Skip to main content

The peacefulness gene promotes aggression in Drosophila

Abstract

Natural aggressiveness is commonly observed in all animal species, and is displayed frequently when animals compete for food, territory and mating. Aggression is an innate behaviour, and is influenced by both environmental and genetic factors. However, the genetics of aggression remains largely unclear. In this study, we identify the peacefulness (pfs) gene as a novel player in the control of male-male aggression in Drosophila. Mutations in pfs decreased intermale aggressiveness, but did not affect locomotor activity, olfactory avoidance response and sexual behaviours. pfs encodes for the evolutionarily conserved molybdenum cofactor (MoCo) synthesis 1 protein (Mocs1), which catalyzes the first step in the MoCo biosynthesis pathway. Neuronal-specific knockdown of pfs decreased aggressiveness. By contrast, overexpression of pfs greatly increased aggressiveness. Knocking down Cinnamon (Cin) catalyzing the final step in the MoCo synthesis pathway, caused a pfs-like aggression phenotype. In humans, inhibition of MoCo-dependent enzymes displays anti-aggressive effects. Thus, the control of aggression by Pfs-dependent MoCo pathways may be conserved throughout evolution.

Introduction

All animal species display aggression, an innate behaviour that is evolutionarily conserved. While natural aggressiveness is important for survival and reproduction, abnormal aggressiveness can cause the waste of energy, severe injuries, wars and destruction. Accumulated evidence supports that aggression is influenced by both environmental and genetic factors [1,2,3]. For instance, social experience has been shown to play an important role in modulating the levels of aggressiveness in humans as well as animal models [3,4,5,6]. Recent studies also begin to reveal genetic factors underlying heritable differences in aggressiveness [7,8,9].

Drosophila melanogaster is an excellent model system for studying neural and genetic basis of aggression. Aggressive behaviours in Drosophila were firstly reported by Alfred Sturtevant [10], and later studied in greater details by the groups of Jacobs [11], Hoffmann [12] and Kravitz [13]. Like that in mammals [14], manipulating the levels of neurotransmitters such as serotonin, dopamine and octopamine modulates aggressiveness in Drosophila [15,16,17,18]. Quantitative-trait linkage analyses by Mackay and coworkers suggest that a number of candidate genes may be associated with aggressive behaviours in Drosophila, many of which have homologs in mammals [19]. A recent study also shows that the fly homolog of the gene encoding for neuropeptide Tachykinin/Substance P associated with aggressive behaviors in mammals [20], is also required for aggression in Drosophila [21]. These studies support the evolutionarily conservation of certain genetic mechanisms underlying the control of aggression.

In a search for genetic factors involved in the control of fly aggression, we identify the peacefulness (pfs) gene as a novel and important player required for male-male aggression. Pfs encodes for molybdenum cofactor (MoCo) synthesis 1 protein, an evolutionarily conserved enzyme that catalyzes the first step in the MoCo biosynthesis pathway [22]. MoCo is absolutely required for the activity of molybdoenzymes such as sulphite oxidase, xanthine oxidase and aldehyde oxidase [23]. Interestingly, inhibition of MoCo-dependent xanthine oxidase has been shown to display anti-aggressive effects in humans [24,25,26,27].

In this report, we describe our study on the identification and characterization of pfs in the control of fly aggression. By taking a combination of behavioural analyses, transgene rescue, cell-type-specific knockdown and overexpression, we investigate the requirements and functions of Pfs in regulating fly aggressiveness.

Results

P-element insertion d03517 decreased intermale aggressiveness

In a search for novel genetic factors involved in the control of aggression, we found that mutants homozygous for P-element insertion P{XP}d03517 (d03517) showed a significant decrease in the levels of intermale aggressiveness (Fig. 1a and b). In each experiment, two male flies of same age (isolated for 5–7 days after eclosion) with similar size were introduced into a small chamber, and their behaviours were immediately videotaped for 15-min period. The movies were subsequently analyzed with an automated analysis system CADABRA [28]. Compared to Canton-S (CS) wild-type flies, d03517 mutant flies displayed much fewer lunges and wing threats (Fig. 1a and b, Additional file 1: Movie 1B). We did not observe tussle, a rare and more intense fighting behaviour, in wild-type (n = 27 pairs) or d03517 mutant flies (n = 27 pairs) within 15-min period.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The P-element insertion d03517 decreased male-male aggressiveness. a and b Intermale aggressive behaviors for 15-min period in wild-type and d03517 mutant flies. Prior to behavioral assays, d03517 flies were backcrossed with CS wild-type flies for four generations and thus were in w+ background. a Number of lunges. b Number of wing threats. Pairs of flies tested: wt, n = 27; d03517/ d03517, n = 27. Mann-Whitney U test, ***P < 0.001. c Aggressive behaviours for 15-min period were examined when a wild-type male fly was paired with a d03517 mutant male fly. Pairs of flies tested: n = 46. Mann-Whitney U test, **P < 0.01. d Dominance index for successful occupancy of food patch by a wild-type or a d03517 mutant male fly after 10-min period. Pairs of flies tested: n = 46. Student’s t-test, **P < 0.01. Error bars represent SEM

Above phenotypes raise at least two possibilities. For instance, d03517 mutant flies were incapable of initiating aggression. Alternatively or additionally, they may be incapable of evoking aggression by other flies. To distinguish among these possibilities, we paired a d03517 mutant male fly with a CS wild-type male fly and examined their behaviours. We found that wild-type flies displayed much higher levels of aggressiveness than d03517 mutants (Fig. 1c). This result suggests that d03517 insertion interferes with the internal state required for aggression, but does not affect the ability to evoke aggression by wild-type flies (Fig. 1c).

We then tested if higher aggressiveness in wild-type flies gives them competitive advantage over d03517 mutants to defending their territory. In such experiments, a wild-type male fly and a d03517 mutant male fly were placed into a small chamber and allowed to compete for food patch in the center. The frequency for successful occupancy of the food patch was quantitated (see Materials and methods). We found that wild-type flies were much more successful in occupying and defending the food patch than d03517 mutant flies (Fig. 1d. Additional file 2: Movie 2).

