Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Covariates of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) from healthy humans

Abstract

Background

Define covariates of cerebrospinal corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) levels in normal humans. CRHCSF was measured in 9 normal subjects as part of an intensive study of physiological responses stressors in chronic pain and fatigue states. CRHCSF was first correlated with demographic, vital sign, HPA axis, validated questionnaire domains, baseline and maximal responses to pain, exercise and other stressors. Significant factors were used for linear regression modeling.

Results

Highly significant correlations were found despite the small number of subjects. Three models were defined: (a) CRHCSF with blood glucose and sodium (explained variance = 0.979, adjusted R2 = 0.958, p = 0.02 by 2-tailed testing); (b) CRHCSF with resting respiratory and heart rates (R2 = 0.963, adjusted R2 = 0.939, p = 0.007); and (c) CRHCSF with SF-36 Vitality and Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory Physical Fatigue domains (R2 = 0.859, adjusted R2 = 0.789, p = 0.02).

Conclusions

Low CRHCSF was predicted by lower glucose, respiratory and heart rates, and higher sodium and psychometric constructs of well being. Responses at peak exercise and to other acute stressors were not correlated. CRHCSF may have reflected an overall, or chronic, set-point for physiological responses, but did not predict the reserves available to respond to immediate stressors.

Background

Corticotropin – releasing hormone (CRH) plays a major role in regulating the hypothalamic – pituitary – adrenal (HPA) axis, acute responses to stressors, and other neurological functions [1]. The cerebrospinal concentrations of CRH (CRHCSF) and neuropeptide Y (NPY), another important neuropeptide involved in pain, autonomic and stress responses [2, 3], were measured in 9 normal humans to better understand these functions. Subjects were studied as part of a large scale investigation of subjective and objective (functional magnetic resonance imaging) responses to pain testing, exercise, and other stressors [4]. Subjects completed questionnaires that assessed pain, anxiety, depression, coping skills, and other psychometric variables. Normal control, fibromyalgia, [5], chronic fatigue syndrome [6], and veterans of the first Persian Gulf War [7] were included. This pilot investigation focuses on the statistical strategy used to analyze the control population (n = 9), and to develop methods for effective evaluation of patient populations. Three multiple linear regression models [8] were defined that predicted CRHCSF based on (a) metabolic, (b) autonomic, and (c) psychometric measures.

Results

Demographics

The mean age for the group was 35.2 yr (95% C.I.: 30.5 to 39.9) years. There were 2 females, 5 African-Americans, 3 Caucasians, and 1 Caucasian Hispanic (TABLE 1). These subjects were a very normal and healthy group based upon their histories and the strict exclusion criteria.

Table 1 Physiological data for each individual stratified by CRHCSF

NPYCSF

Mean NPYCSF was 121.9 (87.8 to 156.0) pg/ml. NPY did not correlate with any variable.

Variables correlated with CRHCSF

Significant covariates of CRHCSF could be grouped into: metabolic, autonomic function, and perceptional and cognitive functions (TABLE 3). Serum glucose was positively, and sodium negatively, correlated with CRHCSF (FIGURE 1). Explained variances (R2) were 0.97 and 0.66, respectively. Glucose and sodium were also negatively correlated (R2 = 0.85). Resting norepinephrine levels at 2 time points and heart rate at 4 time points were colinear and positively correlated to CRHCSF.

Table 3 Significant covariates of CFHCSF in normal subjects.
Figure 1
figure 1

Relationships between CRHCSF, plasma glucose, and serum sodium. Data from single individuals are shown with identical symbols (e.g. solid black squares on left). Open circles indicate sodium without corresponding glucose values. The linear regression lines for CRHCSF with sodium (top line) and glucose (bottom line) gave explained variances (r2) of 0.66 and 0.97, respectively.

