Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Cognitive decline and mortality among community-dwelling Chinese older people

Abstract

Background

Whether cognitive decline is related to a higher risk of death independent of the initial cognitive function is inconclusive. Evidence of the association between cognitive decline and mortality among Chinese older people is limited. We aimed to examine whether cognitive decline, assessed by the rate of decrease in the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score, was associated with mortality independent of initial cognitive function (baseline MMSE score) among Chinese older people.

Methods

We established two successive and non-overlapping cohorts of older adults nested within the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS), an ongoing, open, community-based cohort survey conducted every 2–3 years. Cognitive function was measured using the Chinese version of the MMSE. A total of 11,732 older adults who completed two consecutive cognitive function examinations were included and followed for 3 years. A Cox proportional hazards model was used to examine the association of cognitive decline with mortality after adjusting for sociodemographic characteristics, health behaviours, comorbidities and initial cognitive function.

Results

The mean age was 82.5 years old, and 44.9% (5264/11732) of participants were men. After adjusting for baseline MMSE scores and other covariates, the rate of change in MMSE scores over 3 years was monotonically and positively associated with subsequent 3-year mortality. Compared to those with stable cognitive function, participants with rapid cognitive decline (decline faster than average, a reduction of MMSE scores > 1.62 points/year) had a 75% higher risk of death (hazard ratio = 1.75, 95% confidence interval 1.57–1.95). The association between cognitive decline and mortality was stronger among relatively younger Chinese older people (aged 65–79 years versus 80 years and over) and those with normal cognitive function at baseline (MMSE scores ≥ 24 versus < 24 points), respectively, but did not differ by cohort and sex.

Conclusion

Faster cognitive decline was associated with higher mortality independent of initial cognitive function, especially among those aged 65–79 years and those with normal cognitive function at baseline. The association was consistent across two successive cohorts. Our findings indicate the practical significance of monitoring cognitive change in older adults.

Peer Review reports

Background

With the global population ageing, dementia is a growing public health concern [1]. Preservation of cognitive function is a central component of successful ageing [2]. Numerous studies have shown that lower cognitive function is associated with an increased risk of death [3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10].

It was suggested that cognitive function generally declines with age among older adults [11], and the rate of cognitive decline (i.e. reduction in cognitive function) is related to initial cognitive function [12]. However, it is inconclusive whether cognitive decline is independently associated with mortality. Most previous studies found that faster cognitive decline was associated with an increased risk of death after adjusting for initial cognitive function [5, 13,14,15,16,17,18]. However, Bruce et al. reported that the association between cognitive decline and mortality attenuated to null when adjusted for baseline cognitive function [19]. Bosworth et al. found that time to death was not highly related to changes in intellectual performance [20]. Hassing et al. found that the decedents did not decline more rapidly in all 11 cognitive measurements across a 4-year period than the survivors [8]. Discrepancies may partly be explained by relatively small sample sizes [8, 14, 16,17,18, 20] or differences in age distributions in previous studies [15, 19, 20]. China has the largest ageing population and is one of the fastest ageing countries in the world [21]. To our knowledge, the association between cognitive decline and mortality has not been thoroughly evaluated among a national cohort of Chinese older people.

In addition, it has been reported that the cognitive function of older adults might change at different times owing to changes in education, comorbidities and social progress [22, 23]. The strength of the association between cognitive function and mortality might accordingly change over time [22, 24]. However, whether the association of cognitive decline with mortality differs at different times is unknown.

The Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS) includes a dynamic, prospective and national cohort of community-dwelling Chinese older people. In this study, we investigated whether cognitive function change over 3 years was associated with subsequent 3-year mortality after adjusting for baseline cognitive function and determined whether the association differed in two large, successive and non-overlapping cohorts within the CLHLS.

Methods

Study design and participants

The CLHLS is an ongoing, prospective cohort study of community-dwelling Chinese older people. Details can be found elsewhere [25,26,27]. Briefly, the CLHLS is a nationwide survey covering approximately 85% of China’s population and provides representative data to investigate determinants of longevity. It began in 1998, and examinations are carried out every 2–3 years. To reduce the attrition due to death and loss to follow-up, new participants are enrolled during the follow-up. The surveys are administered in participants’ homes by trained interviewers with a structured questionnaire. Proxy respondents, usually a spouse or other close family members, are interviewed when the participants are unable to answer questions, but questions regarding cognitive function and mood are answered by participants themselves.