In summary, d03517 insertion caused a significant decrease in male fly aggressiveness, which may at least partially account for their disadvantage in defending territory when paired with wild-type male flies.

d03517 insertion did not affect locomotor activity

To test if the decrease in aggressiveness in d03517 mutants was due to some general defects in physical capabilities, we examined fly locomotion over 15-min period. A wild-type male fly was paired with a d03517 mutant male fly, and their movements were videotaped and analyzed. No significant difference in the patterns or total distance of movements was observed between wild-type and d03517 mutant male flies (Additional file 3: Figure S1A).

We also performed climbing test to examine potential effects of d03517 insertion on fly motor functions. No significant difference in climbing ability was observed between wild-type and d03517 mutant male flies (Additional file 3: Figure S1B). These results argue against that the observed decrease in aggressiveness was caused by defective physical capabilities.

d03517 insertion did not affect olfactory avoidance response

Olfactory sensation plays important roles in regulating fly behaviours, such as aggression and courtship [29,30,31]. This raises the possibility that the decrease in aggressiveness of d03517 mutants was caused by defective olfactory sensation. To test this, we performed experiments to examine the response of wild-type and d03517 mutant flies to benzaldehyde, a strong odorant repellent (Additional file 4: Figure S2A). We found that like wild-type flies, d03517 mutant flies could effectively detect and avoid the area with benzaldehyde (Additional file 4: Figure S2B).

d03517 insertion did not affect sexual behaviours

When encountering other flies, a male fly has to make certain mutually exclusive decisions, such as fighting or courtship. The observed decrease in intermale aggression of d03517 mutant male flies may reflect a specific failure of initiating and/or executing fighting, or reflect a switch in decision making due to altered sexual orientation.

To distinguish among these possibilities, we assessed the ability of d03517 mutant male flies to distinguish between males and females. A decapitated virgin female and a decapitated male were placed on different areas in a small chamber. A wild-type or a d03517 mutant male fly was then introduced into the chamber. Like wild-type male flies (Fig. 2a), d03517 mutant males selected the decapitated virgin female over the decapitated male for showing courtship behaviours (Fig. 2a, Additional file 5: Movie 3). This result indicates that d03517 mutant male flies were still able to recognize sexual identities of other flies, and their sexual preference was not altered.

Fig. 2
figure 2

d03517 insertion did not affect sexual behaviours. a Sexual discrimination assay. Both wild-type and d03517 mutant male flies preferred to show courtship behaviours towards decapitated virgin female flies than decapitated male flies. Number of male flies tested: wt, n = 20; d03517, n = 20. Mann-Whitney U test, **P < 0.01. b-d Male-male courtship behaviours for 15-min period. Wild-type and d03517 mutant male flies showed very similar male-male courtship indices (Mann-Whitney U test, P > 0.05), including one-wing extension frequency (b), circling frequency (c), latency to courtship (d). “ns”, not significant, P > 0.05. Pairs of flies tested: wt, n = 27; d03517, n = 27. Error bars represent SEM

Above results, however, do not exclude the possibility that when only encountering a single male fly, a d03517 mutant male fly may choose courtship over aggression leading to a decrease in aggressiveness. To address this possibility, we examined male-male courtship behaviours. A male fly was paired with another male fly, and courtship indices (i.e. unilateral wing vibration, circling frequency, and courtship latency) were analyzed. Wild-type flies showed low-frequency male-male courtship behaviours (Fig. 2b-d). Compared to wild-type male flies, d03517 mutant male flies did not show an increase in male-male courtship behaviours (Fig. 2b-d).

We also analyzed male-female sexual behaviours. A male fly was paired with a virgin female fly, and courtship indices (i.e. unilateral wing vibration, circling frequency, courtship latency and copulation latency) were analyzed. No significant difference in male-female mating behaviours was observed between wild-type and d03517 mutant male flies (Additional file 6: Figure S3).

In summary, d03517 insertion decreased fly aggressiveness, but did not affect locomotor activity, olfactory avoidance response and sexual behaviours. We named the corresponding gene of this phenotype (i.e. decrease in aggressiveness) peacefulness (pfs), and d03517 insertion is hereinafter referred to as pfsd03517.

The pfs gene encodes for the fly ortholog of Mocs1

pfsd03517 is inserted into a genomic site within the first exon of the gene CG33048 located on the 3rd chromosome (Fig. 3a) [32, 33]. CG33048 encodes for an enzyme that is the fly ortholog of Molydenum Cofactor Protein 1 (Mocs1). In addition to CG33048, several other genes are also located close to the d03517 insertion site. Since d03517 is inserted into the first exon of CG33048, we performed complementation tests to examine if d03517 is allelic to available mutations affecting CG33048. We firstly tested mocs11, a partial loss-of-function mutation that decreases the enzymatic activity of Mocs1 [34, 35]. Sequencing analysis shows that mocs11 carries a C-to-T point mutation in the coding sequence that changes Thr304 into Ile (see Materials and methods). We found that both mocs11 homozygotes and d03517/mocs11 trans-heterozygotes showed a significant decrease in the levels of intermale aggressiveness (Fig. 3b). We then examined another P-element insertion line PBac{WH}f03019 (f03019) in which P-element is inserted into the 4th exon of CG33048 [32, 33]. Similarly, a significant decrease in intermale aggressiveness was observed in f03019 homozygotes and f03019/mocs11 transheterozygotes (Fig. 3b). These results suggest strongly that the pfs gene is CG33048.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The pfs gene is CG33048 that encodes for the fly ortholog of Mocs1. a The organization of genes near insertion sites of d03517 and PBac{WH}f03019 (f03019). d03517 is inserted into the 1st exon of CG33048, 126 bp downstream of the transcription start site. f03019 is inserted into the 4th exon of CG33048, 104 bp upstream of the transcription stop site. Exons and introns of CG33048 are indicated by blue rectangles and black lines, respectively. b Complementation tests and transgene rescue. Behaviours of a pair of male flies for 15-min period were recorded and analyzed by the CADABRA automated analysis system [28]. All mutant alleles were backcrossed with CS wild-type flies for four generations and thus were in w+ background. Pairs of flies tested: wt, n = 28; mocs11/ mocs11, n = 26; f03019/f03019, n = 25; f03019/mocs11, n = 21; d03517/mocs11, n = 22; genomic rescue construct/+; d03517/mocs11, n = 33. Kruskal-Wallis and post hoc Mann-Whitney U tests, ***P < 0.001. Error bars represent SEM

To further confirm this, we performed transgene rescue experiments. We generated transgenic flies carrying a genomic rescue construct containing entire CG33048 sequence, and then crossed this rescue transgene into pfs mutant background. We found that the aggression phenotype in pfs mutants could be completely rescued by CG33048 (Fig. 3b).