CRHCSF was negatively correlated with the Holter monitor-derived measure of log heart rate summed for the daytime, and the threshold temperature causing an initial, mild sensation of burning pain (Stressor I). The latter indicated that subjects with higher CRHCSF perceived the burning pain of the cutaneous forearm hyperthermic stimulation at a lower threshold temperature than their peers. This may indicate an increased sensitivity to nociceptive stimuli. However, there was no correlation with deep pressure – induced pain. Negative correlations were also found with the SF-36 Vitality and SES Manage Symptoms domains. High scores were normal for these questionnaires, with lower scores indicating dysfunction.

Effects in males

All parts of the study were completed by at least 5 males. Analysis of the male subgroup gave some information about the role of gender. The pattern of significant covariables was different from the total group (TABLE 4). Respiratory rate was the only vital or physiological sign related to CRHCSF. Perceptions of vulnerability, physical functioning and self-efficacy were more highly related.

Table 4 Significant covariates of CFHCSF and relevant statistics in males.

Statistical models

Sets of the significant metabolic, autonomic, and perceptual variables were grouped and analyzed by 3 linear regression models in order to detect significant relationships between independent variables and CRHCSF. The 3 models had high significance levels (TABLE 5). A metabolic model related CRHCSF to glucose and sodium. An autonomic model linked CRHCSF to resting respiratory and heart rates. The optimum perceptual model predicted CRHCSF based on SF-36 Vitality and MFI Physical Fatigue. The latter 2 domains were not the same as those in TABLES 3 and 4 because many of these variables were co-linear or surrogates of one another. This was reinforced by the similarity of R2 and p values where these domains were substituted for Vitality and Physical Fatigue. These results were remarkable because it is very unusual for models with such small numbers of observations (n ≤ 9) to be so significant (p < 0.02). The explained variances were very high (R2 > 0.85) suggesting that the factors may be causally connected.

Table 5 Linear regression models of CRHCSF

CRHCSF did not correlate with variables associated with maximum exercise, heat – and pressure – (dolorimetry) induced pain, or other rapid onset stressors (TABLE 1).

Discussion

Despite the small numbers of normal subjects in this pilot investigation, the data, the explained variances for relationships, and final 3 models predictive of CRHCSF were highly significant. The models were of importance, since they reflected the specific neurological functions of this neurohormone. CRH and NPY were co-expressed in the hypothalamus [2], but NPYCSF did not correlate with CRHCSF or any other variable.

CRH and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis maintain numerous systemic functions. Our metabolic model showed a tightly correlated relationship between 2 pm serum glucose, sodium and CRHCSF. Relatively higher CRHCSF levels were associated with elevated serum glucose levels. When glucose is elevated, it is pumped into cells along with sodium ions [9]. In our model, this may have been reflected by the reduced serum sodium concentrations (FIGURE 1). These measurements were taken at different times, suggesting that the CRHCSF set a long-term operating range for this system. The variables were interrelated. CRHCSF in the low normal range was inferred from a low glucose and relatively high sodium. Other reports also suggest a role of CRH and energy balance [10].

Neuroendocrine responses such as these rely solely on CRH type 1 (CRH1) receptors and the HPA axis [11]. The other CRH receptor gene, CRH2, has 3 splice variants (α, β and γ) but only CRH2α is expressed in the brain. CRH1 and CRH2α receptors have nonoverlapping distributions, but mediate many similar defensive behaviors suggesting that they act in parallel neural circuits. Different stressors may act by separate circuits and have distinct feedback and control systems [11–13]. CRH1 receptors in the central nucleus of the amygdala may participate in conditioned fear responses [12]. These CRH neurons may project to the hippocampus and CRH1 receptors in the locus ceruleus to induce defensive behaviors and autonomic reflexes [13, 14]. Dorsal raphe nucleus neurons may release CRH that acts on inhibitory CRH2α receptors in the lateral septum [15]. Neurons from the lateral septum tonically inhibit periaqueductal grey regions that induce similar defensive behaviors. These nuclei are probably additional sources of CRH in the CSF.

