 41, 53]. With the exception of Lengagne [115], studies involving owls have looked for temporal stability in vocal features with time as an indication of vocal stability [54, 77]. They have also used previous studies showing long-term vocal stability in different owl species to support their assumptions [75, 76], although none of those individuals were independently marked. When using vocal individuality to re-identify individuals, for example when monitoring populations, establishing vocal stability needs special consideration and, as with bitterns (see above) may require an increase in effort, equipment and knowledge. Such extra effort may limit the applicability of vocal individuality in monitoring contexts.

Monitoring females

For many species, especially avian, monitoring the presence and movement of females will be difficult without radio-tracking. For example females of most temperate avian species are quiet and do not have long-range vocalizations. Exceptions to this include some non-oscine species such as petrels and some owl species. Thus in many cases population estimates are reported as the number of calling males rather than pairs (e.g. corncrakes and bitterns [108]). Indirect measures of female presence are sometimes possible, for example male corncrakes cease calling for some days when they have attracted a mate [13]. There are also exceptions, for example, female brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater) have a loud long-range individually distinctive call that can be used to identify them [119]. As mentioned above, male and female wood owls have long-range territorial vocalizations [77]. In cases where females do not have obvious long-range vocalisations, it may be possibly to exploit calls used to maintain contact between pairs, with offspring or with other social group members as a way of identifying and tracking females. For example, Campbell et al [97] showed that female Stellar sea lions could be identified from their mother-pup calls.

Colonial breeding species, such as many species of seabird and marine mammal, rely on contact calls (and other cues) to locate their mates and offspring [6, 97, 120, 121]. As such, the vocalisations of these species have been shown to contain high levels of individuality. In most cases, both the males and females vocalise, often with discriminable sex differences. Information from individuality could be combined with other census methods involving calls for these species, for example many species of petrel are burrow-nesting and nocturnal, however, they possess extensive vocal repertoires [122]. These species also readily respond to playback of calls and this method is used to census populations and to determine burrow occupation [123–125]. In some species, primarily tropical and monogamous, duetting is common between pair-bond members, and calls become more similar between the male and female as the bond develops with increasing time (e.g. gibbon species [126]; red-fronted parrots, Poicephalus gulielmi [127]; twites, Acanthus flavirostris [128]. Although this may make the discrimination task more difficult, it may yield information on pair identity when the individuals are separated and the length of the pair bond.

In species with more complex social systems females are also vocally active, and often both sexes can be discriminated by individual and gender [129]. Female chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) have individually distinctive contact and alarm calls [130] and Weiss et al [131] showed that cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) calls contain information on individual, sex and group identity. In several social canid species, females are as vocally active as males and take part in long-range vocalizing [132–136]. Vocalizations have been found to be individually distinctive for many of these species [78, 132–134]. However, the rate of vocalizing in some canid species is related to social dominance, season, and whether they are transient or resident groups [135, 136], and monitoring strategies would have to take account of this.

Vocal individuality as an effective conservation tool

Most of the literature suggesting that vocal individuality has a role in conservation is in fact represented by studies of the presence of vocal individuality and not whether it provides an effective conservation tool. To provide an indication of effectiveness as a conservation tool requires information on several topics and consideration of several issues. We consider the following to be most important.

  1. 1)

    The vocalization used should be easily recordable and provide the best potential for vocal individuality. It is generally considered that long-range advertising vocalizations most readily fulfil these criteria [65]. Species with a repertoire of such vocalizations may present the problem of which vocalization to use for individual identification. The problem can be addressed by either choosing one vocalization (such as the most common and/or distinctive) or by looking at features common to all vocalizations in the repertoire (e.g. [137, 138]).

  2. 2)

    The sample size tested should be similar to the number of individuals that will be discriminated when the technique is used. In many cases vocal individuality studies have used smaller sample sizes (10–20 individuals) than if the technique were to be used as a conservation tool.

  3. 3)

    Careful consideration is needed in choosing which measures to take and every effort should be made to pare them down (using principle components analysis or stepwise discriminant analysis) to the most effective discriminators. The best measures will be those that can be extracted from recordings of varying quality. Some techniques standardize recording quality by only accepting those recordings that include specific sections of the signal, usually of lower amplitude, for analysis [53]. Atmospheric conditions affect temporal, amplitude and frequency components of a signal in different ways [139]. Different analysis types are also affected by recording quality, for example the instantaneous frequency of bittern booms can discriminate between individuals even in poor quality recordings [110]. However, if temporal measures are to be taken from a waveform display, recordings have to be of a higher quality because background noise will mask the signal.

  4. 4)

    When multivariate statistical tools are used to discriminate and identify individuals, we recommend the use of similarity techniques (see above) as they do not need the population size to be known and they also allow for the identification of additional individuals.