Taken together, these results indicate that the corresponding gene of the aggression phenotype is CG33048 that encodes for the fly ortholog of Mocs1.

Pfs is broadly expressed in the brain

We then performed in situ hybridization to examine the expression pattern of Pfs/Mocs1. We found that pfs mRNAs were broadly expressed throughout the brain (Fig. 4a). The intensity of staining was significantly decreased in pfsd03517 homozygous mutants, supporting the specificity of the staining (Fig. 4b and c). Within the brain, pfs mRNA showed higher levels of expression in superior medial protocerebrum (SMP), antennal lobe (AL), and suboesophageal ganglion (SOG) (Fig. 4a).

Fig. 4
figure 4

pfs is broadly expressed in the adult male fly brain. a and b in situ hybridization detecting pfs mRNAs in an adult male fly brain. a Wild type. b pfsd03517 homozygous mutant. c Relative expression levels of pfs were quantified. Compared to that in wild type, the level of pfs mRNAs was significantly reduced in pfsd03517 homozygous mutants. Number of flies tested: wt, n = 7; pfsd03517, n = 7. Student’s t-test, *P < 0.05. Error bars represent SEM. d and e Confocal sectioning of fluorescence in male adult brains of w1118 control flies (d) and w1118 flies carrying GFP-tagged genomic pfs transgene (i.e. genomic-pfs-GFP) (e). d Autofluorescence background in w1118 control flies. e Pfs-GFP was broadly expressed in the adult brain of w1118 flies carrying genomic-pfs-GFP. Abbreviations: SMP, superior medial protocerebrum; AL, antennal lobe; SOG, suboesophageal ganglion. Scale bar: 50 μm

To examine the distribution of Pfs protein, we tagged the Pfs protein by engineering the pfs genomic construct that rescued the aggression phenotype (see Fig. 3b). Consistent with the results from in situ hybridization (Fig. 4a), we found that the tagged Pfs under control of the endogenous regulatory sequence showed a broad distribution in the adult brain (Fig. 4e).

Neuronal-specific knockdown of pfs decreased intermale aggressiveness

To determine if pfs is required in neurons for the control of aggression, we performed neuronal-specific knockdown of pfs. A UAS-pfs-RNAi transgene (pfs GL01549) was expressed in all neurons under control of the neuronal-specific driver nSyb-Gal4. We found that male flies expressing this UAS-pfs-RNAi transgene displayed a significant decrease in aggressiveness (Fig. 5a and b). Similar results were obtained when pfs was knocked down by neuronal-specific expression of another independent UAS-pfs-RNAi transgene (pfs7858R1) (Fig. 5a and b). These results indicate an essential role for pfs in neurons for fly aggression.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Neuronal-specific knockdown of pfs decreased aggressiveness. Pfs was knocked down in flies carrying a pan-neuronal-specific driver nSyb-Gal4 and a UAS-pfs-RNAi transgene. Two independent UAS-pfs-RNAi transgenes pfsGL01549 and pfs7858R1 were used in the experiments. a Number of lunges. b Number of wing threats. Behaviours of a pair of male flies for 15-min period were examined. Pairs of flies tested: nSyb-Gal4/+, n = 25; pfsGL01549/+, n = 22; nSyb-Gal4/ pfsGL01549, n = 23; pfs7858R1/+, n = 28; nSyb-Gal4/+; pfs7858R1/+, n = 20. Kruskal-Wallis and post hoc Mann-Whitney U tests, **P < 0.01. ***P < 0.001. Error bars represent SEM

Overexpression of pfs greatly increased intermale aggressiveness

Above results indicate a necessary role for pfs in the control of fly aggression. To determine if Pfs actively promotes aggressiveness, we overexpressed Pfs in flies and examined their intermale aggressive behaviours. The genomic rescue transgene containing the entire pfs gene was crossed into wild-type flies. Although one copy of this transgene did not significantly increase the number of lunges or wing threats (Fig. 6a and b), we found that with one copy of this transgene, there was a small but significant increase in tussling, an intense fighting behaviour that is rarely observed in wild-type flies (Fig. 6c). More strikingly, when two copies of this transgene were introduced into wild-type flies, all agonistic behaviours were greatly increased (Fig. 6a-c, compare Additional file 7: Movie S4B to S4A).

Fig. 6
figure 6

pfs overexpression greatly increased intermale aggressiveness. Behaviours of a pair of male flies for 15-min period were examined. a Number of lunges. b Number of wing threats. c Number of tussles. Introduction of one copy of the pfs genomic rescue construct into wild-type flies did not increase the number of lunges or wing threats, but led to a small but significant increase in tussling, an intense fighting behaviour rarely observed in wild type. When two copies of the pfs genomic rescue construct were introduced into wild-type flies, all agonistic behaviours were significantly increased. Pairs of flies tested: wt, n = 22; 1× genomic construct, n = 27; 2× genomic construct, n = 25. Kruskal-Wallis and post hoc Mann-Whitney U tests, *P < 0.05, ***P < 0.001. “ns”, not significant, P > 0.05. Error bars represent SEM

Knocking down another component of the MoCo synthesis pathway also decreased intermale aggressiveness