Resting respiratory and pre-exercise heart rates and CRHCSF were positively correlated. The statistical model indicated that a low CRHCSF was predicted by low respiratory and heart rates. Respiratory and cardiac functions are rigorously controlled by brainstem and other nuclei that integrate incoming signals of plasma O2, CO2 and H+ concentrations, activity needs, anxiety and other stressors. Efferent cardiovascular and other autonomic reflexes are modulated by CRH in man [16]. These central nervous system effects may be due to, or highly correlated with, CRHCSF. This is supported by studies in mice that genetically overexpress CRH. They develop chronic stress – like autonomic and physiological alterations [17]. Stressors acting via conditioned fear responses may involve CRH1 receptors in the central nucleus of the amygdala. Some of these CRH neurons project to locus ceruleus neurons [17] that activate autonomic reflexes and defensive behaviors such as "freezing" (immobility) in rodents [14]. An example of this valuable defense would be the freezing of prey in the presence of a predator. Immobility would allow the prey's camouflage to blend into the surroundings without generating motion – induced visual cues for the predator. In humans, excessive, aberrant or dysregulated manifestions of defense behaviors such as freezing may contribute to the immobility, inertia, or even catatonia that contribute to the clinical picture of depression [1].

Extrapolation of these concepts suggests that elevated CRH may be related to anxiety, depression, or other disorders associated with chronic stress responses. If so, then lower, but normal, CRHCSF should be present in persons lacking these stressor states. This was supported by the negative correlations of CRHCSF with scores for the SF-36 Vitality and Change in Health, Self Efficacy Scale Manage Symptoms, MIQ Vulnerability, and MFI Physical Functioning domains. Each scale has an idealized "normal" end of the range of scores. Deviation towards either higher (MFI) or lower (SF-36) ends of the scales provides an estimate of dysfunction. For each of these domains, the lower CRHCSF were associated with more normal scores, while higher CRHCSF was associated with scores that were beginning to shift away from the normal pole of each scale. These trends were further supported by the statistical model where the optimum covariates of CRHCSF were SF-36 Vitality and MFI Physical Fatigue domains. The model predicted that CRHCSF would be in the low normal range when Vitality and Physical Fatigue domain scores were high (normal). Taken together, these results confirm the consistent physical status, mental coping skills, and general health of these subjects.

Studies of intraventricular CRH injection in primates support our findings [33]. The CRH diffused to brain regions that led to 3 types of behavioral changes. Externally oriented behaviors such as locomotion and environmental exploration were significantly decreased. Anxiety – related self – clasping was increased. Depression – like behaviors of avoidance of social contact, huddling, slouching, and wall facing were seen only in social settings. There were high interindividual differences in responses, but a key element was the social context in this study. This social context is lacking in rodent studies where animals are typically studied in isolation.

Conclusion

This small but intensively studied group of normal humans demonstrated surprisingly robust relationships between CRHCSF and metabolic, autonomic, and psychometric measures. These are novel findings in humans, but are consistent with data on CRH in acute and chronic stress models, and proposed CRH neural circuits. It will now be of great interest to contrast these statistical models identified for normal subjects with the other chronic pain and fatigue patient subsets to determine if CRHCSF correlates with different sets of variables.

Methods

Subjects

Nine normal, healthy subjects (2 females) gave informed consent for this paid, Institutional Review Board – approved protocol. They had a comprehensive screening evaluation to exclude: severe physical impairment, morbid obesity, autoimmune/inflammatory diseases, cardiopulmonary disorders, uncontrolled endocrine or allergic disorders, malignancy, severe psychiatric illnesses (e.g., schizophrenia, substance abuse), factors known to affect the HPA axis or autonomic function (cigarette smoking, daily intake of caffeine exceeding the equivalent of 2 cups of coffee), or medication use. Subjects had a history and physical examination, were administered the Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) (SCID II) [19, 20], Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) [21, 22] and Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale (CESD) [23] to detect psychiatric co-morbidities, and completed the self – report Short Form 36 (SF-36) [24, 25], Self Efficacy Scale (SES) [26], Meaning of Illness Questionnaire (MIQ) [27], and Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) [28].

