Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

How does bridging social capital relate to health-behavior, overweight and obesity among low and high educated groups? A cross-sectional analysis of GLOBE-2014

Abstract

Background

Social capital is an important determinant of health, but how specific sub-dimensions of social capital affect health and health-related behaviors is still unknown. To better understand its role for health inequalities, it is important to distinguish between bonding social capital (connections between homogenous network members; e.g. similar educational level) and bridging social capital (connections between heterogeneous network members). In this study, we test the hypotheses that, 1) among low educational groups, bridging social capital is positively associated with health-behavior, and negatively associated with overweight and obesity, and 2) among high educational groups, bridging social capital is negatively associated with health-behavior, and positively with overweight and obesity.

Methods

Cross-sectional data on educational level, health-behavior, overweight and obesity from participants (25ā€“75ā€‰years; Eindhoven, the Netherlands) of the 2014-survey of the GLOBE study were used (NĀ =ā€‰2702). Social capital (ā€œHow many of your close friends have the same educational level as you have?ā€) was dichotomized as: bridging (ā€˜about halfā€™, ā€˜someā€™, or ā€˜none of my friendsā€™), or bonding (ā€˜allā€™ or ā€˜most of my friendsā€™). Logistic regression models were used to study whether bridging social capital was related to health-related behaviors (e.g.Ā smoking, food intake, physical activity), overweight and obesity, and whether these associations differed between low and high educational groups.

Results

Among low educated, having bridging social capital (i.e. friends with a higher educational level) reduced the likelihood to report overweight (OR 0.73, 95% CI 0.52ā€“1.03) and obesity (OR 0.58, 95% CI 0.38ā€“0.88), compared to low educated with bonding social capital. In contrast, among high educated, having bridging social capital (i.e. friends with a lower educational level) increased the likelihood to report daily smoking (OR 2.11, 95% CI 1.37ā€“3.27), no leisure time cycling (OR 1.55, 95% CI 1.17ā€“2.04), not meeting recommendations for vegetable intake (OR 2.09, 95% CI 1.50ā€“2.91), and high meat intake (OR 1.39, 95% CI 1.05ā€“1.83), compared to high educated with bonding social capital.

Conclusions

Bridging social capital had differential relations with health-behavior among low and high educational groups. Policies aimed at reducing segregation between educational groups may reduce inequalities in overweight, obesity and unhealthy behaviors.

Peer Review reports

Background

Social capital is acknowledged as an important ā€˜social determinant of healthā€™ that can promote (or harm) health through several mechanisms [1,2,3,4]. Two broad streams of research have emerged with regard to social capital. The first conceptualizes social capital on the collective level, as the resources available to members of a community such as trust, or exercise of sanctions; well-known from the work of Putnam [5]. The second stream conceptualizes social capital on the individual level, i.e. as the resources that are embedded within an individualā€™s social network, e.g. social support, norms; as in the work of Bourdieu [6, 7]. Both collective and individual social capital are independently associated with health [8,9,10], but via different pathways. In this study, we focus on individual-level social capital (hereafter: social capital, unless indicated otherwise). Although there is an abundance of evidence that confirms relationships between socioeconomic position, social capital and health in general, fewer studies have tested more specific underlying pathways, e.g. the more detailed roles of sub-dimensions of social capital, or differential roles of social capital for low and high socioeconomic groups [1, 3, 7,8,9,10,11].

In relation to health inequalities, the distinction between bonding and bridging social capital is particularly important. Bonding social capital refers to ā€œinward-lookingā€ connections between members of a network who are similar to each other (for instance with respect to ethnicity, age, or social class), which enhances access to internal resources, possibly by reinforcing exclusive social identities [2, 12, 13]. Bridging social capital, by contrast, refers to the ā€œoutward-lookingā€ connections between members of a network who are dissimilar to each other, and thus to ties between heterogeneous, socially diverse groups which may enhance access to external resources [2, 12, 13]. Interactions that represent bridging prospects have declined over time, which has been observed in the U.S. [14] as well as other Western societies [15, 16], like the Netherlands. In the Netherlands, educational attainment is the most important dimension of segregation, and it has been shown that low and high educated people increasingly live separate lives, with different preferences and different lifestyles [15, 17]. Also, health-related behaviors differ remarkably when comparing low and high educated groups ā€“ more so than when comparing income or occupational groups [18,19,20]. Therefore, in the Dutch context, it is especially relevant to understand how education-specific bridging social capital (i.e. having friends with a higher or lower educational level compared to oneā€™s own educational level) relates to health-behavior.