The current study was based on two successive and non-overlapping cohorts (the 2002 and 2008 cohorts) within the CLHLS. Three surveys were performed in each cohort (2002, 2005 and 2008 in the 2002 cohort and 2008, 2011 and 2014 in the 2008 cohort). We included 13,601 participants who were free of dementia at baseline (the first survey) and completed cognitive function assessments during both the first and second surveys. We then excluded 1869 participants who were lost to follow-up before the third survey. A total of 11,732 participants were included as the study population, with 6626 from the 2002 cohort and 5106 from the 2008 cohort (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow chart of the study population

The CLHLS study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Peking University (IRB00001052-13074), and all participants or their proxy respondents provided written informed consent.

Outcome

The participants’ survival status and date of death were collected through interviews with close family members during the third survey in each cohort.

Exposure

Cognitive function was measured by the Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) during each survey. The validity and reliability of the Chinese MMSE has been verified [9, 25, 26]. Based on the literature [28], we treated responses of “unable to answer” as “wrong”. The MMSE score ranged from 0 to 30, and a higher score indicated better cognitive function.

Cognitive decline was assessed using the rate of change in MMSE score, which was calculated as the difference between the MMSE score at baseline and the second survey divided by the number of years between the two examinations ((MMSE score at baseline − MMSE score at the second survey)/the interval between two examinations, years). We then categorized the rate of change in the MMSE score into four groups: cognitive improvement (a rate of change in MMSE score less than zero), stable cognitive function (a rate of change in MMSE score equal to zero), slow decline (a rate of change in MMSE scores greater than zero but equal to or less than the median of those showing decline [rate of change in MMSE score greater than zero]) and rapid decline (a rate of change in MMSE score greater than the median of those showing decline).

Covariates

We adjusted sociodemographic characteristics, health behaviours and health status at baseline in the model. These covariates were selected a priori as potential confounders based on the literature [3, 9, 10, 15, 18, 24, 29]. The sociodemographic characteristics were age (continuous), sex, education (no schooling/some schooling), main occupation (white-collar/other), residence (urban/rural), marital status (married/other) and co-residence with family members (yes/no). Health behaviours were smoking status (never/past/current), current alcohol intake (yes/no) and regular exercise at present (yes/no). Health status was common chronic diseases (self-reported hypertension, diabetes, heart disease, cerebrovascular disease and respiratory disease), body mass index (BMI, underweight (< 18.5)/normal (18.5–23.9)/overweight (24–27.9)/obese (≥ 28) [30]) and symptoms of psychological distress (measured based on loneliness, usefulness and fearfulness [31], categorized into four groups according to the quartile of the total score). In addition, a cohort identifier was established to indicate which cohort the participants were from.

Statistical analyses

Cox proportional hazards models were constructed to determine the association of cognitive decline with mortality. The follow-up started from the second survey and ended on the date of death or the end of the study (the third survey), whichever was earlier. The endpoint events were defined as death or the end of the study, whichever came first. A small percentage of the data for most covariates were missing (< 3%), with the exception of symptoms of psychological distress (7.8%). For these missing values, we used missing indicators. We ascertained that the proportional hazard assumption had not been violated.

Cognitive decline was modelled as both a continuous variable (rate of change in MMSE score) and a categorical variable (improvement, stable, slow decline and rapid decline). Data are reported as adjusted hazard ratios (HRs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs). The adjustment was accomplished via three stages: (1) we adjusted for age and sex; (2) we added the cohort identifier, education, occupation, type of residence, marital status, co-residence, smoking, alcohol intake, and exercising, hypertension, diabetes, heart disease, cerebrovascular disease, respiratory disease, BMI and symptoms of psychological distress; and (3) we additionally adjusted for baseline MMSE score (the fully adjusted model).

To examine the shape of the association between the rate of change in MMSE score and mortality, we conducted a restricted cubic spline analysis. We examined whether the association of cognitive decline with mortality differed by cohort, sex, age (65–79 versus ≥ 80 years old) and baseline cognitive function (MMSE score ≥ 24 versus < 24 points) by separately adding an interaction term to the fully adjusted model.

To address the issue of loss to follow-up, we conducted a sensitivity analysis by considering the losses censored at the end of the study. We also performed a sensitivity analysis by excluding deaths that occurred in the first year of the follow-up to account for the possibility that a pre-mortality drop in cognitive function and/or disease status in the last year of life could have influenced our results.