  5. 5)

    Any application of vocal individuality will not use recordings in isolation to identify individuals. A lot of data can be collected at the time of recording that will reduce the number of individuals that have to be discriminated. The person making the recording can note other simultaneously calling individuals and the locations and times of all recordings. This can be important if a technique seems to lack discrimination power.

  6. 6)

    The most effective test of vocal individuality will be through simulated census and monitoring situations [81, 98]. These can be achieved through blind trials and repeated random sampling from known data sets to create population samples of unknown size and composition. We tested the use of neural network models in census and monitoring tasks by using a data set of 30 individuals that was randomly sampled [98]. The same individual could appear several times, and we used neural networks to classify individuals in a series of blind trials. These kinds of test more accurately simulate how the technique will perform as a conservation tool.

  7. 7)

    In most cases the people developing and testing monitoring techniques are not the same as those who will be using them in the field. Thus, one important, if obvious, point is that specific guidelines need to be given to those who will collect and analyse recordings. For many endangered or low-density populations, collecting recordings requires considerable fieldwork (e.g. [53]), and therefore this warrants the most efficient (effort vs. results) analysis possible. It may not be possible to rely on spontaneous vocalizing to collect enough recordings and playback is often used to elicit calls. However, the time of playback or its overuse can also have biasing effects, for example by causing individuals to move off their territories [37, 38].

Vocal individuality in mammals

As demonstrated by the examples we have used in this review, applications of vocal individuality have been limited to avian species, usually vocally active species that are difficult to monitor with conventional methods. A notable exception is the study of swift foxes (Vulpes velox) by Darden et al [79]. Many other mammalian species are vocally activity on land and are difficult to monitor conventionally because they occur at low densities in dense habitat. In addition, many have a rich vocal repertoire, with both sexes vocalizing or with vocal communication between parents and offspring. However, the general structure of the mammalian vocal system causes not only complex signal structures (e.g. harmonics) but also modifications (e.g. biphonation and chaotic noise), making it more difficult to analyse (but see [79]). Perhaps more importantly, the signalling context can have large effects on the structure of the sound produced, which can affect its use in identification [140]. This seems to be a common phenomenon in primates. For example, the degree of vocal variability in chimpanzees is related to the amount of social chorusing between males, with the amount of time males spent together increasing the similarity of their calls [140, 141]. Several marmoset species show individually distinctive vocalizations when either isolated [142] or in stable social groups, but in novel social conditions the vocal structures change [143]. Despite these potential problems, the number of reports of vocal individuality in mammals indicates that it could be a useful conservation tool (e.g. [144]).

For cetaceans, the underwater acoustic channel is the most important means of communication [145, 146]. Studying individual vocal differences in cetaceans and other aquatic mammals has proved to be difficult due to problems in identifying which individual is calling, with the result that most studies have been at the group or population level [145]. More recently, various techniques have been used to locate individual callers either with theodolites, hydrophone arrays, or tags [146]. Also individuals have been identified over longer periods of time using photo-identification, mostly of dorsal fin [147] or tail-fluke features [148]. However, visual identification techniques are less readily applied when large groups of individuals are together [146]. Few studies have been made of individually distinctive features of their vocal behaviour, and none have used this individuality to follow individuals.

The most studied cetaceans are dolphins. Bottle-nose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) have individually distinctive signature whistles [149–151], and these whistles can remain constant for over a decade [151, 152] but they can change depending on the social context [153, 154], and individuals are capable of copying the whistles of others [152] which then may be used in matched calling encounters [155]. Signature whistles would seem to be ideal for vocal individuality; however, social effects present several potential problems in their use. Also, the use of signature whistles may decrease when individuals are in groups [154]. It remains to be seen whether acoustic signals can be used to monitor dolphin species.

Large cetaceans are capable of communicating over large distances and are acoustically active, especially during the breeding season [145]. These acoustic signals have the ability to generate information on group identity, body size and interactions [146]. Studying cetaceans in stable social groups has revealed the more complex aspects of communication. Both sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) and killer whales (Orcinus orca) produce both individually and group distinctive signals [107, 156, 157]. The structure of sperm whale clicks also gives information about the size of the caller [158]. Few studies have used individuality to follow individuals (but see Fig. 10 in [152]).

Conclusion

There will be many instances where vocalizations are the only evidence of the presence of members of a population. Often, these populations are of conservation concern and baseline demographic information is difficult to obtain. Identifying individuals using individually distinctive vocalizations offers an alternative to marking that avoids problems associated with handling and sampling biases. As with other monitoring techniques, vocal individuality contains biases that have to be accounted for when it is used. Vocal individuality is not a panacea to all monitoring problems, but where a species is sensitive to disturbance or difficult to monitor (either through its behaviour or because of its environment), utilising its vocal behaviour can provide an effective conservation tool.