We hypothesize that low educated groups with education-specific bridging social capital, i.e. ties to higher educated groups, may be more likely to behave healthily, than low educated with bonding social capital. We reason that high educated, more often than low educated, behave healthily, have positive attitudes towards health-behavior, and provide social support for health-behavior [21,22,23,24], and that therefore, low educated with higher educated friends (i.e. bridging social capital) may be more likely to experience descriptive norms and social support for health-behavior, orĀ to (unconsciously)Ā mimic their higher-educated friendsā€™ health-behavior. For high educated groups, the reverse may be true: high educated with bridging social capital (i.e. friends with a lower educational level) may be more likely to behave unhealthily, compared to high educated with bonding social capital. These hypotheses have not been tested before. Therefore, this study addresses the following research question: to what extent is bridging social capital differentially related to health-behavior of high and low educated groups?

Methods

Data were collected by means of a large-scale postal survey in 2014, administered as the fifth wave of data collection for the longitudinal Dutch GLOBE study (responseā€‰=ā€‰45.5%) [18]. A cross-sectional stratified sample of the 25ā€“75ā€‰years old population in the city of Eindhoven was used in the analyses (NĀ =ā€‰2812) [20, 25]. More detailed information on the objectives, study design, and data collection of the Dutch GLOBE study can be found elsewhere [26, 27]. The use of personal data in the GLOBE study is in compliance with the Dutch Personal Data Protection Act and the Municipal Database Act, and has been registered with the Dutch Data Protection Authority (number 1248943).

Educational level and education-specific bridging social capital

Educational level is an important indicator of social stratification in contemporary Dutch society [15, 17, 28]. Survey participants reported their highest attained educational level, which was classified according to the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED): 1ā€“ high education (tertiary education (ISCED 5ā€“7)); 2ā€“ mid education (upper secondary education (ISCED 3ā€“4)); 3ā€“ low education (primary education and lower secondary education (ISCED 0ā€“2)).

Education-specific bridging social capital was measured with the question: ā€œHow many of your close friends have the same educational level as you have?ā€, with five answering options: all, most of them, about half, some, and none. Bridging social capital was coded as ā€˜1ā€‰=ā€‰bridgingā€™ for those who answered ā€˜about halfā€™, ā€˜someā€™, or ā€˜none of my friendsā€™, and ā€˜0ā€‰=ā€‰bondingā€™ for those who answered ā€˜allā€™ or ā€˜most of my friendsā€™.

Health-behavior, overweight and obesity

Sports participation was measured by means of the validated Short QUestionnaire to ASsess Health-enhancing physical activity (SQUASH) [29]. Participants were asked to think about their sports participation over the past month and to write down up to four different types of sports they had participated in on a weekly basis. For each type of sport they reported frequency (days per week), duration (hours and minutes per day) and intensity (light, moderate, intense). Self-reported intensity and activity-specific intensity Metabolic Equivalents (METs) were used to calculate the numbers of days participants participated in sports for at least 30ā€‰min at moderate or vigorous intensity (moderate intensityā€‰=ā€‰4ā€“6 MET for 18ā€“55ā€‰years and 3ā€“5 MET for 55+ years). The variable was dichotomized into 1) no sports participation at least once per week for >ā€‰30ā€‰min at moderate intensity, vs. 0) sports participation at least once per week for >ā€‰30ā€‰min at moderate intensity (reference group).