A two-tailed P value < 0.05 was used to determine statistical significance. All analyses were performed with SPSS 20.0 for Windows (IBM SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) except for the restricted cubic spline analysis, which was performed using R version 3.4.2 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing).

Results

Participant characteristics

The mean (standard deviation) age was 82.5 (11.3) years old at baseline, and 44.9% of participants (5264/11732) were men. Approximately 60% of the individuals were illiterate. There were 29.2% participants with cognitive impairment at baseline and 37.4% with cognitive impairment at the second survey. The median (25th, 75th percentile) of the rate of change in MMSE score was 0.0 (− 1.6, 0.6) points per year. At a median of 2.8 years of follow-up, 4253 (36.3%) deaths were identified. The characteristics of participants by cohort are shown in Table 1.

Table 1 Characteristics (data are expressed as counts (percentages) except when specified otherwise) of 11,732 community-dwelling Chinese older people at baseline by cohort

Association of cognitive decline with mortality

Table 2 shows the association of cognitive decline with mortality. A multivariable-adjusted model showed that participants with a one more point reduction in MMSE score per year had an 11% higher risk of death (HR = 1.11, 95%CI 1.10–1.12). We observed a positive and monotonic association between the rate of change in MMSE score and mortality: the faster the cognitive decline, the higher the risk of death (P for trend < 0.0001, Fig. 2). Compared to those with stable cognitive function, participants with rapid cognitive decline had 75% higher mortality (HR = 1.75, 95%CI 1.57–1.95). Those with slow cognitive decline and cognitive improvement had a similar risk of death as those with stable cognitive function (Table 2 and Fig. 3).

Table 2 The association of 3-year cognitive decline with subsequent 3-year mortality among 11,732 community-dwelling Chinese older people
Fig. 2
figure 2

Association of the rate of change in Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score with mortality (hazard ratios are indicated by solid lines and 95% confidence intervals by shaded areas, reference point is the lowest value of rate of change in MMSE score, with knots placed at 5th, 35th, 65th and 95th percentiles), after adjusting for baseline cognitive function, age, sex, cohort, education, occupation, type of residence, marital status, co-residence, smoking, alcohol intake, exercise, hypertension, diabetes, heart disease, cerebrovascular disease, respiratory disease, body mass index and psychological distress

Fig. 3
figure 3

Survival probabilities by the level of cognitive decline after adjusting for baseline cognitive function, age, sex, cohort, education, occupation, type of residence, marital status, co-residence, smoking, alcohol intake, exercise, hypertension, diabetes, heart disease, cerebrovascular disease, respiratory disease, body mass index and psychological distress

Subgroup analysis

The association between the rate of change in MMSE score and mortality among those aged 65–79 years was stronger than that among those aged 80 years and over and among those with baseline MMSE scores ≥ 24 points compared to those with MMSE scores < 24 points but did not differ by cohort and sex (Fig. 4). The association of categorical cognitive decline with mortality did not differ by cohort, sex, age or baseline cognitive function, although it appeared stronger in the 2002 cohort, among women, among relatively younger Chinese older people and among those with baseline MMSE scores ≥ 24 points (Fig. 5).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Hazard ratios and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the association between the rate of change in the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score and mortality by cohort, sex, age and baseline cognitive function, adjusting for baseline cognitive function, age, sex, cohort, education, occupation, type of residence, marital status, co-residence, smoking, alcohol intake, exercise, hypertension, diabetes, heart disease, cerebrovascular disease, respiratory disease, body mass index and psychological distress

Fig. 5
figure 5

Hazard ratios and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for the association between cognitive decline and mortality, with stable cognitive function as a reference group, by cohort, sex, age and baseline cognitive function, adjusting for baseline cognitive function, age, sex, cohort, education, occupation, type of residence, marital status, co-residence, smoking, alcohol intake, exercise, hypertension, diabetes, heart disease, cerebrovascular disease, respiratory disease, body mass index and psychological distress

Sensitivity analysis

Our findings remained robust when we treated participants lost to follow-up as censored at the third survey or excluded participants who died in the first year of follow-up (Table 3).

Table 3 Sensitivity analyses for the association of 3-year cognitive decline with subsequent 3-year mortality

Discussion

In this large prospective and community-based study, after adjusting for baseline cognitive function and other confounders, we found that the rate of change in an individual’s MMSE score over 3 years was monotonically and positively associated with mortality. Participants with rapid cognitive decline had higher mortality during the subsequent 3 years. The association between cognitive decline and mortality was stronger among relatively younger Chinese older people (aged 65–79 versus ≥ 80+ years) and those with normal cognitive function at baseline (MMSE score ≥ 24 versus < 24 points) but did not differ by cohort and sex.