References

  1. Baptista LF, Gaunt SLL: Bioacoustics as a tool in conservation studies. Behavioural Approaches to Conservation in the Wild. Edited by: Clemmens JR, Buchholz R. 1997, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 209-211.

    Google Scholar 

  2. McGregor PK, Peake TM, Gilbert G: Communication behaviour and conservation. Behaviour and Conservation. Edited by: Sutherland WJ, Gosling M. 2000, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 261-280.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Curio E: Conservation needs ethology. Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 1996, 11: 260-263.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Emlen JT, Dejong MD: Counting birds: the problem of variable hearing abilities. Journal of Field Ornithology. 1992, 63: 26-31.

    Google Scholar 

  5. McGregor PK, Peake TM: The role of individual identification in conservation biology. Behavioral Ecology and Conservation Biology. Edited by: Caro TM. 1998, Oxford; Oxford University Press, 31-55.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Falls JB: Individual recognition by sound in birds. Acoustic Communication in Birds. Edited by: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH. 1982, New York: Academic Press, 2: 237-278.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Stoddard PK: Vocal recognition of neighbours by territorial passerines. Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds. Edited by: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH. 1996, Ithaca: Cornell University Press, 356-374.

    Google Scholar 

  8. McGregor PK: Signalling in territorial systems: a context for individual identification, ranging and eavesdropping. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B. 1993, 340: 237-244.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Suthers RA: Variable asymmetry and resonance in the avian vocal tract: a structural basis for individually distinct vocalizations. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1994, 175: 457-466.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Eakle WL, Mannan RW, Grubb TG: Identification of individual breeding bald eagles by voice analysis. Journal of Wildlife Management. 1989, 53: 450-455.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Gilbert G, McGregor PK, Tyler G: Vocal individuality as a census tool, practical considerations illustrated by a study of two rare species. Journal of Field Ornithology. 1994, 65: 335-348.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Puglisi L, Adamo C: Discrimination of individual voices in male great bitterns (Botaurus stellaris) in Italy. The Auk. 2004, 121: 541-527.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Peake TM, McGregor PK, Smith KW, Tyler G, Gilbert G, Green RE: Individuality in Corncrake Crex crex vocalisations. Ibis. 1998, 140: 120-127.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Beightol DR, Samuel DE: Sonagraphic analysis of the American woodcock's peent call. Journal of Wildlife Management. 1973, 37: 470-475.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Saunders DA, Wooller RD: Consistent individuality of voice in birds as a management tool. Emu. 1988, 88: 25-32.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Goss-Custard JD, Caldow RWG, Clarke RT, LeVDit Durell SEA, Urfi J, West AD: Consequences of habitat loss and change to populations of wintering migratory birds: predicting the local and global effects from studies of individuals. Ibis. 1995, 137: S56-S66.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Sutherland WJ: From Individual Behaviour to Population Ecology. 1996, Oxford: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  18. Goss-Custard JD, Sutherland WJ: Individual behaviour, populations and conservation. Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Edited by: Krebs JR, Davies NB. 1997, Oxford: Blackwell, 373-395.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Stillman RA, Goss-Custard JD, West AD, Le V Dit Durell SEA, Caldow RWG, McGrorty S, Clarke RT: Predicting mortality in novel environments: tests and sensitivity of a behaviour-based model. Journal of Applied Ecology. 2000, 37: 564-588.

    Google Scholar 

  20. West AD, Goss-Custard JD, Stillman RA, Caldow RWG, Le V Dit Durell SEA, McGrorty S: Predicting the impacts of disturbance on shorebird mortality using a behaviour-based model. Biological Conservation. 2002, 106: 319-328.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Reed JM, Mills LS, Dunning JB, Menges ES, McKelvey KS, Frye R, Beissinger SR, Anstett MC, Miller P: Emerging issues in population viability analysis. Conservation Biology. 2002, 16: 7-19.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Lande R: Risks of population extinction from demographic and environmental stochasticity and random catastrophes. American Naturalist. 1993, 142: 911-927.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Kendall BE, Fox GA: Variation among individuals and reduced demographic stochasticity. Conservation Biology. 2002, 16: 109-116.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Fox GA, Kendall BE: Demographic stochasticity and the variance reduction effect. Ecology. 2002, 83: 1928-1934.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Kruger O, Lindstrom J: Lifetime reproductive success in common buzzard, Buteo buteo : from individual variation to population demography. Oikos. 2001, 93: 260-273.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Newton I: The contribution of some recent research on birds to ecological understanding. Journal of Animal Ecology. 1995, 64: 675-696.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Kelly MJ: Lineage loss in Serengeti cheetahs: consequences of high reproductive variance and heritability of fitness on effective population size. Conservation Biology. 2001, 15: 137-147.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Clutton-Brock TH: Reproductive success. Reproductive Success. Edited by: Clutton-Brock TH. 1988, University of Chicago Press, 472-485.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Gompper ME, Stacey PB, Berger J: Conservation implications of the natural loss of lineages in wild mammals and birds. Conservation Biology. 1997, 11: 857-867.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Smith JNM, Arcese P: How fit are floaters? Consequences of alternative territorial behaviors in a nonmigratory sparrow. American Naturalist. 1989, 133: 830-845.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Bayne EM, Hobson KA: Effects of habitat fragmentation on pairing success of ovenbirds: importance of male age and floater behaviour. The Auk. 2001, 118: 380-388.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Kempenaers B, Everding S, Bishop C, Boag P, Robertson RJ: Extra-pair paternity and the reproductive role of male floaters in the tree swallow (Tachycineta bicolor). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology. 2001, 49: 251-259.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Komdeur J: Breeding the Seychelles magpie robin Copsychus sechellarum and implications for its conservation. Ibis. 1996, 138: 485-498.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Bibby CJ, Burgess ND, Hill DA, Mustoe SH: Bird Census Techniques. 2000, London: Academic Press, 2