Walking and cycling in leisure time were also measured as part of the SQUASH. Participants reported frequency (days per week), duration (hours and minutes per day) and intensity (light, moderate, intense) for walking and cycling in their leisure time. For both activities separately, we calculated how many days per week the participant walked and cycled for at least 30ā€‰min at moderate intensity. Both variables were dichotomized: 1) no walking at least once per week for >ā€‰30ā€‰min at moderate intensity, vs. 0) walking at least once per week for >ā€‰30ā€‰min at moderate intensity (reference group); and 1) no cycling at least once per week for >ā€‰30ā€‰min at moderate intensity, vs. 0) cycling at least once per week for >ā€‰30ā€‰min at moderate intensity (reference group).

Fruit and vegetable consumption were measured as part of a food frequency questionnaire [30]. Participants reported frequency (days per week) over the past month, for fruit and vegetable consumption separately. They also indicated the portion size (i.e. number of pieces of fruit, and number of serving spoons [=50ā€‰g] of vegetables) on a typical occasion. Based on this, we calculated whether participants did or did not meet recommendations for fruit and vegetable consumption. Those who did not consume two pieces of fruit every day were coded as ā€œnot meeting recommended fruit intakesā€ (and those eating two or more pieces of fruit every day were used as reference group). Those who not consumed 200ā€‰g of vegetables every day were coded as ā€œnot meeting recommended vegetable intakesā€ (with eating >ā€‰200ā€‰g of vegetables every day as reference group).

Water intake was also part of the food frequency questionnaire. Participants reported how many days per week, over the past month, they consumed tap water or spring water. Water consumption less than daily was coded as ā€˜no daily water intakeā€™ (with ā€˜daily water intakeā€™ as reference group). Further, participants reported how many days per week, over the past month, they consumed meat. Meat intake on 5 till 7ā€‰days per week was coded as ā€˜high meat intakeā€™, whereas meat intake on less than 5ā€‰days per week was coded ā€˜no high meat intakeā€™ (reference group).

Body mass index (BMI) was calculated by self-reported height and weight. Participants with a BMI higher than 25 were categorized as overweight (and BMI <ā€‰25 as no overweight, reference group). A BMI higher than 30 was categorized as obesity (and BMI <ā€‰30 as no obesity, reference group). Overweight and obesity were applied as outcomes since these are related to two types of health-behaviour: diet and physical activity.

Confounders

Potential confounders were included in all analyses: sex (male, female), age (in 10-year age groups), country of birth (Netherlands, other), living together with a partner (yes, no), children living in your household (yes, no), employment status (employed, unemployed, retired, or other (e.g. homemaker, student)), fatherā€™s highest attained educational level, and motherā€™s highest attained educational level. The latter two were included as confounders, since parental education level could influence the likelihood that low educated become friends with higher educated, and the likelihood to behave healthily (and the opposite for high educated becoming friends with lower educated). Low educated who were raised by high-educated parents may be more likely to get in touch with higher educated (e.g. directly, via their parents connections, or indirectly, as they have learned to speak the ā€˜appropriateā€™ language to connect with higher educated [31]). Also, low educated with high educated parents may be raised in a sociocultural environment in which healthy behavior was more prevalent.

Statistical analysis

Participants with missing values for educational level or the confounders sex, age, country of birth, living with a partner, children, or employment status, were excluded from the analyses (nĀ =ā€‰110). This resulted in an analytic sample of NĀ =ā€‰2702. To estimate the main effects of bridging social capital and educational level on the outcomes, a logistic regression model was run for each outcome separately, including bridging social capital, educational level, and confounders (results in AdditionalĀ fileĀ 1). To examine whether the association between bridging social capital and health-behavior was modified by educational level, Knol and VanderWeeleā€™s template for reporting analysis investigating effect modifications was used [32]. Their recommendations include running three different logistic regression models for each outcome (referred to as model A, B and C below), in order to provide readers with sufficient information to draw conclusions on the size and statistical significance of the effect modification [32]. Model A is most commonly used in the field of public health for calculating effect modification (or interaction), namely a model with educational level, bridging social capital and confounders, plus a multiplicative interaction term between educational level and bridging social capital. The results from model A are presented in the footnotes of TablesĀ 2-4, and used as an overall test of whether education significantly modifies the association between bridging social capital and outcomes. In model B, odds ratios (ORs) for each combination of educational level and bridging capital were calculated. Thereto, we first composed a combined ā€˜education*capitalā€™-variable, resulting in a variable with six categories (1ā€‰=ā€‰high education with bonding capital (reference group); 2ā€‰=ā€‰high education with bridging capital; 3ā€‰=ā€‰mid-education with bonding capital; 4ā€‰=ā€‰mid education with bridging capital; 5ā€‰=ā€‰low education with bonding capital; 6ā€‰=ā€‰low education with bridging capital). In model B, this combined education*capital-variable and confounders were included. In model C, bridging social capital and confounders were included in a regression model, and results were stratified by educational level. In this way, model C produced ORs for the association between bridging social capital and the outcomes for each educational group separately (low, mid and high educated). All statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS 23.0.

