Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Hippocampal activity during the transverse patterning task declines with cognitive competence but not with age

Abstract

Background

The hippocampus is a brain region that is particularly affected by age-related morphological changes. It is generally assumed that a loss in hippocampal volume results in functional deficits that contribute to age-related cognitive decline. In a combined cross-sectional behavioural and magnetoencephalography (MEG) study we investigated whether hippocampal-associated neural current flow during a transverse patterning task - which requires learning relational associations between stimuli - correlates with age and whether it is modulated by cognitive competence.

Results

Better performance in several tests of verbal memory, verbal fluency and executive function was indeed associated with higher hippocampal neural activity. Age, however, was not related to the strength of hippocampal neural activity: elderly participants responded slower than younger individuals but on average produced the same neural mass activity.

Conclusions

Our results suggest that in non-pathological aging, hippocampal neural activity does not decrease with age but is rather related to cognitive competence.

Background

Normal aging is accompanied by a multitude of morphological changes in the brain. Consistently, volumetric MRI studies show a decline in total brain volume due to aging [e.g. [1–7]]. Although brain shrinkage is relatively widespread, brain regions differ in their intensity of volume loss [e.g. [8, 9]]. Longitudinal [e.g. [8, 10]], as well as cross-sectional studies [e.g. [11]] demonstrate that especially the caudate, the cerebellum, tertiary association cortices and the hippocampus are affected by shrinkage. Furthermore, Raz et al. [8] report that particularly in the hippocampus - essential for declarative memory, spatial navigation and contextual relations [12, 13] - the magnitude of shrinkage accelerates with increasing age: the hippocampus shrinkage rate in healthy older people is more than twice that of younger people [1, 14–16]. Volume reductions within the hippocampus were also analyzed in a recent MRI study with volume reduction found to be more severe in posterior regions [17]. In sum, these observations demonstrate that the hippocampus is structurally especially affected by the aging process. The loss may be pronounced in pathological ageing, e.g., in Morbus Alzheimer the hippocampus is one of the regions being particularly vulnerable to degeneration [18].

Morphological changes, however, are not linearly related to brain function. Nonetheless, it is plausible to assume that regional brain damage will alter neural firing patterns in space and time. Hence, age-related changes in neural activation during memory tasks should be expected in the medial temporal lobe, notably the hippocampal region. On this account, several studies employing functional neuroimaging methods examined age effects on neuronal activation patterns during diverse memory tasks [e.g. [19–30]]. Results concerning age effects on activity of the hippocampal region differ across studies. Some studies report more activity in elderly people in right parahippocampal regions during memory retrieval [20], in the right hippocampus during autobiographical memory tasks [30] as well as higher cerebral blood flow increase in the medial temporal lobe during memory encoding [29]. In contrast, there are also studies that report less activation for elderly people in mediotemporal structures, including the hippocampus and the parahippocampus, during encoding [21, 24], encoding and retrieval [19], correct word recognition [27], working memory tasks [25, 26] as well as during visual attention and episodic retrieval [26]. Furthermore, there are also a number of studies that do not find age-related differences in the magnitude of activation in the hippocampal region during memory encoding [22, 23, 28, 31–33].

Given that the hippocampus is thought to be responsible for the organization and structuring of memory storage [34, 35] rather than for keeping the code of memory contents, this might explain why the results of studies concerning age-related differences in mediotemporal activation patterns during memory tasks are rather inconsistent. In the present study we therefore employed the transverse patterning task [36, 37], i.e., contextual processing which consistently engages hippocampal function.

The transverse patterning paradigm requires learning nontransitive binary relations between three stimuli, pairs of which are presented. The well-known game "rock-paper-scissors" is an example of a transverse patterning paradigm with meaningful stimuli. The problem cannot be solved with elemental discriminations of single stimuli but requires learning relational associations [37, 38]. The hippocampal formation is thought to play a central role in configural learning [12, 13]. Consistent with this theory are the findings that damage of the hippocampal formation in humans [39–41], rats [37] and monkeys [42–44] impairs performance on the transverse patterning task. Furthermore, Driscoll and colleagues [45] found age-related differences in behavioral performance on a transverse patterning task: compared to younger people, elderly people were less often able to complete the transverse patterning task successfully, required more trials to reach criterion and made more mistakes. Here, we used magnetoencephalography (MEG) to track the fast neural responses induced by the transverse patterning task. A series of studies has demonstrated that activity from the medial temporal lobe can be assessed with MEG [46–63].

We specifically planned on assessing whether the hippocampal-associated activation pattern in healthy people changes with age. Furthermore, we recorded the extent to which this neural activity is associated with cognitive competence.

Methods

Participants

Fifty-two right-handed and healthy subjects (23 males and 29 females) ranging in age from 18 to 89 years (m = 52.4 years, sd = 19.6) (Additional file 1) with a mean MMSE score of 29.4 (SD = 1.08) participated in this study. Their mean education was 15 years (ranging from 10 to 22 years). None of the included participants reported a history of drug and/or alcohol abuse. Subjects were recruited by notifications posted on the campus of the University of Konstanz and in several residential homes for the elderly in the Konstanz area and by advertisement in the local newspaper and radio station. They were paid 30€ for their participation. Exclusion criteria were acute as well as a history of psychiatric diseases, a history of psychopharmacological medication, left-handedness (to control for potential influences of brain lateralization due to handedness), metal objects in the body as well as a history of severe head injuries or neurological problems (like epilepsy, strokes, brain tumors etc.). The ethics committee of the University of Konstanz approved this study.