    Google Scholar 

  35. Greenwood JJD: Basic Techniques. Ecological Census Techniques. Edited by: Sutherland WJ. 2000, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 11-110.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Conway CJ, Eddleman WR, Anderson SH, Hanebury LR: Seasonal changes in Yuma clapper rail vocalisation rate and habitat use. Journal of Wildlife Management. 1993, 57: 282-290.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Legare ML, Eddleman WR, Buckley PA, Kelly C: The effectiveness of tape playback in estimating black rail density. Journal of Wildlife Management. 1999, 63: 116-125.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Ogutu JO, Dublin HT: The response of lions and spotted hyaenas to sound playbacks as a technique for estimating population size. African Journal of Ecology. 1998, 36: 83-95.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Eriksen T: Booming bitterns Botaurus stellaris : ecology, behaviour and conservation implications. MSc thesis. 1999, Copenhagen University, Zoological Institute

    Google Scholar 

  40. Peake TM, McGregor PK: Corncrake Crex crex census estimates: a conservation application of vocal individuality. Animal Biodiversity & Conservation. 2001, 24: 81-90.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Stowe TJ, Hudson AV: Corncrake studies in the western isles. Royal Society for the Protection of Birds Conservation Review. 1988, 2: 38-42.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Moorhouse TP, MacDonald DWJ: Indirect negative impacts of radio-collaring: sex-ratio variation in water voles. Journal of Applied Ecology. 2005, 42: 91-98.

    Google Scholar 

  43. de Villiers MS, Meltzer DGA, van Heerden J, Mills MGL, Richardson PRK, van Jaarsveld AS: Handling-induced stress and mortalities in African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. 1995, 262: 215-220.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Woodruffe R: Assessing the risks of intervention: immobilization, radio-collaring and vaccination of African wild dogs. Oryx. 2001, 35: 234-244.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Castelli PM, Trost RE: Neck bands reduce survival of Canada geese in New Jersey. Journal of Wildlife Management. 1996, 60: 891-898.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Menu S, Hestbeck JB, Gauthier G, Reed A: Effects of neck bands on survival of greater snow geese. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2000, 64: 544-552.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Schmutz JA, Morse JA: Effects of neck collars and radiotransmitters on survival and reproduction of emperor geese. Journal of Wildlife Management. 2000, 64: 231-237.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Alisauskas RT, Lindberg MS: Effects of neckbands on survival and fidelity of white-fronted and Canada geese captured as non-breeding adults. Journal of Applied Statistics. 2002, 29: 521-537.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Nimon AJ, Schroter RC, Stonehouse B: Heart-rate of disturbed penguins. Nature. 1995, 374: 415-

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Culik B, Wilson R: Penguins crowded out. Nature. 1991, 351: 340-

    Google Scholar 

  51. Animal Behaviour: Guidelines for the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching. Animal Behaviour. 2005, 69: i-vi. http://0-www-elsevier-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/framework_products/promis_misc/622782guide.pdf

    Google Scholar 

  52. Caughley G, Gunn A: Conservation Biology in Theory and Practice. 1996, Massachusetts: Blackwell Science

    Google Scholar 

  53. Gilbert G, Tyler GA, Smith KW: Local annual survival of booming male great bittern Botaurus stellaris in Britain, in the period 1990–1999. Ibis. 2002, 144: 51-61.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Galeotti P, Sacchi R: Turnover of territorial scops owl Otus scops as estimated by spectrographic analysis of male hoots. Journal of Avian Biology. 2001, 32: 256-262.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Rohner C: The numerical response of great horned owls to the snowshoe hare cycle: consequences of non-territorial 'floaters' on demography. Journal of Animal Ecology. 1996, 65: 359-370.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Hopp SL, Owren MJ, Evans CS, eds: Animal Acoustic Communication: Recent Technical Advances. 1998, Berlin: Springer