Results

Bridging social capital was more prevalent among low educated (47.7%) than high educated (22.3%). Low educated were older, more often retired or unemployed, and more often lived without children in their household (TableĀ 1). Overall, bridging social capital increased the likelihood of daily smoking, no sports participation, no leisure time cycling, and not meeting recommendations for vegetable intake, compared to bonding social capital (see Additional fileĀ 1). Also, educational inequalities in health-behavior were observed, with low educated and mid-educated more likely to report unhealthy behavior, overweight and obesity compared to high educated (see Additional fileĀ 1).

Table 1 Study sample characteristics of the GLOBE-2014 sample

The results of model A indicated that educational level significantly modified the relation between bridging social capital and outcomes, except for sports participation, leisure time walking and fruit intake (see the p-values in the footnotes of TablesĀ 2-4). The results of models B and C give more insight in the direction and size of the effect modification. Regarding health-behavior (TablesĀ 2-3), bridging social capital mattered mostly for high educated. Among high educated, having bridging social capital increased the likelihood to report daily smoking (OR 2.11, 95% CI 1.37ā€“3.27), no leisure time cycling (OR 1.55, 95% CI 1.17ā€“2.04), not meeting recommendations for vegetable intake (OR 2.09, 95% CI 1.50ā€“2.91), and a high meat intake (OR 1.39, 95% CI 1.05ā€“1.83). On the other hand, regarding overweight and obesity (TableĀ 4), bridging social capital mattered mostly for low educated, but in the opposite direction than among high educated. Among low educated, bridging social capital reduced the likelihood to report overweight (OR 0.73, 95% CI 0.52ā€“1.03) (non-significant) and obesity (OR 0.58, 95% CI 0.38ā€“0.88).

Table 2 Multivariable logistic regression models for modification of the association between bridging social capitala and daily smoking, no sports participation, no leisure time walking, and no leisure time cycling, by educational level
Table 3 Multivariable logistic regression models for modification of the association of bridging social capitala on vegetable intake, fruit intake, water intake, and meat intake, by educational level
Table 4 Multivariable logistic regression models showing ORs for modification of the effect of having bridging social capitala on overweight (BMI >ā€‰25) and obesity (BMIā€‰>ā€‰30), by educational level

Discussion

Main findings

Different relations of bridging social capital with health-behavior, overweight and obesity for low and high educated were found. This partly confirmed our hypothesis that bridging social capital would have beneficial relations with health-behavioral outcomes for low educated, but not for high educated. Indeed, among low educated, bridging social capital reduced the likelihood of overweight and obesity, but did not reduce the likelihood of unhealthy behavior. Among high educated, bridging social capital increased the likelihood of some unhealthy behaviors: smoking, low cycling levels, low vegetable intake, and high meat intake.

Interpretation in light of the literature

Research into bridging social capital suffers from the lack of a standardized measurement approach [13], which makes it difficult to compare our results to previous studies. A recent review found evidence that collective social capital had a stronger positive association with good health for people with a lower socioeconomic status, and may buffer against the negative health effects of a low socioeconomic status [3]. This parallels our findings with respect to overweight and obesity. Two Japanese studies and a British study were somewhat similar to our study in the sense that these measured individual-level bridging social as some degree of heterogeneity in social contacts [12, 33, 34]. Where we found that bridging social capital increased the odds of some types of unhealthy behavior (but had no relations with other), these studies showed that bridging social capital had beneficial relations with several outcomes: a lower odds of physical inactivity [33], inverse associations with depressive mood [12], and positive associations with self-rated health [34]. Differences in the measurement of bridging social capital likely contribute to the contrasting results: whereas we measured bridging social capital referring to (dis) similarities in the educational level of close friends, other studies referred to more general connections (network connections, participants of community activities), and (dis) similarities in ā€˜social characteristicsā€™ in general (e.g. age, sex) [12, 33] or ethnicity and income [34].