Procedures

Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants were familiarized with the MEG chamber, and study procedures and goals were clarified. All participants gave written informed consent. Afterwards subjects were screened with the MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview [64] to assure that the participants did not suffer from a psychiatric disease. Subsequently, demographic data were assessed and handedness was determined using the Edinburgh Inventory [65]. Furthermore, cognitive abilities were assessed with the CERAD-NP-plus test battery [66] with the subtests Verbal Fluency (VF = sum score of semantic and phonemic fluency), Word List Learning (WLL), Word List Delayed Recall (WLDR), Word List Recognition (WLR), Figure Recall (FR), Trail Making Test A and B (TMT-A/B). Additionally, the Digit Symbol and the Digit Span subtests of the German version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale [HAWIE-R; [67]] as well as the Benton Visual Retention Test - revised form [68] were conducted.

Training

The two tasks and the basic design that were used in this experiment were adapted from the study of Hanlon and colleagues [50, 51]: the Rock, Paper, Scissors task (RPS) and the Transverse Patterning task (TP) (Figure 1). The well-known childhood game "Rock, Paper, Scissors" is an analogous task to the TP task with meaningful stimuli and relations. It was chosen as a training task to familiarize subjects with the underlying logic of transverse patterning designs.

Figure 1
figure 1

Stimuli of the Rock, Paper, Scissors (S+P-, P+R-, R+S-) and the Transverse Patterning (B+Y-, Y+R-, R+B-) task.

Immediately prior to the MEG measurement, participants were trained on both tasks to familiarize themselves with the procedure. The first task to train was RPS where two out of three pictures of a hand symbolizing rock, paper or scissors were presented simultaneously. The significance of any one item depends on with which other item it is currently paired: When rock and scissors are paired, rock is correct, when scissors and paper are paired, scissors are correct, and if paper and rock are presented together, paper is correct (R+ S-, S+ P-, P+ R-).

In the second task (TP), the stimuli consisted of three disks in the colors blue, yellow or red. To solve the task, participants had to learn the following rule: When blue and yellow are presented together, blue is correct, when yellow and red are presented together, yellow is designated correct, and when red and blue are shown together, red is correct (B+ Y-, Y+ R-, R+ B-) (Figure 1). Participants learned the abstract transverse patterning rule through explicit instruction.

In each trial of the RPS and TP task, two of the three pictures were presented next to each other on a grey computer screen using Presentation® (Neurobehavioral Systems, version 9.00). All stimuli were 9 × 10 cm (height × width). Participants were instructed to choose the correct picture as fast as possible by pressing the corresponding mouse button (left button for the left picture and right button for the right picture). The button press cleared the stimulus pair from the screen. Pictures were balanced for left and right side presentation.

Each trial began with the presentation of a grey screen with a fixation cross for 3-7 s to aid participants in maintaining gaze on the center of the screen, followed by the presentation of the stimulus pairs. Each pair of stimuli was presented for up to 10 s or until the participant responded via mouse button press. The response was followed immediately by a feedback sound: a high-pitched tone (1000 Hz) if the reaction was correct and a low-pitched tone (250 Hz) if the answer was incorrect. If the subject did not respond after 10 s, the trial was considered incorrect and the picture disappeared, the fixation cross appeared, and a new trial started (Figure 2). Participants either needed to accomplish 10 correct training trials in a row or 42 trials altogether.

Figure 2
figure 2

Experimental design of the Transverse Patterning task.

MEG testing

After training, MEG recordings were obtained, first for the RPS task to familiarize subjects with the MEG environment and then for the TP task. For artifact rejection, we recorded the electro-occulogram (EOG) using bipolar electrode configuration where electrodes were attached near the left and right outer canthus and below and above the right eye. For recording of the electrocardiogram (ECG), two additional electrodes were attached at the left lower forearm and the right collarbone. Subsequently, participants were seated in the magnetically shielded room (Vakuumschmelze Hanau) and their head shapes were digitized with a Polhemus 3 Space Fasttrack (Polhemus, Colchester, VT, USA). Five index points were determined to calculate the relative head position within the MEG sensor for source analysis. The subjects' head position relative to the pickup coils of the sensor was estimated before and after the measurement. During MEG measurement subjects were lying supine in a comfortable position. A video camera installed inside the chamber allowed monitoring subjects' behavior and ensured compliance throughout the experiment. To avoid artifacts, subjects were asked to stay still and to avoid head and eye movements as well as eye blinks during trials.