Pfs may regulate intermale aggression through its function in the MoCo synthesis pathway. Alternatively, Pfs may function in a different pathway that is required for fly aggression. To distinguish among these possibilities, we tested if knocking down cinnamon (cin), encoding for another enzyme catalyzing the last step in the MoCo synthesis pathway (Fig. 7a) [22], causes a pfs-like aggression phenotype.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Knocking down cin decreased aggressiveness. a Schematic illustration of the MoCo biosynthesis pathway in Drosophila and humans. Mocs1 catalyzes the conversion of GTP to cPMP, which is converted to molybdopterin (MPT) by Mocs2 in Drosophila and by Mocs2A and 2B in humans. Cin or its human ortholog Gephyrin catalyzes the conversion of MPT to MoCo. b and c cin was knocked down in flies carrying a pan-neuronal-specific driver nSyb-Gal4 and a UAS-cin-RNAi transgene. Two independent UAS-cin-RNAi transgenes cinKK102795 and cinHMS00420 were used in the experiments. b Number of lunges. c Number of wing threats. Behaviours of a pair of male flies for 15-min period were examined. Pairs of flies tested: nSyb-Gal4/+, n = 21; cinKK102795/+, n = 24; nSyb-Gal4/cinKK102795, n = 25; cinHMS00420/+, n = 22; nSyb-Gal4/+;cinHMS00420/+, n = 23. Kruskal-Wallis and post hoc Mann-Whitney U tests, *P < 0.05. **P < 0.01. ***P < 0.001. Error bars represent SEM

The expression of cin was knocked down in flies by expressing a UAS-cin-RNAi transgene (i.e. cinKK102795) under control of the neuronal-specific driver nSyb-Gal4. Compared to control flies carrying either the driver or the UAS-cin-RNAi alone, cin knockdown flies showed a significant decrease in aggressiveness (Fig. 7b and c). We also performed knockdown by using a different UAS-cin-RNAi transgene (i.e. cinHMS00420). A similar decrease in aggressiveness was observed (Fig. 7b and c).

To determine potential effects of reducing cin on physical capabilities of knockdown flies, we examined fly climbing ability and locomotor activity. No significant difference in climbing ability was observed when knockdown flies carrying both nSyb-Gal4 driver and UAS-cin-RNAi (i.e. cinKK102795 or cinHMS00420) were compared to control flies carrying nSyb-Gal4 driver or UAS-cin-RNAi only (Additional file 8: Figure S4A).

We also examined fly locomotion over 15-min period. For knockdown using UAS-cinHMS00420, no significant difference in total distance of movements was observed when comparing knockdown flies carrying both nSyb-Gal4 driver and UAS-cinHMS00420 to control flies carrying nSyb-Gal4 driver or UAS-cinHMS00420 only (Additional file 8: Figure S4B). For knockdown using UAS-cinKK102795, the travel distance of knockdown flies carrying both nSyb-Gal4 driver and UAS-cinKK102795 is lower than that of control flies carrying nSyb-Gal4 driver only, but is not significantly different from that of control flies carrying UAS-cinKK102795 only (Additional file 8: Figure S4B). Those results argue against that the decrease in the levels of aggressiveness in cin knockdown flies was caused by impaired physical capabilities.

Taken together, these results support that Pfs/Mocs1 regulates aggression through its action in the MoCo biosynthesis pathway.

Discussion

In this study, we identify Pfs as a novel and important player in the control of intermale aggression in Drosophila. Mutations in pfs decreased intermale fly aggressiveness, but did not affect locomotor activity, climbing ability, olfactory avoidance response and sexual behaviours. Like pfs mutations, knocking down another component (i.e. Cin) of the MoCo synthesis pathway also decreased intermale aggressiveness, supporting a necessary role for Pfs/Mocs1 in the MoCo biosynthesis pathway for fly aggressiveness. That overexpression of Pfs caused a dramatic increase in intermale aggressiveness suggests strongly that Pfs/Mocs1 and the MoCo synthesis pathway actively promote intermale aggression in Drosophila.

We propose that Pfs/Mocs1 controls fly aggression by regulating the synthesis of MoCo, which in turn modulates the activity of MoCo-dependent molybdoenzymes. The MoCo biosynthesis pathway is conserved throughout evolution [22]. MoCo synthesis involves multiple steps that convert guanosine triphosphate (GTP) to MoCo. Mocs1 catalyzes the first step that is the conversion of GTP to cyclic pyranopterin monophosphate (cPMP). cPMP is then converted to molybdopterin (MPT) dithiolate by MPT synthase, which consists of two subunits Mocs2A and Mocs2B. The final step is catalyzed by Gephyrin, leading to the conversion of MPT to MoCo. MoCo forms the active site of all eukaryotic molybdenum-dependent molybdoenzymes such as sulphite oxidase, xanthine oxidase/dehydrogenase and aldehyde oxidase [23]. Interestingly, it is reported that allopurinol, an inhibitor of xanthine oxidase, displays anti-aggressive effects, and could effectively treat dementia and schizophrenia patients associated with escalated aggression [24,25,26,27]. Thus, Pfs/Mocs1-dependent MoCo pathways may control aggression across phylogeny.

Pfs/Mocs1 may control aggression by regulating metabolic activities in the brain. MoCo-dependent molybdoenzymes are involved in the regulation of a number of metabolic activities [23]. Sulphite oxidase is required for the degradation of sulphur-containing amino acids and lipids [36]. Xanthine oxidase mediates the catabolism of purines by converting hypoxanthine to uric acid [37]. And aldehyde oxidase is involved in the catabolism of bioamines such as serotonin and dopamine [38]. Together, these molybdoenzymes may modulate the metabolic state within the brain for promoting aggression. A link between glucose metabolism and aggressiveness has been reported recently [39]. By manipulating oxidation phosphorylation in honeybee and Drosophila, Robinson and coworkers show that aerobic glycolysis increases aggressiveness. Similarly, we speculate that Pfs/Mocs1 regulates MoCo-dependent molybdoenzymes through MoCo synthesis, which in turn modulate metabolic plasticity in the brain for the control of aggression.

Pfs/Mocs1-dependent MoCo pathways may also promote aggressiveness by increasing oxidative stress. Both xanthine oxidase and aldehyde oxidase catalyze the reactions leading to the generation of reactive oxygen species (ROS), such as hydrogen peroxide and superoxide ion [40, 41]. Oxidative stress caused by the accumulation of ROS, has been linked to anxiety and aggression in animal models [42, 43]. For instance, mouse defective in superoxide dismutase 1 (Sod1), an enzyme with antioxidant activity, displays a dramatic increase in aggressiveness [43].