Our study extends previous research by investigating whether associations of bridging social capital with health-behavior differ for educational groups. Indeed, bridging social capital increased the likelihood of smoking, low cycling levels, low vegetable intake, and high meat intake among high educated groups, but was unrelated to health-behavior of low educated. An explanation for this may be that healthy behavior, for most people, requires more effort and constraint than unhealthy behavior (e.g. it is often easier to be inactive than sufficiently active, and it is easier to eat few than recommended amounts of vegetables). It seems as if behaving healthily is most ā€˜doableā€™ for high educated with high educated friends (i.e. with bonding social capital), possibly because they experience stronger descriptive norms, and more social support and role-modelling to adopt and maintain healthy behavior, than high educated with low educated friends. High educated with bridging social capital (i.e. with lower educated friends) may perceive less social support, or less strict descriptive norms, making them more likely to adopt unhealthy behavior.

Our results for overweight and obesity seem in contrast to this explanation: bridging social capital did reduce the likelihood to be overweight and obese among low educated, although one could similarly argue for overweight/obesity that the unhealthy option (putting on weight) is easier than the healthy option (weight control). However, there is an important difference between overweight/obesity and health-behavior that could play a role here: there is a strong slimness ideal in Western societies, and such a strong ideal is lacking for most types of health-behavior. Although this slimness ideal may be strongest among high educated, qualitative studies show that weight control and bodily appearance also matter to low educated [35, 36], whereas health-behavior on the other hand is more often rejected or opposed by low educated (e.g. ā€˜healthy food is tasteless, boring and insufficiently satisfyingā€™) [36]. So, as interpretation of our findings for obesity, we surmise that these may show that the healthy norm outweighs the unhealthy norm. For this reason, it may be that low educated with higher educated friends have a reduced risk of obesity (instead of high educated with low educated friends having an increased risk of obesity - what we observed for health behavior).

The previous interpretations assume a causal relation between bridging capital and health-behavior. However, as the cross-sectional design of our study does not give insight in the direction of the observed relations, another plausible explanation may be that ā€˜like attracts likeā€™: low educated with a healthy lifestyle may 'select'Ā friends with a similar healthy lifestyle, who are more likely to be higher educated. Studies from sociology and social psychology show that, before all, the composition and structure of personal networks is affected by the social contexts that a person enters during daily life, such as the work place, school and voluntary associations [37]. Who then, of all people you meet within these contexts, become your friends, is further determined by similarities in age, sex, ethnicity, educational level [38], and personality [39]. The role of peopleā€™s health-behavior in the process of who becomes friends with whom, is less well known.

Since, in the Netherlands, educational inequalities in health-behavior are larger than those by income (and occupation), we expected education-specific bridging links to matter more for health-behavior than income-specific bridging links. We tested this assumption in additional analyses. In our survey, respondents indicated their income level, and how many close friends had a similar income level. In additional analyses, we tested whether income level modified the association between income-specific bridging capital and health-behavior. No meaningful effect modification was found (results in AdditionalĀ fileĀ 2). Apparently, in the Netherlands, educational level is the crucial dimension of bridging social capital in relation to health-behavior inequalities. In other societies, in which race and income level are important dimensions of societal segregation, bridging social capital with respect to race and income level may be just as important.

Strengths & limitations

An important strength of this study is that we conducted a more detailed measurement of bridging social capital compared to previous studies. Also, we tested whether associations of bridging social capital differed for educational groups. Thereby, this study took a differentiated approach to bridging capital, which has often been called for [3, 7, 13], providing more insight in possible underlying pathways between socioeconomic position, bridging social capital and health-behavior. Further, we measured multiple types of health-behavior, as well as overweight and obesity, which allowed us to rigorously test our hypothesis. Lastly, we controlled our cross-sectional analyses for a wide range of potential confounders, including parental education. In this way, we tried to rule out confounding effects as much as possible, by controlling for factors that may affected the respondentsā€™ educational level, bridging social capital and/or health-behavior.