The experimental set-up and the stimuli were similar to the training procedure. Pictures were presented with a video projector (JVCâ„¢, DLA-G11E) with a refresh rate of 100 Hz on a white plastic screen attached to the ceiling of the room. Subjects responded by pressing the corresponding button on a button box with their right hand. In order to avoid magnetic interference, the feedback tones were presented through ear tubes. Sound intensity was adapted to the individual hearing level before measurement. MEG was recorded continuously and digitized at a rate of 678.17 Hz using a 148-channel whole head magnetometer (MAGNESâ„¢, 2500 WH, 4 D Neuroimaging, San Diego, USA). A real band-pass filter of 0.1 - 200 Hz was used for data acquisition. EOG and ECG were recorded with a SynAmps amplifier (Neuroscan â„¢) using Ag/AgC1 electrodes.

The experiment ended either after the subject had achieved 120 correct trials or when 180 trials in total had been presented.

Data Analysis

Since the RPS task was chosen as a training task in order to familiarize subjects with the transverse patterning design and the MEG environment, data analysis was solely conducted for the transverse patterning task. After noise-reduction of environmental magnetic noise (noise reduction procedure, 4 D Neuroimaging), using the reference channels of the WH2500 data were analyzed using BESA (Brain Electrical Source Analyses; MEGIS Software GmbH) software version 5.2.4.. Cardiac activity and eye movements were removed with a semi-automated procedure implemented in BESA. For each subject, epochs with a 200 ms baseline and a post trigger (stimulus presentation) window of 800 ms were generated. After baseline correction, a bandpass-filter of 1-40 Hz (Low Cutoff filter: 1 Hz, forward, 6 dB/oct; High Cutoff filter: 40 Hz, zero phase, 24 dB/oct) was applied to the resulting data epochs. Afterwards, data epochs were averaged separately for correct and incorrect trials. In order to transform surface MEG into brain source activity, a source montage with 51 fixed regional dipoles in a homogeneous sphere was defined [69]. In the case of MEG a regional dipole consists of two perpendicular, fixed equivalent current dipoles describing the two tangential components of a source, while the radial component does not contribute to the magnetic field outside the volume conductor. As we were interested in brain activity originating from hippocampal regions, the source montage consisted of 2 regional dipoles (Talairach coordinates: x = ± 42.4; y = -9.5; z = -23.9 for the right/left hemisphere) for assessing activity from the medial temporal lobes and 49 other regional dipoles that represented other brain areas, thus increasing the sensitivity of the fixed sources that had been positioned in the hippocampal region. All dipoles of the source montage were set by hand with fixed location and orientation in similar distance to the head surface to minimize differences in the sources' explanation of variance. In order to explore the time window for further analyses of dipole strength, a grand average of the correct trials of all subjects was generated. The time segment of 130-220 ms after stimulus presentation, containing the peak amplitude of the hippocampal source waveforms in both hemispheres, was chosen for further analyses (Figure 3 and 4). After selecting the time window of interest, dipole analysis was carried out on each participant. By using the source montage in the individual headframe, the mean of the norm of the dipole moment of the hippocampal-associated sources was calculated for the above-mentioned time segment per person and then analyzed statistically.

Figure 3
figure 3

Source waveforms of the hippocampal sources during the transverse patterning task. Source waveforms (grand average of the correct trials over all subjects) of the hippocampal sources for the left and right hemispheres during the transverse patterning task. The marked time segments of 130-220 ms after stimulus presentation were chosen for further analyses.

Figure 4
figure 4

Source waveforms of the hippocampal sources for two age groups found by median split. Source waveforms of the hippocampal sources for the left and right hemispheres during the transverse patterning task plotted separately for the two age groups found by median split (median: 54,5 years; young group: 18-54 years; old group: 55-89 years).

Statistics

Statistical analyses were conducted using R 2.8.1 [R Development Core [70]] and SPSS (SPSS Statistics 17.0). For assessing age effects on test results (cognitive capacity) and behavioral data (number of training trials needed to reach criterion, number of errors, reaction times), linear regressions were conducted with the constant variable age and dependent variables test results and behavioral data, respectively. For examining effects of age and cognitive competence on hippocampal activity, repeated measurement analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) with the factor hemisphere (left, right) and the covariates age and neuropsychological test results were conducted. In order to capture possible nonlinearities, restricted cubic spline transformations of age were investigated [71], but they did not improve model fit as measured by AIC [72] and were discarded.

Results

Neuropsychological test performance

Significant effects of age on cognitive performance were found for several tests. Elderly people performed worse in the digit symbol test, the digit span test, Benton test, Boston naming test, Mini Mental State Examination, word list learning, word list delayed recall, figure recall, trail making test version A and version B. No significant effects of age were found for verbal fluency, figure drawing as well as word list recognition (for details see Table 1).

Table 1 Influence of age on neuropsychological test performance

Behavioral performance during the Transverse Patterning task

There was no age effect on the number of training trials needed to reach criterion or on the number of incorrect trials during MEG measurement. Only reaction times were significantly longer for elderly people (Table 2).

Table 2 Influence of age on behavioral performance in the transverse patterning task

Neuromagnetic data

For the selected time window of 130-220 ms after stimulus presentation, subjects showed a mean source strength of 2.18 nAm (SD = 1.19) for the left and 1.97 nAm (SD = .99) for the right hemisphere. Statistical analyses did not show significant effects of age (F(1,50) = .41; p = .53) (see Figure 5), sex (F(1,50) = .27; p = .61) or hemisphere (F(1,51) = 1.94; p = .17) on the mean amplitudes of the hippocampal sources.