In line with most previous studies [5, 13,14,15,16,17,18], the findings from our data suggest that cognitive decline is related to mortality independent of initial cognitive function. Our results suggest that rapid decline in cognitive function may be a sign of the approach of life’s end. Considering the effect of cognitive training interventions on cognitive function [32], it is possible that maintaining cognitive function would be beneficial for survival in older adults, even for those already experiencing lower cognitive function. It has been reported that vascular risk factors, vascular diseases and psychological well-being are associated with cognitive decline [33,34,35,36]. The associations of cognitive decline with mortality in our study did not substantially change after adding comorbid diseases to the models (Table 2), which is similar to previous studies [14,15,16]. This evidence suggests that the association between cognitive decline and mortality might not be attributed to underlying diseases. Whether cognitive decline is a marker of biological ageing or plays a causative role in longevity or whether some possible factors simultaneously contribute to both cognitive decline and mortality deserves further investigation.

The association between cognitive decline and mortality was not substantially different in the 2008 cohort compared to the 2002 cohort. Similar to previous studies [7, 9], we found no evidence for a differing cognitive decline-mortality association between men and women. Consistent with previous studies [5, 16, 19], we observed that age influenced the association of cognitive decline with mortality. That is, relatively younger Chinese older people (65–79 versus 80+ years) had a higher risk of death related to cognitive decline. It has been suggested that cognitive decline among relatively younger older people may reflect underlying brain disease or some potential processes that are associated with increased risk of death [5, 14]. Moreover, the smaller association of cognitive decline and mortality among individuals aged 80 years and over may be partially explained by survival bias. We also found that those with baseline MMSE scores ≥ 24 points had higher mortality related to cognitive decline compared to those with MMSE scores < 24 points, which suggests that those with normal cognitive function would probably benefit more from cognitive function preservation than those already cognitively impaired. In addition, owing to the lower sensitivity of MMSE for mild impairment [37], there might be some overlap between the group with MMSE scores ≥ 24 points and those with MMSE scores < 24 points. Therefore, the difference in the association of cognitive decline and mortality between individuals with normal cognitive function and those with cognitive impairment at baseline might be attenuated in our study. Overall, our findings indicate that preventing cognitive decline as early as possible would potentially extend a patient’s lifespan.

One of our study’s limitations was that only participants who completed two cognitive examinations (for assessing cognitive decline) were included in the present analyses. Those who only completed the baseline survey had lower initial cognitive function (data not shown). The risk of death related to cognitive decline was slightly weaker among those with cognitive impairment; therefore, the association between cognitive decline and mortality in our study sample might be slightly higher than that in the whole population. In addition, practice effects have been found to persist over periods up to 7 years [38]. The average interval of two MMSE examinations was 3 years in the current study, and the practice effects would have underestimated decline and therefore its impact. Moreover, it was reported that the rate of terminal cognitive decline can be highly variable but is likely to accelerate approximately 3 years before death [39]. Our findings have limited generalizability to the association of long-term cognitive decline with mortality. Furthermore, some potential covariates, either unmeasured (e.g. medical treatment, inflammation, diet) [4, 7] or unknown, might confound the association between cognitive decline and mortality. On the other hand, we accounted for a set of common confounders, and the results were robust. Residual confounding was another potential concern.

The strengths of this study included the large sample size, its prospective nature and a community-based representative sample. To our knowledge, this is the first study that investigated the relationship between cognitive decline and mortality in a national cohort of Chinese older people. The current study included a large population of Chinese older adults aged 80 years and over, allowing a robust assessment of the association between cognitive decline and mortality among this group. Additionally, we used the same protocol for sampling and data collection in the two cohorts; therefore, they were comparable to some extent (Table 1).

Given that cognitive function in older adults may be modifiable [40], our findings have potential public health significance. The results indicate the practical significance of the detection of cognitive decline with the MMSE or other brief instruments in community settings, especially for relatively young Chinese older people and those with normal cognitive function.

Conclusions

Cognitive decline, independent of initial cognitive function, was associated with higher mortality. Individuals aged 65–79 years and those with normal cognitive function may have a higher risk of death related to cognitive decline. The association was strong and consistent across two large and successive cohorts. This study provides evidence for monitoring cognitive change in older adults.