MoCo deficiency (MOCOD) is a rare and severe disease in humans [44]. Patients with MOCOD display severe neurological symptoms, such as intellectual disability, autism, seizures, feeding difficulties, and neurodevelopmental abnormalities. It is suggested that neural damages are mainly due to sulfite oxidase deficiency and the accumulation of toxic levels of sulphite [45]. Over 50% of MoCo deficiency in humans are due to mutations in the MOCS1A open reading frame [46], which mostly result in early death of children [47]. By contrast, we did not observe any developmental defects in fly pfs mutants. One likely explanation is that pfs alleles are not null, and thus do not completely eliminate the activity of MoCo-dependent molybdoenzymes. Alternatively or additionally, flies may be more resistant to the accumulation of toxic metabolic intermediates due to the decrease in the activities of MoCo-dependent molybdoenzymes.

Our results showing the aggression phenotype caused by manipulating the MoCo biosynthesis pathway in Drosophila, together with observed anti-aggressive effects by the inhibition of MoCo-dependent enzymes in human patients, support the existence of a novel and evolutionarily conserved MoCo-dependent mechanism for the control of aggression. A number of neurological and psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia, dementia and Alzheimer’s disease, show a substantial association with abnormal aggressiveness [14, 48]. It would be interesting to determine if patients with these disorders show elevated levels of MoCo and/or MoCo-dependent molybdoenzymes. Targeting Pfs/Mocs1 and molybdoenzymes may thus allow the development of novel therapeutic strategies to treat diseases associated with escalated aggression.

Materials and methods

Genetics and rearing conditions

P-element insertion lines P{XP}d03517 and PBac{WH}f03019 were obtained from the Exelixis collection at Harvard. mocs11, UAS-pfs-RNAi-GL01549, and UAS-cin-RNAi-HMS00420 lines were obtained from Bloomington Stock Center. The UAS-pfs-RNAi-7858R1 line was obtained from National Institute of Genetics Fly Stock Center in Japan. The UAS-cin-RNAi-KK102795 line was obtained from the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center. To eliminate the effects of different genetic backgrounds on fly behaviours, all pfs mutant alleles were backcrossed with Canton-S (CS) wild-type flies for 4 generations and were in w+ background. CS flies were used as wild-type controls in the experiments. For knockdown experiments, female flies carrying the Gal4 driver were crossed with male flies carrying the UAS-RNAi transgene. The progeny male flies carrying both Gal4 and UAS-RNAi transgenes were then compared to male flies carrying Gal4 driver or UAS-RNAi transgene only. Flies were reared on standard corn meal at 25 °C and 50–60% humidity with 12 h light-dark cycle.

For rescue experiments, the genomic fragment containing the entire coding sequence (2823 bp) of the CG33048 gene, the 1019 bp sequence upstream of CG33048 and the 755 bp sequence downstream of CG33048, was amplified by PCR with two primers 5’ CTCCGAGCGGAGACTCTAGCGCTAGCCTCTGTGTACTGCACCGTGTA 3′ and 5’ CTCACCATGGATCCAGATCCACTAGTGGGCCCAAAGATGGATGACA 3′. The resulting PCR fragment and the pJFRC-MUH-eGFP vector linearized with Nhe1 and Spe1, were ligated together using In-Fusion HD Cloning Plus Kits (Takara Bio UAS Inc.) to generate genomic rescue construct for making transgenic flies. Genetic crosses were then performed to introduce the CG33048 genomic rescue construct into mocs1 trans-heterozygous mutants (i.e. pfsd03517/mocs11). To overexpress Pfs, wild-type flies carrying one or two copies of the CG33048 genomic rescue construct were generated.

To tag the pfs coding sequence in above genomic rescue construct with GFP, the genomic rescue construct was used as templates for PCR amplification. Two primers 5’ CTCCGAGCGGAGACTCTAGCGCTAGCCTCTGTGTACTGCACCGTGTA 3′ and 5’ CTCACCATGGATCCAGATCCACTAGTTTCGACTTCTGTAACTATCC 3′ were used for amplifying the entire upstream regulatory sequence and the pfs coding sequence before the stop codon. The resulting PCR fragment and the pJFRC-MUH-eGFP vector linearized with Nhe1 and Spe1, were ligated together using In-Fusion HD Cloning Plus Kits (Takara Bio UAS Inc.) to generate the genomic-pfs-GFP construct. The genomic pfs-GFP construct differs from the rescue construct only in the sequence downstream from the coding sequence; while the genomic rescue construct contains 755 bp genomic sequence downstream of the pfs coding sequence, the genomic pfs-GFP construct contains an in-frame fusion between the last codon of the pfs coding sequence and the GFP sequence.

To identify mutation sites in the mocs11 allele, genomic DNA was isolated from homozygous mocs11 mutant flies and used as templates for PCR amplification. The resulting fragment containing the entire sequence (2823 bp) of pfs was sequenced completely, which was then compared to that of wild-type pfs genomic sequence. A C-to-T missense mutation in the coding sequence that changes Thr304 to Ile in Pfs/Mocs1 protein was identified in the genomic DNA of the mocs11 allele.

Aggression assays

Newly emerged male flies were collected and isolated in 2 ml Eppendorf tubes containing 1 ml fly food for 5 to 7 days before behavioural experiments. Male flies of same age with similar body size were selected for behavioural assays. Aggressive behaviours were examined similarly as described previously [28]. Briefly, two male flies were gently aspirated into a fighting chamber at 25 °C with 50–60% humidity. Their behaviours were immediately recorded with a CCD camera for 15-min period. The movies were then analyzed by using the CADABRA (Caltech Automated Drosophila Aggression-Courtship Behavioral Repertoire Analysis) automated analysis system [28]. For examining aggressive behaviours between a wild-type and a mutant male fly, wild-type flies were labeled with color paint.

To determine the potential dominance, wild-type and mutant male flies were anesthetised by CO2, and marked on thorax with yellow and white acrylic paints, respectively. Flies were allowed for recovery at least 24 h before behavioural assays. A wild-type male fly was paired with a mutant male fly and introduced into a small chamber containing a food patch in the center similarly as described previously [49]. A score of 1 was given if a fly (e.g. wild type) successfully occupied the central food patch after 10-min period. A score of 0 indicates that the fly did not occupy the central food patch after 10-min period. Successful occupancy of the central food patch is considered as an indication of dominance.