Figure 5
figure 5

Correlation of the average hippocampal activation (nAm) and age (years).

Influence of cognitive competence on brain activity

There were significant relationships between several neuropsychological test results and hippocampal activation (see Table 3). The influence of two verbal memory tests were positive, i.e. higher scores in word list learning (p = .03) and word list delayed recall (p = .04) were associated with higher hippocampal activity (β(WLL) = .08; β(WLDR) = .14). Additionally, higher scores in both versions of the trail making test (which means worse performance) were associated with lower hippocampal activity (p(TMT-A) = .03; β(TMT-A) = -.02; p(TMT-B) = .045; β(TMT-B)= -.01) (Figure 6).

Table 3 Influence of test performance and age on mean amplitude of the hippocampal sources
Figure 6
figure 6

Correlation of hippocampal activation and performance in Trail Making Test - B. Correlation of the average hippocampal activation (nAm) and the performance in the Trail Making Test - B (higher number means worse performance).

Furthermore, there were statistical trends of positive relationships between the digit symbol test and hippocampal activity (p = .08; β(Digit Symbol) = .02) and verbal fluency and hippocampal activity (p = .08; β(VF) = .03). The relationship between the covariates digit span test, Benton test, word list recognition as well as figure recall with hippocampal activity did not reach significance.

Discussion

This study examined whether hippocampal activity during the transverse patterning task varies with age and whether it is also modulated by cognitive competence. Our study yielded two main findings: First, no age effect on hippocampal activity was observed. Second, hippocampal activity was associated with cognitive competence. The strengths and limitations of this study are summarized in table 4.

Table 4 Strengths and limitations of the study

Age-related effects on hippocampal activity

Young and elderly people showed a similar pattern of hippocampal source strength. This is in line with a number of studies that also did not find age-related differences in the extent of hippocampal activation during memory tasks [22, 23, 28, 31–33, 73]. These studies provide evidence of intact hippocampal activation during healthy aging, which is consistent with our findings. Many other studies assessing neuronal activity during memory tasks, however, found either more [20, 29, 30] or less [19, 21, 24–27] activity in the hippocampal region for elderly people. They typically investigated brain activation during memory tasks that activate a wide network of different brain areas and therefore it is not surprising that the results are inconsistent. In this study however, we used a hippocampus specific task and neither did we observe age-related changes in hippocampal current flow nor age-related differences in behavioral performance. At first glance this seems to be contradictory to the age-related deficits in behavioral performance found by Driscoll and colleagues [45]. However, the lack of age-related differences in our study could be explained by differences in experimental setup: Driscoll et al. (2003) used a stepwise approach where subjects had to detect the rules on their own by trial and error which is most likely more difficult than learning the rules by explicit instructions. Given that older adults show longer periods of task acquisition and lower mastery than their younger counterparts with increasing task difficulty [e.g. [74]], it is not surprising that the performance of the elderly subjects was inferior to the performance of the young group in the Driscoll (2003) study. In our study, participants learned the rule by explicit instruction and except for the reaction times, we did not observe age-related deficits in the performance on the transverse patterning task. Since young and elderly performed similarly well, age-related differences in activation of hippocampal-associated components could not necessarily be expected.

Hippocampal activation seems to differ between healthy elderly people and people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or Alzheimer's disease (AD). While we did not find age-related differences in hippocampal activation in our healthy sample, it was reported that people with AD show decreased hippocampal activity [31, 75], whereas people with MCI paradoxically show more hippocampal activation [32, 75]. Less impaired people with MCI show a pattern of hippocampal hyperactivation, whereas more impaired persons demonstrate hippocampal hypoactivation [32]. Miller and colleagues [76] found that greater hippocampal activation during memory tasks in people with MCI serves as a predictor for greater subsequent cognitive decline. Furthermore, they suggested that hippocampal hyperactivation may represent an attempted compensatory response to accumulating neurodegenerative pathology. In addition, Dickerson and colleagues [77] hypothesized that increased hippocampal activation in MCI may serve as a marker for impending clinical decline. In fact, plaques disrupt the synchrony of convergent inputs and thus may lead to an increased number of failures for signal transmission [78]. This in turn may lead to larger rather than smaller PSPs and with it to a smaller intracellular current flow (which produces the MEG signal).

One further explanation for our results could be that we had a cognitively still highly fit sample: there was neither a significant effect of age on the number of training trials needed to reach criterion, nor on the number of errors made during MEG measurement. Furthermore, we merely investigated the correct trials. As could be expected, elderly people responded significantly slower than younger people but besides this, young and elderly people performed similarly well. Therefore, age-related changes in neuronal activation patterns could not necessarily be expected.