Abbreviations

BMI:

Body mass index

CI:

Confidence interval

CLHLS:

Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey

HR:

Hazard ratio

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

References

  1. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet. 2017;390:2673–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Rowe JW, Kahn RL. Successful aging. Gerontologist. 1997;37:433–40.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Liu IY, LaCroix AZ, White LR, Kittner SJ, Wolf PA. Cognitive impairment and mortality: a study of possible confounders. Am J Epidemiol. 1990;132:136–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Gale CR, Martyn CN, Cooper C. Cognitive impairment and mortality in a cohort of elderly people. BMJ. 1996;312:608–11.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Bassuk SS, Wypij D, Berkman LF. Cognitive impairment and mortality in the community-dwelling elderly. Am J Epidemiol. 2000;151:676–88.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Perna L, Wahl HW, Mons U, Saum KU, Holleczek B, Brenner H. Cognitive impairment, all-cause and cause-specific mortality among non-demented older adults. Age Ageing. 2015;44:445–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Batty GD, Deary IJ, Zaninotto P. Association of cognitive function with cause-specific mortality in middle and older age: follow-up of participants in the English longitudinal study of ageing. Am J Epidemiol. 2016;183:183–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Hassing LB, Johansson B, Berg S, Nilsson SE, Pedersen NL, Hofer SM, et al. Terminal decline and markers of cerebro- and cardiovascular disease findings from a longitudinal study of the oldest old. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57:268–P76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. An R, Liu GG. Cognitive impairment and mortality among the oldest-old Chinese. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2016;31:1345–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Gao S, Jin Y, Unverzagt FW, Cheng Y, Su L, Wang C, et al. Cognitive function, body mass index and mortality in a rural elderly Chinese cohort. Arch Public Health. 2014;72:9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Lipnicki DM, Crawford JD, Dutta R, Thalamuthu A, Kochan NA, Andrews G, et al. Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: a collaborative cohort study. PLoS Med. 2017;14:e1002261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Wilson RS, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Albert MS, Evans DA. Change in cognitive function in older persons from a community population: relation to age and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:1274–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Connors MH, Sachdev PS, Kochan NA, Xu J, Draper B, Brodaty H. Cognition and mortality in older people: the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. Age Ageing. 2015;44:1049–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Lavery LL, Dodge HH, Snitz B, Ganguli M. Cognitive decline and mortality in a community-based cohort: the Monongahela Valley Independent Elders Survey. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2009;57:94–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Shipley BA, Der G, Taylor MD, Deary IJ. Association between mortality and cognitive change over 7 years in a large representative sample of UK residents. Psychosom Med. 2007;69:640–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Schupf N, Tang MX, Albert SM, Costa R, Andrews H, Lee JH, et al. Decline in cognitive and functional skills increases mortality risk in nondemented elderly. Neurology. 2005;65:1218–26.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Deeg DJ, Hofman A, van Zonneveld RJ. The association between change in cognitive function and longevity in Dutch elderly. Am J Epidemiol. 1990;132:973–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. van Gelder BM, Tijhuis MA, Kalmijn S, Giampaoli S, Kromhout D. Decline in cognitive functioning is associated with a higher mortality risk. Neuroepidemiology. 2007;28:93–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Bruce ML, Hoff RA, Jacobs SC, Leaf PJ. The effects of cognitive impairment on 9-year mortality in a community sample. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1995;50:P289–96.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Bosworth HB, Schaie KW, Willis SL, Siegler IC. Age and distance to death in the Seattle Longitudinal Study. Res Aging. 1999;21:723–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. United Nations, World Population Ageing 2017 - Highlights (ST/ESA/SER.A/397). Department of Economic and Socal Affairs, Population Division. 2017.