    Google Scholar 

  57. Kroodsma DE, Budney GF, Grotke RW, Vielliard JME, Gaunt SLL, Ranft R, Veprintseva OD: Natural sound archives: guidance for recordists and a request for cooperation. Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds. Edited by: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH. 1996, Ithaca: Cornell University Press, 474-486.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Gerhardt HC: Acoustic signals of animals: recording, field measurements, analysis and description. Animal Acoustic Communication: Recent Technical Advances. Edited by: Hopp SL, Owren MJ, Evans CS. 1998, Berlin: Springer, 1-26.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Charif RA, Mitchell S, Clark CW: Canary 1.1 User's Manual. 1993, Ithaca: Cornell University Press

    Google Scholar 

  60. Clements M: Digital signal acquisition and representation. Animal Acoustic Communication: Recent Technical Advances. Edited by: Hopp SL, Owren MJ, Evans CS. 1998, Berlin: Springer, 27-58.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Stoddard PK: Application of filters in bioacoustics. Animal Acoustic Communication: Recent Technical Advances. Edited by: Hopp SL, Owren MJ, Evans CS. 1998, Berlin: Springer, 105-128.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Beecher M: Signalling systems for individual recognition: an information theory approach. Animal Behaviour. 1989, 38: 248-261.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Gilbert G: Vocal individuality as a census and monitoring tool: practical considerations illustrated by a study of the bittern Botaurus stellaris and the black-throated diver Gavia immer. PhD thesis. 1993, Nottingham University, Department of Life Science

    Google Scholar 

  64. Miller E, Dring T: Territorial defense of multiple lakes by common loons: a preliminary report. Papers from the 1987 Conference on Loon Research and Management. Edited by: Strong PIV, Merideth NH. 1988, North American Loon Fund, 1-14.

    Google Scholar 

  65. McGregor PK, Byle P: Individually distinct bittern booms: potential as a census tool. Bioacoustics. 1992, 4: 93-109.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Janik VM: Pitfalls in the categorization of behaviour: a comparison of dolphin whistle classification methods. Animal Behaviour. 1999, 57: 133-143.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Jones AE, ten Cate C, Bijleveld CCJH: The inter-observer reliability of scoring sonagrams by eye: a study on methods, illustrated on zebra finch songs. Animal Behaviour. 2001, 62: 791-801.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Clapperton BK, Jenkins PF: Individuality in contact calls of the Pukeko (Aves: Rallidae). New Zealand Journal of Zoology. 1987, 14: 19-28.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Clark CW, Marler P, Beeman K: Quantitative analysis of animal vocal phonology: an application to swamp sparrow song. Ethology. 1987, 76: 105-115.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Williams JM: Objective comparisons of song syllables – a dynamic programming approach. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1993, 161: 317-328.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Harper DGC: Some comments on the repeatability of measurements. Ringing and Migration. 1994, 15: 84-90.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Sparling DW, Williams JD: Multivariate analysis of avian vocalisations. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 1978, 74: 83-107.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Manly BFJ: Multivariate Statistical Methods: A Primer. 1986, London: Chapman & Hall

    Google Scholar 

  74. Bauer H-G, Nagl W: Individual distinctiveness and possible function of song parts of short-toed treecreepers (Certhia brachydactyla). Evidence from multivariate song analysis. Ethology. 1992, 91: 108-121.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Galeotti P, Pavan G: Individual recognition of male tawny owls (Strix aluco) using spectrograms of their territorial calls. Ethology, Ecology and Evolution. 1991, 3: 113-126.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Galeotti P, Paladin M, Pavan G: Individually distinct hooting in male pygmy owls Glaucidium passerinum : a multivariate approach. Ornis Scandinavica. 1993, 24: 15-20.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Delport W, Kemp AC, Ferguson JWH: Vocal identification of individual African wood owls Strix woodfordii : a technique to monitor long-term adult turnover and residency. Ibis. 2002, 144: 30-39.

    Google Scholar 

  78. Durbin LS: Individuality in the whistle call of the Asiatic wild dog Cuon alpinus. Bioacoustics. 1998, 9: 197-206.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Darden SK, Dablesteen T, Pedersen SB: A potential tool for swift fox Vulpes velox conservation: individuality of long-range barking sequences. Journal of Mammalogy. 2003, 84: 1417-1427.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Jorgensen DD, French JA: Individuality but not stability in marmoset long calls. Ethology. 1998, 104: 729-742.