Effects of cognitive competence on hippocampal activation

Our data reveal a significant relation between cognitive competence and hippocampal source strength: better performance in tests for verbal memory, executive functioning, verbal fluency, and cognitive speed was associated with higher hippocampal activity, whereas worse performance in executive functions (higher scores in the Trail Making test) was related to lower hippocampal activity. As mentioned above, several studies exist which examine age-related hippocampal activation during different experimental tasks and related performance in these tasks [e.g. [19–24, 27–33, 73]]. However, to our knowledge, this study is the first to examine the correlation between hippocampal activity and independently assessed general cognitive capacity. Results indicate that there might be a more general relationship between medial temporal lobe activity and cognitive performance, with higher general cognitive competence being associated with higher hippocampal activity. This is also in line with the findings from the MCI and AD studies mentioned above [31, 32, 75–77]: the more cognitively impaired the participants were in these studies, the lower was their hippocampal activation. Further studies are needed to clarify and confirm the significance of these findings.

Hippocampal activation in MEG

Results indicate that MEG is a useful method to investigate functional changes in deeper brain structures such as the hippocampus, in line with several earlier studies using similar methods [e.g. [50, 51, 58, 63]]. One limitation of MEG technology, however, is that the detected activity might not have originated from the hippocampus directly but from adjacent temporal lobe structures. However, the transverse patterning task is a well-known paradigm for assessing hippocampal function, which has been demonstrated in many studies (e.g. Astur 1998; Reed and Squire 1999; Hanlon, Weisend et al. 2003; Astur and Constable 2004; Hanlon, Weisend et al. 2005; Meltzer 2007; Moses 2007).

Conclusions

It is generally assumed that age-related morphological changes in the hippocampus result in functional deficits that contribute to cognitive decline. In the present study we employed the hippocampus-dependent transverse patterning task to assess whether hippocampal activity varies with increasing age and whether it is also associated with cognitive competence. To our knowledge, this study is the first to examine the correlation between hippocampal activity and general cognitive capacity in healthy aging. While one might assume that hippocampal activity would decrease with increasing age, we found no such effect in healthy aging. In addition, our results suggest that hippocampal neural activity during the transverse patterning task is associated with cognitive competence. In sum, our results support the notion that in non-pathological aging, hippocampal neural activity does not necessarily decrease with age but is rather related to cognitive competence.

References

  1. Liu RS, Lemieux L, Bell GS, Sisodiya SM, Shorvon SD, Sander JW, Duncan JS: A longitudinal study of brain morphometrics using quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and difference image analysis. Neuroimage. 2003, 20 (1): 22-33. 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00219-2.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Fotenos AF, Snyder AZ, Girton LE, Morris JC, Buckner RL: Normative estimates of cross-sectional and longitudinal brain volume decline in aging and AD. Neurology. 2005, 64 (6): 1032-1039.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Coffey CE, Wilkinson WE, Parashos IA, Soady SA, Sullivan RJ, Patterson LJ, Figiel GS, Webb MC, Spritzer CE, Djang WT: Quantitative cerebral anatomy of the aging human brain: a cross-sectional study using magnetic resonance imaging. Neurology. 1992, 42 (3 Pt 1): 527-536.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Jernigan TL, Press GA, Hesselink JR: Methods for measuring brain morphologic features on magnetic resonance images. Validation and normal aging. Arch Neurol. 1990, 47 (1): 27-32.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Pfefferbaum A, Mathalon DH, Sullivan EV, Rawles JM, Zipursky RB, Lim KO: A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study of changes in brain morphology from infancy to late adulthood. Arch Neurol. 1994, 51 (9): 874-887.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Skullerud K: Variations in the size of the human brain. Influence of age, sex, body length, body mass index, alcoholism, Alzheimer changes, and cerebral atherosclerosis. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl. 1985, 102: 1-94.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Brown J, Cooper-Kuhn CM, Kempermann G, Van Praag H, Winkler J, Gage FH, Kuhn HG: Enriched environment and physical activity stimulate hippocampal but not olfactory bulb neurogenesis. Eur J Neurosci. 2003, 17 (10): 2042-2046. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02647.x.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, Williamson A, Dahle C, Gerstorf D, Acker JD: Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb Cortex. 2005, 15 (11): 1676-1689. 10.1093/cercor/bhi044.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Burke SN, Barnes CA: Neural plasticity in the ageing brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2006, 7 (1): 30-40. 10.1038/nrn1809.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Resnick SM, Pham DL, Kraut MA, Zonderman AB, Davatzikos C: Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: a shrinking brain. J Neurosci. 2003, 23 (8): 3295-3301.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Salat DH, Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Greve DN, Desikan RS, Busa E, Morris JC, Dale AM, Fischl B: Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cereb Cortex. 2004, 14 (7): 721-730. 10.1093/cercor/bhh032.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Sutherland RJ, Rudy JW: Configural association theory: The role of the hippocampal formation in learning, memory, and amnesia. Psychobiology. 1989, 17 (2): 129-144.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Cohen NJ, Eichenbaum H: Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system. 1993, Cambridge: MA US: The MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  14. Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Head D, Kennedy KM, Acker JD: Differential aging of the medial temporal lobe: a study of a five-year change. Neurology. 2004, 62 (3): 433-438.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Acker JD: Hormone replacement therapy and age-related brain shrinkage: regional effects. Neuroreport. 2004, 15 (16): 2531-2534. 10.1097/00001756-200411150-00020.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Raz N, Gunning-Dixon F, Head D, Rodrigue KM, Williamson A, Acker JD: Aging, sexual dimorphism, and hemispheric asymmetry of the cerebral cortex: replicability of regional differences in volume. Neurobiol Aging. 2004, 25 (3): 377-396. 10.1016/S0197-4580(03)00118-0.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Malykhin NV, Bouchard TP, Camicioli R, Coupland NJ: Aging hippocampus and amygdala. Neuroreport. 2008, 19 (5): 543-547. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f8b18c.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Geula C: Abnormalities of neural circuitry in Alzheimer's disease: hippocampus and cortical cholinergic innervation. Neurology. 1998, 51 (1 Suppl 1): S18-29. discussion S65-17