As said, the most important limitation of our study is its cross-sectional design, which does not give insight in the direction of the relations observed. Another limitation is that our data were collected by means of a survey, which may have led to a selective sample of respondents, i.e. those interested in, and capable of, filling in a 16-page survey on health and living conditions. We may have missed the lowest educated, and therefore, the educational inequalities in health-behavior we found are likely an underestimation of the real inequalities. Further, we excluded respondents from the analyses with missing data on one of the confounders (nĀ =ā€‰110). This excluded group was lower educated, older, more often retired, not born in the Netherlands, and more often reported overweight, obesity, and no sports participation. Also for this reason, the health-behavioral inequalities we found are likely an underestimation of the true inequalities. In order to check the representativeness of our sample for the local population, we compared prevalence rates of the health-behaviors in our survey, compared to a survey carried out by the municipal health service in the same target population, which showed that prevalence rates were comparable. However, the comparison data likely suffers from similar caveats as these were also collected by means of a survey. As the lowest socioeconomic groups are likely underrepresented in both datasets, the generalization of our results to these groups is limited.

A final limitation is related to our measure of education-specific bridging capital. We did not ask whether close friends had a higher or lower educational level, but just whether they had a different educational level compared to respondent. This is problematic for the mid-educated, as the answer that most friends have a different educational level does not reveal whether they were lower or higher educated. Future research should consider measuring education-specific bridging social capital with two questions, asking respondents what proportion of their friends has a higher, and what proportion has a lower educational level than themselves.