    Google Scholar 

  81. Terry AMR, McGregor PK, Peake TM: A comparison of some techniques used to assess vocal individuality. Bioacoustics. 2001, 11: 169-188.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Nelson DA: Song overproduction and selective attrition lead to song sharing in the field sparrow (Spizella puslla). Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology. 1992, 30: 415-424.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Rebbeck M, Corrick R, Eaglestone B, Stainton C: Recognition of individual European nightjars Caprimulgus europaeus from their song. Ibis. 2001, 143: 468-475.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Khanna H, Gaunt SLL, McCallum DA: Digital spectrographic cross-correlation: tests of sensitivity. Bioacoustics. 1997, 7: 209-234.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Dahlquist FC, Schemnitz SD, Flachs BK: Distinguishing individual male wild turkeys by analyzing vocalisations using a personal computer. Bioacoustics. 1990, 2: 303-316.

    Google Scholar 

  86. Evans MR Evans JA: A computer based technique for the quantitative analysis of animal sounds. Bioacoustics. 1994, 5: 281-290.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Lessells CM, Rowe CL, McGregor PK: Individual and sex differences in the provisioning calls of European bee-eaters. Animal Behaviour. 1995, 49: 244-247.

    Google Scholar 

  88. McGregor PK, Anderson CM, Harris J, Seal JR, Soul JM: Individual differences in songs of fantailed warblers Cisticola juncidis in Portugal. Airo. 1995, 5: 17-21.

    Google Scholar 

  89. Latruffe C, McGregor PK, Tavares JP, Mota PG: Microgeographic variation in corn bunting (Miliaria calandra) song: quantitative and discrimination aspects. Behaviour. 2000, 137: 1241-1255.

    Google Scholar 

  90. Nelson DA, Marler P, Palleroni A: A comparative approach to vocal learning: intra-specific variation in the learning process. Animal Behaviour. 1995, 50: 83-97.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Cortopassi KA, Bradbury JW: The comparison of harmonically rich sounds using spectrographic cross-correlation and principle coordinates analysis. Bioacoustics. 2000, 11: 89-127.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Peake TM: Variation in the vocal behaviour of the corncrake Crex crex : potential for conservation. PhD thesis. 1997, University of Nottingham, Department of Life Science

    Google Scholar 

  93. Buck JR, Tyack PL: A quantitative measure of similarity for Tursiops truncatus signature whistles. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1993, 94: 2497-2506.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Anderson SE, Dave AS, Margoliash D: Template based automatic recognition of birdsong syllables from continuous recordings. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1996, 100: 1209-1219.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Kogan JA, Margoliash D: Automated recognition of bird song elements from continuous recordings using dynamic time warping and hidden Markov models: a comparative study. Journal of the Acoustical Society America. 1998, 103: 2185-2197.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  96. Reby D, Lek S, Dimopoulos I, Joachim J, Lauga J, Aulagnier S: Artificial neural networks as a classification method in the behavioural sciences. Behavioural Processes. 1997, 40: 35-43.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Campbell GS, Gisiner RC, Helweg DA, Milette LL: Acoustic identification of female Steller sea lions (Eumetopias jubatus). Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 2002, 111: 2920-2928.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Terry AMR, McGregor PK: Census and monitoring based on individually identifiable vocalizations: the role of neural networks. Animal Conservation. 2002, 5: 103-111.

    Google Scholar 

  99. Hinton GE: How neural networks learn from experience. Scientific American. 1992, 105-109.

    Google Scholar 

  100. Gurney K: An Introduction to Neural Networks. 1997, London: UCL Press

    Google Scholar 

  101. Bishop CM: Neural Networks for Pattern Recognition. 1995, Oxford: Clarendon Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  102. Kohonen T: Self-organising Maps. 1997, Berlin: Springer

    Google Scholar 

  103. Phelps SM, Ryan MJ: Neural networks predict response biases of female tungara frogs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. 1998, 265: 279-285.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  104. Phelps SM, Ryan MJ: History influences signal recognition: neural network models of tungara frogs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. 2000, 267: 1633-1639.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Reby D, Joachim J, Lauga J, Lek S, Aulagnier S: Individuality in the groans of fallow deer (Dama dama) bucks. Journal of Zoology London. 1998, 245: 79-84.

    Google Scholar 

  106. Placer J, Slobodchikoff CN: A fuzzy-neural system for identification of species-specific alarm calls of Gunnison's prairie dogs. Behavioural Processes. 2000, 52: 1-9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Deecke VB, Ford JKB, Spong P: Quantifying complex patterns of bioacoustic variation: use of a neural network to compare killer whale (Orcinus orca) dialects. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1999, 105: 2499-2507.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Gibbons DW, Avery M, Baillie S, Gregory R, Kirby J, Porter R, Tucker G, Williams G: Bird species of conservation concern in the United Kingdom, Channel Islands and Isle of Man: revising the Red data list. RSPB Conservation Review. 1996, 10: 7-18.