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Murty VP, Sambataro F, Das S, Tan HY, Callicott JH, Goldberg TE, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Weinberger DR, Mattay VS: Age-related Alterations in Simple Declarative Memory and the Effect of Negative Stimulus Valence. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009, 21 (10): 1920-1933. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21130.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Rajah MN, McIntosh AR: Age-related differences in brain activity during verbal recency memory. Brain Res. 2008, 1199: 111-125. 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.12.051.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Dennis NA, Hayes SM, Prince SE, Madden DJ, Huettel SA, Cabeza R: Effects of aging on the neural correlates of successful item and source memory encoding. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2008, 34 (4): 791-808. 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.791.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Morcom AM, Good CD, Frackowiak RS, Rugg MD: Age effects on the neural correlates of successful memory encoding. Brain. 2003, 126 (Pt 1): 213-229. 10.1093/brain/awg020.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Duverne S, Motamedinia S, Rugg MD: The Relationship between Aging, Performance. and the Neural Correlates of Succesful Memory Encoding. Cereb Cortex. 2009, 19 (3): 733-744. 10.1093/cercor/bhn122.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Gutchess AH, Welsh RC, Hedden T, Bangert A, Minear M, Liu LL, Park DC: Aging and the neural correlates of successful picture encoding: frontal activations compensate for decreased medial-temporal activity. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005, 17 (1): 84-96. 10.1162/0898929052880048.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Park DC, Welsh RC, Marshuetz C, Gutchess AH, Mikels J, Polk TA, Noll DC, Taylor SF: Working memory for complex scenes: age differences in frontal and hippocampal activations. J Cogn Neurosci. 2003, 15 (8): 1122-1134. 10.1162/089892903322598094.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Cabeza R, Daselaar SM, Dolcos F, Prince SE, Budde M, Nyberg L: Task-independent and task-specific age effects on brain activity during working memory, visual attention and episodic retrieval. Cereb Cortex. 2004, 14 (4): 364-375. 10.1093/cercor/bhg133.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Dennis NA, Kim H, Cabeza R: Age-related differences in brain activity during true and false memory retrieval. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008, 20 (8): 1390-1402. 10.1162/jocn.2008.20096.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Miller SL, Celone K, DePeau K, Diamond E, Dickerson BC, Rentz D, Pihlajamaki M, Sperling RA: Age-related memory impairment associated with loss of parietal deactivation but preserved hippocampal activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008, 105 (6): 2181-2186. 10.1073/pnas.0706818105.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Restom K, Bangen KJ, Bondi MW, Perthen JE, Liu TT: Cerebral blood flow and BOLD responses to a memory encoding task: a comparison between healthy young and elderly adults. Neuroimage. 2007, 37 (2): 430-439. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.024.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Maguire EA, Frith CD: Aging affects the engagement of the hippocampus during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain. 2003, 126 (Pt 7): 1511-1523. 10.1093/brain/awg157.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Sperling RA, Bates JF, Chua EF, Cocchiarella AJ, Rentz DM, Rosen BR, Schacter DL, Albert MS: fMRI studies of associative encoding in young and elderly controls and mild Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003, 74 (1): 44-50. 10.1136/jnnp.74.1.44.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Celone KA, Calhoun VD, Dickerson BC, Atri A, Chua EF, Miller SL, DePeau K, Rentz DM, Selkoe DJ, Blacker D, et al.: Alterations in memory networks in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: an independent component analysis. J Neurosci. 2006, 26 (40): 10222-10231. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2250-06.2006.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Rand-Giovannetti E, Chua EF, Driscoll AE, Schacter DL, Albert MS, Sperling RA: Hippocampal and neocortical activation during repetitive encoding in older persons. Neurobiol Aging. 2006, 27 (1): 173-182. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.12.013.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Bontempi B, Laurent-Demir C, Destrade C, Jaffard R: Time-dependent reorganization of brain circuitry underlying long-term memory storage. Nature. 1999, 400 (6745): 671-675. 10.1038/23270.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Amidzic O, Riehle HJ, Fehr T, Wienbruch C, Elbert T: Pattern of focal gamma-bursts in chess players. Nature. 2001, 412 (6847): 603-10.1038/35088119.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Spence KW: The nature of the response in discrimination learning. Psychol Rev. 1952, 59 (1): 89-93. 10.1037/h0063067.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Alvarado MC, Rudy JW: Rats with damage to the hippocampal-formation are impaired on the transverse-patterning problem but not on elemental discriminations. Behav Neurosci. 1995, 109 (2): 204-211. 10.1037/0735-7044.109.2.204.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Astur RS: Configural learning in humans: The transverse patterning problem. Psychobiology. 1998, 26 (3): 176-182.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Rickard TC, Grafman J: Losing their configural mind. Amnesic patients fail on transverse patterning. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998, 10 (4): 509-524. 10.1162/089892998562915.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Reed JM, Squire LR: Impaired transverse patterning in human amnesia is a special case of impaired memory for two-choice discrimination tasks. Behav Neurosci. 1999, 113 (1): 3-9. 10.1037/0735-7044.113.1.3.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Rickard TC, Verfaellie M, Grafman J: Transverse patterning and human amnesia. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006, 18 (10): 1723-1733. 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1723.