Analysis of pfs expression in male adult brains

For fluorescence in situ hybridization, custom Stellaris® FISH probes were designed to detect pfs/mocs1 mRNAs by utilizing the Stellaris® FISH Probe Designer (Biosearch Technologies, Inc., Petaluma, CA) available online at www.biosearchtech.com/stellarisdesigner. The probes were conjugated to the Quasar670 dye and used in FISH assays as described previously [50]. Confocal microscopy was performed by using Olympus laser scanning microscope FV1000. For comparing the relative levels of pfs mRNAs in wild-type and pfs mutant flies, fluorescent intensities in the central brain region were measured.

To examine the expression pattern of genomic-pfs tagged with GFP, adult heads were dissected in phosphate buffer. The brains were immediately mounted on glass slides in phosphate buffer and imaged with confocal microscopy using Olympus laser scanning microscope FV1000.

Male-male courtship assay

To examine male-male courtship behaviours, a pair of male flies with same genotype were introduced into a rectangular chamber. Their behaviours were recorded for 15 min. Unilateral wing extensions and circling numbers were quantified by using the CADABRA automated analysis system. Courtship latency was quantified manually.

Sexual discrimination assay

For each experiment, a CS wild-type virgin female fly and a CS wild-type male fly were decapitated. The decapitated flies were placed to different areas in a rectangular chamber. A test male fly (wild-type or mutant) was then introduced into the chamber. The time that the test male fly showed courtship behaviours towards the decapitated female or the decapitated male fly was quantified.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using GraphPad Prism 7 software. Before data analysis, their normality distributions were examined. Nonparametric tests were performed for data not normally distributed. For comparing more than two genotypes, a Kruskal-Wallis test was performed. If the null hypothesis (i.e. means of all genotypes were the same) was rejected (P < 0.05), we performed multiple Mann-Whitney U tests between a pair of interest to assess whether the means of the two genotypes were significantly different. For comparing two independent groups, an unpaired Mann-Whitney U test was performed. Student’s t-test was performed for data normally distributed.

Abbreviations

AL:

Antennal lobe

CADABRA:

Caltech Automated Drosophila Aggression-Courtship Behavioral Repertoire Analysis

Cin:

Cinnamon

MoCo:

Molybdenum cofactor

Mocs1:

Molybdenum cofactor synthesis 1 protein

Pfs:

Peacefulness

SMP:

Superior medial protocerebrum

SOG:

Suboesophageal ganglion

References

  1. Kravitz EA, Huber R. Aggression in invertebrates. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13(6):736–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Tecott LH, Barondes SH. Genes and aggressiveness. Behavioral genetics. Curr Biol. 1996;6(3):238–40.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Loeber R, Hay D. Key issues in the development of aggression and violence from childhood to early adulthood. Annu Rev Psychol. 1997;48:371–410.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  4. Hoffmann AA. The influence of age and experience with conspecifics on territorial behavior inDrosophila melanogaster. J Insect Behav. 1990;3(1):1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Matsumoto K, Pinna G, Puia G, Guidotti A, Costa E. Social isolation stress-induced aggression in mice: a model to study the pharmacology of neurosteroidogenesis. Stress. 2005;8(2):85–93.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Wang L, Dankert H, Perona P, Anderson DJ. A common genetic target for environmental and heritable influences on aggressiveness in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(15):5657–63.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Kravitz EA, Fernandez Mde L. Aggression in Drosophila. Behav Neurosci. 2015;129(5):549–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Barr CS, Driscoll C. Neurogenetics of aggressive behavior: studies in primates. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2014;17:45–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Takahashi A, Miczek KA. Neurogenetics of aggressive behavior: studies in rodents. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2014;17:3–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Sturtevant AH. Experiments on sex recognition and the problem of sexual selection in Drosoophilia. J Anim Behav. 1915;5(5):351–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Jacobs ME. Influence of light on mating of Drosophila melanogaster. Ecology. 1960;41(1):182–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Hoffmann AA. A laboratory study of male territoriality in the sibling species Drosophila melanogaster and D. simulans. Anim Behav. 1987;35(3):807–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Chen S, Lee AY, Bowens NM, Huber R, Kravitz EA. Fighting fruit flies: a model system for the study of aggression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99(8):5664–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Swann AC. Neuroreceptor mechanisms of aggression and its treatment. J Clin Psychiatry. 2003;64(Suppl 4):26–35.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Hoyer SC, Eckart A, Herrel A, Zars T, Fischer SA, Hardie SL, Heisenberg M. Octopamine in male aggression of Drosophila. Curr Biol. 2008;18(3):159–67.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Zhou C, Rao Y, Rao Y. A subset of octopaminergic neurons are important for Drosophila aggression. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11(9):1059–67.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Dierick HA, Greenspan RJ. Serotonin and neuropeptide F have opposite modulatory effects on fly aggression. Nat Genet. 2007;39(5):678–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Alekseyenko OV, Chan YB, Li R, Kravitz EA. Single dopaminergic neurons that modulate aggression in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(15):6151–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Edwards AC, Ayroles JF, Stone EA, Carbone MA, Lyman RF, Mackay TF. A transcriptional network associated with natural variation in Drosophila aggressive behavior. Genome Biol. 2009;10(7):R76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Katsouni E, Sakkas P, Zarros A, Skandali N, Liapi C. The involvement of substance P in the induction of aggressive behavior. Peptides. 2009;30(8):1586–91.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Asahina K, Watanabe K, Duistermars BJ, Hoopfer E, Gonzalez CR, Eyjolfsdottir EA, Perona P, Anderson DJ. Tachykinin-expressing neurons control male-specific aggressive arousal in Drosophila. Cell. 2014;156(1–2):221–35.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Mendel RR, Leimkuhler S. The biosynthesis of the molybdenum cofactors. J Biol Inorg Chem. 2015;20(2):337–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Schwarz G, Mendel RR, Ribbe MW. Molybdenum cofactors, enzymes and pathways. Nature. 2009;460(7257):839–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Lara DR, Belmonte-de-Abreu P, Souza DO. Allopurinol for refractory aggression and self-inflicted behaviour. J Psychopharmacol. 2000;14(1):81–3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Lara DR, Brunstein MG, Ghisolfi ES, Lobato MI, Belmonte-de-Abreu P, Souza DO. Allopurinol augmentation for poorly responsive schizophrenia. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2001;16(4):235–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Lara DR, Cruz MR, Xavier F, Souza DO, Moriguchi EH. Allopurinol for the treatment of aggressive behaviour in patients with dementia. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2003;18(1):53–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Carr CN, Straley CM, Baugh TB. Allopurinol for the treatment of refractory aggression: a case series. Pharmacotherapy. 2017;37(6):748–54.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Dankert H, Wang L, Hoopfer ED, Anderson DJ, Perona P. Automated monitoring and analysis of social behavior in Drosophila. Nat Methods. 2009;6(4):297–303.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Fernandez MP, Chan YB, Yew JY, Billeter JC, Dreisewerd K, Levine JD, Kravitz EA. Pheromonal and behavioral cues trigger male-to-female aggression in Drosophila. PLoS Biol. 2010;8(11):e1000541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Wang L, Anderson DJ. Identification of an aggression-promoting pheromone and its receptor neurons in Drosophila. Nature. 2010;463(7278):227–31.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Liu W, Liang X, Gong J, Yang Z, Zhang YH, Zhang JX, Rao Y. Social regulation of aggression by pheromonal activation of Or65a olfactory neurons in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(7):896–902.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Thibault ST, Singer MA, Miyazaki WY, Milash B, Dompe NA, Singh CM, Buchholz R, Demsky M, Fawcett R, Francis-Lang HL, et al. A complementary transposon tool kit for Drosophila melanogaster using P and piggyBac. Nat Genet. 2004;36(3):283–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Bellen HJ, Levis RW, He Y, Carlson JW, Evans-Holm M, Bae E, Kim J, Metaxakis A, Savakis C, Schulze KL, et al. The Drosophila gene disruption project: progress using transposons with distinctive site specificities. Genetics. 2011;188(3):731–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Keller EC Jr, Glassman E. A Third Locus (Lxd) affecting xanthine dehydrogenase in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics. 1964;49:663–8.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Schott DR, Baldwin MC, Finnerty V. Molybdenum hydroxylases in Drosophila. III. Further characterization of the low xanthine dehydrogenase gene. Biochem Genet. 1986;24(7–8):509–27.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Kappler U, Enemark JH. Sulfite-oxidizing enzymes. J Biol Inorg Chem. 2015;20(2):253–64.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Agarwal A, Banerjee A, Banerjee UC. Xanthine oxidoreductase: a journey from purine metabolism to cardiovascular excitation-contraction coupling. Crit Rev Biotechnol. 2011;31(3):264–80.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Beedham C, Peet CF, Panoutsopoulos GI, Carter H, Smith JA. Role of aldehyde oxidase in biogenic amine metabolism. Prog Brain Res. 1995;106:345–53.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Li-Byarlay H, Rittschof CC, Massey JH, Pittendrigh BR, Robinson GE. Socially responsive effects of brain oxidative metabolism on aggression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(34):12533–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Battelli MG, Polito L, Bortolotti M, Bolognesi A. Xanthine oxidoreductase-derived reactive species: physiological and pathological effects. Oxidative Med Cell Longev. 2016;2016:3527579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Kucukgoze G, Terao M, Garattini E, Leimkuhler S. Direct comparison of the enzymatic characteristics and superoxide production of the four aldehyde oxidase enzymes present in mouse. Drug Metab Dispos. 2017;45(8):947–55.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Bouayed J, Rammal H, Soulimani R. Oxidative stress and anxiety: relationship and cellular pathways. Oxidative Med Cell Longev. 2009;2(2):63–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Garratt M, Brooks RC. A genetic reduction in antioxidant function causes elevated aggression in mice. J Exp Biol. 2015;218(Pt 2):223–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Schwarz G. Molybdenum cofactor and human disease. Curr Opin Chem Biol. 2016;31:179–87.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Leimkuhler S, Charcosset M, Latour P, Dorche C, Kleppe S, Scaglia F, Szymczak I, Schupp P, Hahnewald R, Reiss J. Ten novel mutations in the molybdenum cofactor genes MOCS1 and MOCS2 and in vitro characterization of a MOCS2 mutation that abolishes the binding ability of molybdopterin synthase. Hum Genet. 2005;117(6):565–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Reiss J, Johnson JL. Mutations in the molybdenum cofactor biosynthetic genes MOCS1, MOCS2, and GEPH. Hum Mutat. 2003;21(6):569–76.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Carmi-Nawi N, Malinger G, Mandel H, Ichida K, Lerman-Sagie T, Lev D. Prenatal brain disruption in molybdenum cofactor deficiency. J Child Neurol. 2011;26(4):460–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Haller J, Kruk MR. Normal and abnormal aggression: human disorders and novel laboratory models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(3):292–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Ramin M, Domocos C, Slawaska-Eng D, Rao Y. Aggression and social experience: genetic analysis of visual circuit activity in the control of aggressiveness in Drosophila. Mol Brain. 2014;7:55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Raj A, van den Bogaard P, Rifkin SA, van Oudenaarden A, Tyagi S. Imaging individual mRNA molecules using multiple singly labeled probes. Nat Methods. 2008;5(10):877–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank people in the Rao laboratory for comments and suggestions; the Bloomington Stock Center, Exelixis collection at Harvard, National Institute of Genetics Fly Stock Center in Japan and the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center for fly lines; D. Anderson and P. Perona for the CADABRA automated analysis software.

Funding

This work was supported by an operating grant (MOP-14688) awarded to Yong Rao from Canadian Institutes of Health Research (http://www.cihr-irsc.gc.ca), an operating grant (Rgpin 341431–10) to Yong Rao from Natural Science and Engineer Research Council of Canada (http://www.nserc-crsng.gc.ca/index_eng.asp), and a McGill Faculty of Medicine studentship (https://www.mcgill.ca/medresearch/students-postdocs/graduate/funding/studentships) to Mahmoudreza Ramin.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets supporting the conclusion of this study are included in this article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MR contributed to writing and editing of the manuscript, study design, performing experiments, data analysis and interpretation. YL contributed to performing experiments and data analysis. WC contributed to performing experiments. HS contributed to performing experiments. YR contributed to writing and editing of the manuscript, study design, data analysis and interpretation. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Yong Rao.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

No applicable.