    Google Scholar 

  109. Gilbert G, Gibbons DW, Evans J: Bird Monitoring Methods. A Manual of Techniques for Key UK Species. 1998, Sandy: RSPB

    Google Scholar 

  110. Terry AMR: Bioacoustics in conservation: new approaches using vocal individuality. PhD thesis. 2002, Copenhagen University, Zoological Institute

    Google Scholar 

  111. Gregory RD, Noble DG, Cranswick PA, Campbell LH, Rehfisch MM, Baillie SR: The state of the UK's birds 2000. 2001, Sandy: RSPB, BTO and WWT

    Google Scholar 

  112. May L: Individually distinctive corncrake Crex crex calls: a pilot study. Bioacoustics. 1994, 6: 25-32.

    Google Scholar 

  113. Freeman PL: Identification of individual barred owls using spectrogram analysis and auditory cues. Journal of Raptor Research. 2000, 34: 85-92.

    Google Scholar 

  114. Otter K: Individual variation in the advertising call of male Northern saw-whet owls. Journal of Field Ornithology. 1996, 67: 398-405.

    Google Scholar 

  115. Lengagne T: Temporal stability in the individual features in the calls of eagle owls (Bubo bubo). Behaviour. 2001, 138: 1407-1419.

    Google Scholar 

  116. Cavanagh PM, Ritchison G: Variation in the bounce and whinny songs of the Eastern screech-owl. Wilson Bulletin. 1987, 99: 620-627.

    Google Scholar 

  117. Hill FAR, Lill A: Vocalisations of the Christmas Island hawk-owl Ninox natalis : individual variation in advertisement calls. Emu. 1998, 98: 221-226.

    Google Scholar 

  118. Appleby BM, Redpath SM: Variation in the male territorial hoot of the tawny owl Strix aluca in three English populations. Ibis. 1997, 139: 152-158.

    Google Scholar 

  119. Burnell K, Rothstein SJ: Variation in the structure of female brown-headed cowbird vocalizations and its relation to vocal function and development. Condor. 1994, 96: 703-715.

    Google Scholar 

  120. Speirs EAH, Davis LS: Discrimination by Adelie penguins, Pygoscelis adelie, between the loud mutual calls of mates, neighbours and strangers. Animal Behaviour. 1991, 41: 937-944.

    Google Scholar 

  121. Aubin T, Jouventin P: How to vocally identify kin in a crowd: the penguin model. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 2002, 31: 243-277.

    Google Scholar 

  122. Warham J: The Behaviour, Population Biology and Physiology of the Petrels. 1996, San Diego: Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  123. Bretagnolle V: Calls of Wilson's storm petrel: functions, individual and sexual recognitions and geographic variation. Behaviour. 1989, 111: 98-l12.

    Google Scholar 

  124. Bretagnolle V, Robinson P: Species-specific recognition in birds: an experimental investigation in the Wilson's storm petrel (Procellariiformes, Hydrobatidae) using digitalized signals. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1991, 69: 1669-1673.

    Google Scholar 

  125. Berrow SD: The use of acoustics to monitor burrow-nesting white-chinned petrels Procellaria aequinoctialis at Bird Island, South Georgia. Polar Biology. 2000, 23: 575-579.

    Google Scholar 

  126. Geissmann T: Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biological Reviews. 2002, 77: 57-76.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  127. Venuto VMRBL: African parrot vocalizations and their functional significance. Ostrich. 2001, 224-228. Suppl 15

  128. Marler P, Mundinger PC: Vocalizations, social-organization and breeding biology of twite Acanthus flavirostris. Ibis. 1975, 117: 1-6.

    Google Scholar 

  129. Benz JJ, French JA, Leger DW: Sex differences in vocal structure in a callitrichid primate Leontopithecus rosalia. American Journal of Primatology. 1990, 21: 257-264.

    Google Scholar 

  130. Fischer J, Hammerschmidt K, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM: Acoustic features of female chacma baboon barks. Ethology. 2001, 107: 33-54.

    Google Scholar 

  131. Weiss DJ, Garibaldi BT, Hauser MD: The production and perception of long calls by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): acoustic analyses and playback experiments. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2001, 115: 258-271.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  132. Fromolt KH, Kruchenkova EP, Russig H: Individuality of territorial barking in Arctic foxes, Alopex lagopus. Zeitschrift fur Saugetierkunde. 1997, 62: 66-70.

    Google Scholar 

  133. Tooze ZJ, Harrington FH, Fentress JC: Individually distinct vocalisations in timber wolves, Canis lupus. Animal Behaviour. 1990, 40: 723-730.

    Google Scholar 

  134. East ML, Hofer H: Loud calling in a female-dominated mammalian society: I. structure and composition of whooping bouts of spotted hyenas, Crocuta crocuta. Animal Behaviour. 1991, 42: 637-649.

    Google Scholar 

  135. East ML, Hofer H: Loud calling in a female-dominated mammalian society: II. behavioural contexts and functions of whooping of spotted hyenas, Crocuta crocuta. Animal Behaviour. 1991, 42: 651-669.