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Alvarado MC: Selective Neurotoxic Damage to the Hippocampal Formation Impairs Performance of the Transverse Patterning and Location Memory in Rhesus Macaques. Hippocampus. 2005, 15 (1): 118-131. 10.1002/hipo.20037.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Alvarado MC, Bachevalier J: Comparison of the effects of damage to the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortex on transverse patterning and location memory in rhesus macaques. J Neurosci. 2005, 25 (6): 1599-1609. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4457-04.2005.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Alvarado MC, Wright AA, Bachevalier J: Object and spatial relational memory in adult rhesus monkeys is impaired by neonatal lesions of the hippocampal formation but not the amygdaloid complex. Hippocampus. 2002, 12 (4): 421-433. 10.1002/hipo.1115.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Driscoll I, Hamilton DA, Petropoulos H, Yeo RA, Brooks WM, Baumgartner RN, Sutherland RJ: The aging hippocampus: cognitive, biochemical and structural findings. Cereb Cortex. 2003, 13 (12): 1344-1351. 10.1093/cercor/bhg081.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Breier JI, Simos PG, Zouridakis G, Papanicolaou AC: Relative timing of neuronal activity in distinct temporal lobe areas during a recognition memory task for words. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1998, 20 (6): 782-790. 10.1076/jcen.20.6.782.1116.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Breier JI, Simos PG, Zouridakis G, Papanicolaou AC: Lateralization of cerebral activation in auditory verbal and non-verbal memory tasks using magnetoencephalography. Brain Topogr. 1999, 12 (2): 89-97. 10.1023/A:1023458110869.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Gonsalves BD, Kahn I, Curran T, Norman KA, Wagner AD: Memory strength and repetition suppression: multimodal imaging of medial temporal cortical contributions to recognition. Neuron. 2005, 47 (5): 751-761. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.07.013.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Hamada Y, Sugino K, Kado H, Suzuki R: Magnetic fields in the human hippocampal area evoked by a somatosensory oddball task. Hippocampus. 2004, 14 (4): 426-433. 10.1002/hipo.10196.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Hanlon FM, Weisend MP, Huang M, Lee RR, Moses SN, Paulson KM, Thoma RJ, Miller GA, Canive JM: A non-invasive method for observing hippocampal function. Neuroreport. 2003, 14 (15): 1957-1960. 10.1097/00001756-200310270-00015.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hanlon FM, Weisend MP, Yeo RA, Huang M, Lee RR, Thoma RJ, Moses SN, Paulson KM, Miller GA, Canive JM: A specific test of hippocampal deficit in schizophrenia. Behav Neurosci. 2005, 119 (4): 863-875. 10.1037/0735-7044.119.4.863.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Ioannides AA, Liu MJ, Liu LC, Bamidis PD, Hellstrand E, Stephan KM: Magnetic field tomography of cortical and deep processes: examples of "real-time mapping" of averaged and single trial MEG signals. Int J Psychophysiol. 1995, 20 (3): 161-175. 10.1016/0167-8760(95)00031-3.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Martin T, McDaniel MA, Guynn MJ, Houck JM, Woodruff CC, Bish JP, Moses SN, Kicic D, Tesche CD: Brain regions and their dynamics in prospective memory retrieval: a MEG study. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007, 64 (3): 247-258. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.010.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Mikuni N, Nagamine T, Ikeda A, Terada K, Taki W, Kimura J, Kikuchi H, Shibasaki H: Simultaneous recording of epileptiform discharges by MEG and subdural electrodes in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuroimage. 1997, 5 (4 Pt 1): 298-306. 10.1006/nimg.1997.0272.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Moses SN, Ryan JD, Bardouille T, Kovacevic N, Hanlon FM, McIntosh AR: Semantic information alters neural activation during transverse patterning performance. Neuroimage. 2009, 46 (3): 863-873. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.042.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Nishitani N, Nagamine T, Fujiwara N, Yazawa S, Shibasaki H: Cortical-hippocampal auditory processing identified by magnetoencephalography. J Cogn Neurosci. 1998, 10 (2): 231-247. 10.1162/089892998562672.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Papanicolaou AC, Simos PG, Castillo EM, Breier JI, Katz JS, Wright AA: The hippocampus and memory of verbal and pictorial material. Learn Mem. 2002, 9 (3): 99-104. 10.1101/lm.44302.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Riggs L, Moses SN, Bardouille T, Herdman AT, Ross B, Ryan JD: A complementary analytic approach to examining medial temporal lobe sources using magnetoencephalography. Neuroimage. 2009, 45 (2): 627-642. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.018.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Stephen JM, Ranken DM, Aine CJ, Weisend MP, Shih JJ: Differentiability of simulated MEG hippocampal, medial temporal and neocortical temporal epileptic spike activity. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2005, 22 (6): 388-401.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Tendolkar I, Rugg M, Fell J, Vogt H, Scholz M, Hinrichs H, Heinze HJ: A magnetoencephalographic study of brain activity related to recognition memory in healthy young human subjects. Neurosci Lett. 2000, 280 (1): 69-72. 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)01001-0.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Tesche CD: Non-invasive detection of ongoing neuronal population activity in normal human hippocampus. Brain Res. 1997, 749 (1): 53-60. 10.1016/S0006-8993(96)01286-3.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Tesche CD, Karhu J: Theta oscillations index human hippocampal activation during a working memory task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000, 97 (2): 919-924. 10.1073/pnas.97.2.919.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Moses SN, Ryan JD, Bardouille T, Kovacevic N, Hanlon FM, McIntosh AR: Semantic information alters neural activation during transverse patterning performance. Neuroimage. 2009, 46 (3): 863-873. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.042.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Ackenheil M, Stotz G, Dietz-Bauer R, Vossen AI: M.I.N.I. 5.0.0. German version/DSM-IV. 1999, Munich: Psychiatrische Universitätsklinik