Conclusion

In contemporary societies oneā€™s social position is increasingly determined by societal successes, including educational achievements. As a result, low and high educated citizens lead increasingly segregated lives, and few bridges between educational groups remain. The results of our study may imply that lower levels of bridging social capital in contemporary societies may lead to more obesity among low educated groups, and healthier behavior among high educated groups, which would then lead to a widening of health inequalities. However, since our study is cross-sectional, future studies should give more insight in the causal relations of bridging social capital with health-behavior, overweight and obesity among different educational groups, and the underlying mechanisms.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to privacy regulations, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Kawachi I, Berkman LF. Social cohesion, social capital, and health. In: Berkman LF, Kawachi I, editors. Social Epidemiology. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. p. 174ā€“90.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  2. Moore S, Kawachi I. Twenty years of social capital and health research: a glossary. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2017;71(5):513ā€“7.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  3. Uphoff EP, Pickett KE, Cabieses B, Small N, Wright J. A systematic review of the relationships between social capital and socioeconomic inequalities in health: a contribution to understanding the psychosocial pathway of health inequalities. Int J Equity Health. 2013;12:54.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  4. Poortinga W. Do health behaviors mediate the association between social capital and health? Prev Med. 2006;43(6):488ā€“93.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  5. Lochner K, Kawachi I, Kennedy BP. Social capital: a guide to its measurement. Health Place. 1999;5(4):259ā€“70.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  6. Bourdieu P. The forms of capital. In: Richardson R, editor. Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education. New York: Greenwood Press; 1986.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  7. Ferlander S. The importance of different forms of social capital for health. Acta Sociologica. 2007;50(2):115ā€“28.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  8. De Clercq B, Vyncke V, Hublet A, Elgar FJ, Ravens-Sieberer U, Currie C, et al. Social capital and social inequality in adolescents' health in 601 Flemish communities: a multilevel analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2012;74(2):202ā€“10.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  9. Moore S, Bockenholt U, Daniel M, Frohlich K, Kestens Y, Richard L. Social capital and core network ties: a validation study of individual-level social capital measures and their association with extra- and intra-neighborhood ties, and self-rated health. Health Place. 2011;17(2):536ā€“44.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  10. Poortinga W. Social relations or social capital? Individual and community health effects of bonding social capital. Soc Sci Med. 2006;63(1):255ā€“70.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  11. Beaudoin CE. Bonding and bridging neighborliness: an individual-level study in the context of health. Soc Sci Med. 2009;68(12):2129ā€“36.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  12. Murayama H, Nishi M, Matsuo E, Nofuji Y, Shimizu Y, Taniguchi Y, et al. Do bonding and bridging social capital affect self-rated health, depressive mood and cognitive decline in older Japanese? A prospective cohort study. Soc Sci Med. 2013;98:247ā€“52.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  13. Villalonga-Olives E, Kawachi I. The measurement of bridging social capital in population health research. Health Place. 2015;36:47ā€“56.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  14. Clark AK. Rethinking the decline in social capital. Am Politics Res. 2015;43(4):569ā€“601.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  15. Bovens M, Dekker P, Tiemeijer W. Gescheiden werelden. SCP en WRR: Den Haag; 2014.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  16. Li YJ, Savage M, Pickles A. Social capital and social exclusion in England and Wales (1972-1999). Brit J Sociol. 2003;54(4):497ā€“526.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  17. Hvd W. Een kloof van alle tijden. Verschillen tussen lager en hoger opgeleiden in werk, cultuur en politiek. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press; 2015.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  18. Oude Groeniger J, van Lenthe FJ, Beenackers MA, Kamphuis CB. Does social distinction contribute to socioeconomic inequalities in diet: the case of 'superfoods' consumption. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14(1):40.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  19. Oude Groeniger J, Kamphuis CB, Mackenbach JP, van Lenthe FJ. Repeatedly measured material and behavioral factors changed the explanation of socioeconomic inequalities in all-cause mortality. J Clin Epidemiol. 2017;91:137ā€“45.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  20. Oude Groeniger J, Kamphuis CBM, Mackenbach JP, Beenackers MA, van Lenthe FJ. Are socio-economic inequalities in diet and physical activity a matter of social distinction? A cross-sectional study. Int J Public Health. 2019;64(7):1037ā€“47.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  21. Kamphuis CB, Van Lenthe FJ, Giskes K, Huisman M, Brug J, Mackenbach JP. Socioeconomic status, environmental and individual factors, and sports participation. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008;40(1):71ā€“81.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  22. Kamphuis CB, Turrell G, Giskes K, Mackenbach JP, van Lenthe FJ. Socioeconomic inequalities in cardiovascular mortality and the role of childhood socioeconomic conditions and adulthood risk factors: a prospective cohort study with 17-years of follow up. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:1045.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  23. Kamphuis CB, Jansen T, Mackenbach JP, van Lenthe FJ. Bourdieu's cultural Capital in Relation to food choices: a systematic review of cultural capital indicators and an empirical proof of concept. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0130695.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  24. van Wijk DC, Groeniger JO, van Lenthe FJ, Kamphuis CB. The role of the built environment in explaining educational inequalities in walking and cycling among adults in the Netherlands. Int J Health Geogr. 2017;16(1):10.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  25. Beenackers MA, Oude Groeniger J, van Lenthe FJ, Kamphuis CBM. The role of financial strain and self-control in explaining health behaviours: the GLOBE study. Eur J Pub Health. 2018;28(4):597ā€“603.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  26. van Lenthe FJ, Kamphuis CB, Beenackers MA, Jansen T, Looman CW, Nusselder WJ, et al. Cohort profile: understanding socioeconomic inequalities in health and health behaviours: the GLOBE study. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(3):721ā€“30.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  27. Mackenbach JP, van de Mheen H, Stronks K. A prospective cohort study investigating the explanation of socio-economic inequalities in health in the Netherlands. Soc Sci Med. 1994;38(2):299ā€“308.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  28. Bovens M, Anchrit W. Diplomademocratie. Over de spanning tussen meritocratie en democratie. Amsterdam: Bert Bakker; 2010.