    Google Scholar 

  136. Gese EM, Ruff RL: Howling by coyotes (Canis latrans): variation among social classes, seasons and pack sizes. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1998, 76: 1037-1043.

    Google Scholar 

  137. Falls JB, Krebs JR, McGregor PK: Song matching in the great tit (Parus major): the effects of similarity and familiarity. Animal Behaviour. 1982, 30: 997-1009.

    Google Scholar 

  138. Weary DW, Krebs JR: Great tits classify songs by individual voice characteristics. Animal Behaviour. 1992, 43: 283-287.

    Google Scholar 

  139. Wiley RH, Richards DG: Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound transmission and signal detection. Acoustic Communication in Birds. Edited by: Kroodsma DE, Miller EH. 1982, New York: Academic Press, 2: 132-164.

    Google Scholar 

  140. Mitani JC, Brandt KL: Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-distance calls of male chimpanzees. Ethology. 1994, 96: 233-252.

    Google Scholar 

  141. Mitani JC, Gros JL: Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: tests of three hypothesis. Behaviour. 1998, 135: 1041-1064.

    Google Scholar 

  142. Jones BS, Harris DHR, Catchpole CK: The stability of the vocal signature in Phee calls of the common marmoset, Callithrix jacchus. American Journal of Primatology. 1993, 31: 67-75.

    Google Scholar 

  143. Elowson AM, Snowdon CT: Pygmy marmosets, Cebuella pygmaea, modify vocal structure in response to changed social environment. Animal Behaviour. 1994, 47: 1267-1277.

    Google Scholar 

  144. Hartig S: Individual acoustic identification as a non-invasive conservation tool: an approach to the conservation of the African wild dog Lycaon pictus. Bioacoustics. 2005, 15: 35-50.

    Google Scholar 

  145. Tyack PL: Functional aspects of cetacean communication. Cetacean Societies: Field Studies of Dolphins and Whales. Edited by: Mann J, Connor R, Tyack P, Whitehead H. 2000, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 270-307.

    Google Scholar 

  146. Whitehead H, Christal J, Tyack PL: Studying cetacean social structure in space and time: innovative techniques. Cetacean Societies: Field Studies of Dolphins and Whales. Edited by: Mann J, Connor R, Tyack P, Whitehead H. 2000, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 65-90.

    Google Scholar 

  147. Ford JKB: Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia (Canada). Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1991, 69: 1454-1483.

    Google Scholar 

  148. Dufault S, Whitehead H: An assessment of changes with time in the marking patterns used for photo-identification of individual sperm whales, Physeter macrocephalus. Marine Mammal Science. 1995, 11: 335-343.

    Google Scholar 

  149. Caldwell MC, Caldwell DK: Vocalization of naive captive dolphins in small groups. Science. 1968, 159: 1121-1123.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  150. Caldwell MC, Caldwell DK, Tyack PL: Review of the signature-whistle hypothesis for the Atlantic bottlenose dolphin. The Bottlenose Dolphin. Edited by: Leatherwood S, Reeves RR. 1990, San Diego: Academic Press, 199-234.

    Google Scholar 

  151. Sayigh LS, Tyack PL, Wells RS, Scott MD: Signature whistles of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus – stability and mother offspring comparisons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1990, 26: 247-260.

    Google Scholar 

  152. Tyack P: Acoustic communication under the sea. Animal Acoustic Communication: Recent Technical Advances. Edited by: Hopp SL, Owren MJ, Evans CS. 1998, Berlin: Springer, 163-220.

    Google Scholar 

  153. Janik VM, Dehnhardt G, Todt D: Signature whistle variation in a bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1994, 35: 243-248.

    Google Scholar 

  154. Janik VM, Slater PJB: Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls. Animal Behaviour. 1998, 56: 829-838.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  155. Janik VM: Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Science. 2000, 289: 1355-

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  156. Weilgart LS, Whitehead H: Distinctive vocalizations from mature mature sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus). Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1988, 66: 1931-1937.

    Google Scholar 

  157. Ford JKB: Acoustic behavior of resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1989, 67: 727-745.

    Google Scholar 

  158. Goold JC: Signal processing techniques for acoustic measurement of sperm whale body lengths. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 1996, 100: 3431-3441.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

AMRT was funded by the Leverhulme Trust, TMP by the University of Copenhagen and PKM by Statens Naturvidenskablige Forskningsråd grant 21-01-0482. We thank the communication crew, Mark Bee, Vincent Bretagnolle and two anonymous referees for their helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter K McGregor.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

AMRT carried out the initial literature survey and drafted the first version of the manuscript. All authors expanded and developed the review. PKM produced the final version of the manuscript which was read and approved by all authors.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Terry, A.M., Peake, T.M. & McGregor, P.K. The role of vocal individuality in conservation. Front Zool 2, 10 (2005). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1742-9994-2-10

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1742-9994-2-10

Keywords