    Google Scholar 

  65. Oldfield R: The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971, 9 (1): 97-113. 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Morris JC, Mohs RC, Rogers H, Fillenbaum G, Heyman A: Consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease (CERAD) clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1988, 24 (4): 641-652.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Tewes U: Hamburg-Wechsler-Intelligenztest für Erwachsene (HAWIE-R). 1991, Bern: Verlag Hans Huber

    Google Scholar 

  68. Steck PH: A revision of A. L. Benton's Visual Retention Test (BVRT) in two parallel forms. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2005, 20: 409-416. 10.1016/j.acn.2004.09.009.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Scherg M, Bast T, Hoechstetter K, Ille N, Weckesser D, Bornfleth H, Berg P: Brain source montages improve the non-invasive diagnosis in epilepsy. International Congress Series. 2004, 1270: 15-19. 10.1016/j.ics.2004.04.035.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Team RDC: R: A language and environment for statistical computing (version 2.8.1). 2008, Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statisitcal Computing

    Google Scholar 

  71. Harrell FE: Regression Modelling Strategies. 2001, New York: Springer

    Google Scholar 

  72. Akaike H: Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle. 2nd International Symposium on Information Theory: 1973; Akademiai Kiado. Edited by: Petrov BN, Caski S. 1973, 267-281.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Abram K, Teplin L, Charles D, Longworth S, McClelland G, Dulcan M: Posttraumatic stress disorder and trauma in youth in juvenile detention. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004, 61 (4): 403-410. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.403.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Fisk AD, Rogers WA, Cooper BP, Gilbert DK: Automatic category search and its transfer: aging, type of search, and level of learning. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1997, 52 (2): P91-102.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Sperling R: Functional MRI studies of associative encoding in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007, 1097: 146-155. 10.1196/annals.1379.009.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Miller SL, Fenstermacher E, Bates J, Blacker D, Sperling RA, Dickerson BC: Hippocampal activation in adults with mild cognitive impairment predicts subsequent cognitive decline. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008, 79 (6): 630-635. 10.1136/jnnp.2007.124149.

    PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Dickerson BC, Salat DH, Bates JF, Atiya M, Killiany RJ, Greve DN, Dale AM, Stern CE, Blacker D, Albert MS, et al.: Medial temporal lobe function and structure in mild cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol. 2004, 56 (1): 27-35. 10.1002/ana.20163.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Stern EA, Bacskai BJ, Hickey GA, Attenello FJ, Lombardo JA, Hyman BT: Cortical synaptic integration in vivo is disrupted by amyloid-beta plaques. J Neurosci. 2004, 24 (19): 4535-4540. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0462-04.2004.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This study was funded by a grant of the Heidelberg Academy of Sciences awarded to ITK, and a PhD fellowship of the German National Academic Foundation awarded to VML. The authors thank Johanna Fiess, Christiane Wolf and Ursula Lommen for assistance in data collection as well as Patrick Berg for support in MEG data analysis.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Vera M Leirer.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

VML, ITK and TE designed the experiment. VML collected the data and performed the MEG data analysis with support of CW and IPJ. SK performed the statistical analyses. VML drafted the manuscript and ITK, CW, SK, IPJ as well as TE revised it critically. All authors commented on and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

12868_2009_1672_MOESM1_ESM.DOC

Additional file 1: Sample description. The table illustrates each volunteer's age, sex and associated diseases. (DOC 72 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Leirer, V.M., Wienbruch, C., Paul-Jordanov, I. et al. Hippocampal activity during the transverse patterning task declines with cognitive competence but not with age. BMC Neurosci 11, 113 (2010). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2202-11-113

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2202-11-113

Keywords