    Google ScholarĀ 

  29. Wendel-Vos GC, Schuit AJ, Saris WH, Kromhout D. Reproducibility and relative validity of the short questionnaire to assess health-enhancing physical activity. J Clin Epidemiol. 2003;56(12):1163ā€“9.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  30. Bogers RP, Van Assema P, Kester AD, Westerterp KR, Dagnelie PC. Reproducibility, validity, and responsiveness to change of a short questionnaire for measuring fruit and vegetable intake. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;159(9):900ā€“9.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  31. Abel T. Cultural capital and social inequality in health. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008;62(7):e13.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  32. Knol MJ, VanderWeele TJ. Recommendations for presenting analyses of effect modification and interaction. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41(2):514ā€“20.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  33. Ueshima K, Fujiwara T, Takao S, Suzuki E, Iwase T, Doi H, et al. Does social capital promote physical activity? A population-based study in Japan. PLoS One. 2010;5(8):e12135.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  34. Poortinga W. Community resilience and health: the role of bonding, bridging, and linking aspects of social capital. Health Place. 2012;18(2):286ā€“95.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  35. Dammann KW, Smith C. Factors affecting low-income women's food choices and the perceived impact of dietary intake and socioeconomic status on their health and weight. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2009;41(4):242ā€“53.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  36. Gough B, Conner MT. Barriers to healthy eating amongst men: a qualitative analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2006;62(2):387ā€“95.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  37. Mollenhorst G, Volker B, Flap H. Changes in personal relationships: how social contexts affect the emergence and discontinuation of relationships. Soc Networks. 2014;37:65ā€“80.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  38. Kandel DB. Similarity in real-life adolescent friendship pairs. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1978;36(3):306ā€“12.

    ArticleĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

  39. Miller N, Campbell DT, Twedt H, Oconnell EJ. Similarity Contrast and Complementarity in Friendship Choice. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1966;3(1):3 &.

    ArticleĀ  CASĀ  Google ScholarĀ 

Download references

Acknowledgements

CBMK is grateful to the Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study in the Humanities and Social Sciences (NIAS) for providing a stimulating and inspiring work environment. Furthermore, the authors are thankful to the International Society of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity (ISBNPA) for the opportunity to present and discuss this study during an oral session at the annual meeting of ISBNPA in June, 2019 (https://www.isbnpa.org/files/articles/2019/06/25/122/attachments/5d124e1face47.pdf).

Funding

The GLOBE study was supported by a grant from the Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development (grant number 200500005). CBMK was supported by a Lā€™Oreal Unesco For Women in Science fellowship and a Grant from The Netherlands Institute for Advanced Study in the Humanities and Social Sciences (NIAS). JOG, MAB and FJvL were supported by the European Commission HORIZON 2020 research and innovation action (667661). MPP was supported by the Innovational Research Incentives Scheme (#451ā€“16-029), financed by The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). The funders had no role in the study design or the analysis and interpretation of the data. All authors and their institutions reserve intellectual freedom from the funders.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CBMK, JOG, MAB and FJvL contributed to the design of the study and collection of data. CBMK analyzed the data and wrote the first draft. CBMK, JOG, MPP, MAB and FJvL contributed to the interpretation of the results, critically revised draft versions of the manuscript, and approved the final version to be published.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carlijn B. M. Kamphuis.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Under the Dutch law for medical-scientific research (WMO; https://wetten.overheid.nl/BWBR0009408/2019-04-02), no formal approval of the medical ethics committee of the Erasmus University Medical Centre was required for this type of non-invasive survey research. The participants were not asked to actively sign an informed consent form, but the background and objectives of the study were communicated on the first page of the questionnaire and in the accompanying invitation letter. Completion of the questionnaire was voluntary. The use of personal data in the GLOBE study is in compliance with the Dutch Personal Data Protection Act and the Municipal Database Act, and has been registered with the Dutch Data Protection Authority (number 1248943).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisherā€™s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1.

Main effects of educational level and education-specific bridging social capital, adjusted for confounders

Additional file 2.

Models for associations between income-specific bridging social capital and health-behavior, modified by income level

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kamphuis, C.B.M., Oude Groeniger, J., Poelman, M.P. et al. How does bridging social capital relate to health-behavior, overweight and obesity among low and high educated groups? A cross-sectional analysis of GLOBE-2014. BMC Public Health 19, 1635 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-019-8007-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12889-019-8007-3

Keywords