Consent for publication

No applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1:

Movie S1A. and S1B. Video clips showing aggressive behaviours of wild-type (S1A) and d03517 mutant (S1B) male flies, related to Fig. 1a and b. Male flies of the same genotype were paired together. (ZIP 10240 kb)

Movie S2. A video clip showing aggressive behaviours of a wild-type male fly paired with a d03517 mutant male fly, related to Fig. 1d. The thorax of wild-type male fly was painted with a yellow acrylic paint dot, and the thorax of d03517 male fly was painted with a white acrylic paint dot. Wild-type flies showed much higher aggressiveness, and also occupied the food patch much more successfully. (MP4 2580 kb)

Additional file 3:

Figure S1. d03517 insertion did not affect physical capabilities. (A) Locomotor activity for 15-min period. A wild-type male fly was paired with a d03517 mutant male fly. Total travel distance for 15-min period was quantified. No significant difference between wild-type and d03517 male flies was observed (Mann-Whitney U test, “ns”, not significant, P > 0.05). Number of flies tested: wild type, n = 46; d03517/ d03517, 46. Methods: A pair of male flies were introduced into a chamber. Their behaviours were videotaped for 15 minutes. Their movements within 15-minute period were analyzed and quantified using the CADABRA automated analysis system. (B) Climbing test. The percentage of flies that crossed the 10-cm mark after 15-s climbing were quantified. Number of trials: wt, n = 30; d03517/ d03517, n = 30. No significant difference between wild-type and d03517 mutant flies was observed (Mann-Whitney U test, “ns”, not significant, P > 0.05). Error bars represent SEM. Methods: For each trial, 10-12 male flies (5-7 days after eclosion) were transferred into atransparent tube. The flies were allowed to recover for 1 hour. The tube was then tapped three times to force flies down at the bottom of the tube. Their climbing behaviors were videotaped. The percentage of flies that crossed the 10-cm mark after 15 seconds was quantified. The experiment was repeated 3 times for each tube. For each genotype, about 86 to 122 flies were tested. (PDF 105 kb)

Additional file 4:

Figure S2. d03517 insertion did not affect olfactory avoidance response. (A) T-maze apparatus used for testing olfactory avoidance response. The apparatus consists of two separate compartments. One compartment is used for fly habituation following their introduction into the apparatus. The second compartment connects to two plastic tubes. One tube is empty, and another tube is filled with benzaldehyde. (B) Olfactory avoidance responses by wild-type and d03517 mutant flies. No significant difference between wild-type and d03517 male flies was observed (Mann-Whitney U test, “ns”, not significant, P > 0.05). Number of tests per genotype: wt, n = 12; d03517/d03517, n = 7. For each test, 10–20 flies were examined. Error bars represent SEM. Methods: Prior to the experiments, flies were deprived of food for 3-6 hours. They were then introduced into a T-maze apparatus containing two compartments (Supplementary Fig. S2A). The first compartment is for fly habituation. The second compartment connects to two plastic tubes. One tube is empty. Another tube has a cotton ball containing 1ml of benzaldehyde, a strong fruit fly repellent, at the open end. For each experiment, 10-20 flies were gently introduced into the apparatus. Flies were kept in the first compartment for 90 seconds, and then allowed to move into the second compartment for 120 seconds. Number of flies that moved into benzaldehyde-containing tube or empty tube were counted. Smell index 10.1186/s13041-018-0417-0 was then calculated as follows: Smell index= (Number of flies in empty tube-number of flies in benzaldehyde tube)/(Total number of flies). (PDF 220 kb)

Movie S3. A video clip showing a d03517 mutant male fly selected a decapitated female fly over a decapitated male fly for courtship, related to Fig. 2a. (MP4 1487 kb)

Additional file 6:

Figure S3. Male-female courtship behaviours for 15-min period. Wild-type and d03517 mutant male flies showed very similar male-female courtship indices (Mann-Whitney U test, P > 0.05), including one-wing extensions (A), circling frequency (B), latency to courtship (C), and latency to copulation (D). Number of flies tested: wt, n = 20; d03517/ d03517, n = 20. Error bars represent SEM. Methods: To examine male-female courtship behaviours, a CS wild-type virgin female fly was paired with a wild-type or a mutant male fly, and introduced into a rectangular chamber. Their behaviours were recorded for 15 minutes. Unilateral wing extensions and circling numbers were quantified by using the CADABRA automated analysis system. Courtship latency and copulation latency were quantified manually. (PDF 166 kb)

Additional file 7:

Movie S4A. and S4B. Video clips showing aggressive behaviours of wild-type male flies (4A) and male flies overexpressing pfs (4B), related to Fig. 6. Male flies of the same genotype were paired together. pfs overexpression greatly increased fly aggressiveness. (ZIP 8304 kb)

Additional file 8:

Figure S4. cin knockdown did not affect physical capabilities. (A) Climbing test. The percentage of flies that crossed the 10-cm mark after 15-s climbing was quantified. Number of trials: nSyb-Gal4/+, n = 27; cinKK102795/+, n = 33; nSyb-Gal4/cinKK102795, n = 36; cinHMS00420/+, n = 24; nSyb-Gal4/+;cinHMS00420/+, n = 24. No significant difference was observed between knockdown flies and control flies (Kruskal-Wallis and post hoc Mann-Whitney U tests, “ns”, not significant, P > 0.05). (B) Locomotor activity for 15-min period. Total travel distance for 15-min period was quantified. Number of individual flies tested: nSyb-Gal4/+, n = 42; cinKK102795/+, n = 48; nSyb-Gal4/cinKK102795, n = 50; cinHMS00420/+, n = 44; nSyb-Gal4/+;cinHMS00420/+, n = 46. Kruskal-Wallis and post hoc Mann-Whitney U tests, ***P < 0.001. “ns”, not significant, P > 0.05. Error bars represent SEM. (PDF 180 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ramin, M., Li, Y., Chang, WT. et al. The peacefulness gene promotes aggression in Drosophila. Mol Brain 12, 1 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13041-018-0417-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13041-018-0417-0