  22. Sacuiu S, Gustafson D, Sjogren M, Guo X, Ostling S, Johansson B, et al. Secular changes in cognitive predictors of dementia and mortality in 70-year-olds. Neurology. 2010;75:779–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Dodge HH, Zhu J, Lee CW, Chang CC, Ganguli M. Cohort effects in age-associated cognitive trajectories. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2014;69:687–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Langa KM, Larson EB, Karlawish JH, Cutler DM, Kabeto MU, Kim SY, et al. Trends in the prevalence and mortality of cognitive impairment in the United States: is there evidence of a compression of cognitive morbidity? Alzheimers Dement. 2008;4:134–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Lv YB, Gao X, Yin ZX, Chen HS, Luo JS, Brasher MS, et al. Revisiting the association of blood pressure with mortality in oldest old people in China: community based, longitudinal prospective study. BMJ. 2018;361:k2158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Zeng Y, Feng Q, Hesketh T, Christensen K, Vaupel JW. Survival, disabilities in activities of daily living, and physical and cognitive functioning among the oldest-old in China: a cohort study. Lancet. 2017;389:1619–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Zeng Y. Introduction to the Chinese Longitudinal Healthy Longevity Survey (CLHLS). In: Zeng Y, Poston Jr DL, Vlosky DA, Gu D, editors. Healthy longevity in China: demographic, socioeconomic, and psychological dimensions. Dordrecht: Springer Publisher; 2008. p. 23–38.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Zhang Z, Gu D, Hayward MD. Childhood nutritional deprivation and cognitive impairment among older Chinese people. Soc Sci Med. 2010;71:941–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Dewey ME, Saz P. Dementia, cognitive impairment and mortality in persons aged 65 and over living in the community: a systematic review of the literature. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2001;16:751–61.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Wang J, Taylor AW, Zhang T, Appleton S, Shi Z. Association between body mass index and all-cause mortality among oldest old Chinese. J Nutr Health Aging. 2018;22:262–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Gu D, Dupre ME, Sautter J, Zhu H, Liu Y, Yi Z. Frailty and mortality among Chinese at advanced ages. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64:279–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA. 2002;288:2271–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. METACOHORTS Consortium. METACOHORTS for the study of vascular disease and its contribution to cognitive decline and neurodegeneration: an initiative of the Joint Programme for Neurodegenerative Disease Research. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12:1235–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Li J, Wang YJ, Zhang M, Xu ZQ, Gao CY, Fang CQ, et al. Vascular risk factors promote conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer disease. Neurol. 2011;76:1485–91.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Rabin JS, Schultz AP, Hedden T, Viswanathan A, Marshall GA, Kilpatrick E, et al. Interactive associations of vascular risk and beta-amyloid burden with cognitive decline in clinically normal elderly individuals: findings from the Harvard Aging Brain Study. JAMA Neurol. 2018;75:1124–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. John A, Patel U, Rusted J, Richards M, Gaysina D. Affective problems and decline in cognitive state in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2018;49:353–65.

  37. Janssen J, Koekkoek PS, Moll van Charante EP, Jaap Kappelle L, Biessels GJ, Rutten G. How to choose the most appropriate cognitive test to evaluate cognitive complaints in primary care. BMC Fam Pract. 2017;18:101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Rabbitt P, Diggle P, Smith D, Holland F, Mc IL. Identifying and separating the effects of practice and of cognitive ageing during a large longitudinal study of elderly community residents. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:532–43.39.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Wilson RS, Beckett LA, Bienias JL, Evans DA, Bennett DA. Terminal decline in cognitive function. Neurol. 2003;60:1782–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Baumgart M, Snyder HM, Carrillo MC, Fazio S, Kim H, Johns H. Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11:718–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the staff and the participants of the CLHLS study. We thank Donald Halstead and Lori Chibnik of Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health for their comments on the manuscript. We are also grateful for the comments provided by Zhijiang Wang and Jing Wang of the Peking University Institute of Mental Health. The support provided by the China Scholarship Council during a visit of Xiaozhen Lv to Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health is acknowledged.

Funding

This work was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Number 71490732) and the National Key Research and Development Project, Ministry of Science and Technology of China (2017YFC1311100). The funding agency had no role in the design of the study; in collection, analysis or interpretation of the data; in writing or approving the manuscript; or in the decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Availability of data and materials

The CLHLS questionnaires are available at https://sites.duke.edu/centerforaging/programs/chinese-longitudinal-healthy-longevity-survey-clhls/survey-documentation/questionnaires/. The full datasets used in this analysis are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

XL contributed to the study design, data analysis and interpretation and drafted the manuscript. WL and YM contributed to the data analysis and interpretation and critical revision of the manuscript. HC and YZ contributed to the administration of the survey and critical revision of the manuscript. XY, AH and HW contributed to the design of this study, interpretation of data and critical revision of the manuscript. HW has the primary responsibility for the final content. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Huali Wang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The CLHLS study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Peking University (IRB00001052-13074), and all participants or their proxy respondents provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lv, X., Li, W., Ma, Y. et al. Cognitive decline and mortality among community-dwelling Chinese older people. BMC Med 17, 63 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12916-019-1295-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12916-019-1295-8

Keywords