Study protocol

Subjects were admitted to the G-CRC on the evening of Day 1. An 18-gauge catheter was inserted antecubitally and infused with normal saline at 50 ml/hr. A Holter monitor was attached to monitor autonomic regulation of cardiac rhythm [29–31]. Upon awakening, or at 6:30 am, subjects stayed in bed until they had "pre-awakening" blood tests drawn. Breakfast was served between 7:00 and 8:00 a.m. and baseline Day 2 blood samples drawn at 8:30 a.m. Vital signs, questionnaire responses, and other blood samples were drawn from the catheter to assess HPA axis and stress system responses before, during and after Day 2 stressors. Pain responses were tested by pressure applied to each subject's thumbnail and heat to the forearm applied in random staircase testing paradigms (Stressor I) [32, 33]. Stressor II was a series of cognitive challenges including the Benton Visual Retention Test of visual-spatial memory; Digit Span recitation; and Pig Latin, a test of verbal working memory [34]. Lunch was served, followed by a 2 to 2.5 hours rest break to allow post-prandial catecholamine levels to reequilibrate. At 2 pm, serum glucose, electrolytes, plasma catecholamines and other analytes were measured. Subjects then squeezed an isometric hand grip dynamometer to test autonomic function and muscular fatigue [35] (Jamar, Sammons Preston, Bollingbrook, IL) (Stressor III). After 30 min of rest, they had a sub-maximal exercise test on an electronically-braked cycle ergometer (Sensormedics, Yorba Linda, CA). The test was graded in 3 min stages and ended when the subjects' heart rates reached 85% of their age-predicted maximum (Stressor IV). Lumbar punctures were performed 30 min later (approximately 4 pm) (Stressor V). Subjects sat while sterile technique was used to prepare the skin over the L4–L5 lumbar region, infiltrate the subcutaneous and deep tissues with 2% lidocaine, and insert a 22G spinal catheter. CSF was collected as 3 to 4 aliquots of about 2 ml each. Catheters were withdrawn and subjects allowed to rest in their preferred position for 30 min.

Lumbar punctures and CRH and NPY radioimmunoassays (RIA)

Tubes of CSF were immediately placed on ice and then centrifuged at 4°C. The supernatants were rapidly frozen at -80°C. Tube 2 or 3 was removed from the freezer and thawed at 4°C. Peptides were extracted by precipitating high molecular weight proteins by adding an equal volume of 100% ethanol, 0.1 M acetic acid, 0.2% sodium bisulfite [36, 37]. The supernatant was dried (SpeedVac, Thermo Savant, Holbrook, NY), resuspended in phosphate buffered saline with 1% bovine serum albumin (RIA buffer; Peninsula Laboratories, Inc., San Carlos, CA.). Samples and standard amounts of each peptide were aliquoted and peptide specific rabbit antibodies added. After overnight incubation at 4°C, I125-CRH or -NPY was added, tubes gently vortexed, and again incubated overnight. Goat anti-rabbit antibodies were added, tubes incubated, and immune complexes precipitated by centrifugation. Radioactivity was counted for the standards, and the concentrations for each sample interpolated from the standard curves. Spiking CSF with fixed amounts of I125-peptides and performing the RIA shifted the curves to the left by the anticipated concentrations. Standard curves were reproducible to within 10%.

Plasma catecholamine levels were measured by HPLC (Mayo Clinic Laboratories, Rochester, MN).

Statistical methods

All the data from the provocation studies, questionnaire domains, blood work, and neuropeptide analysis were entered into a SAS spreadsheet (SAS, Carey, NC) using sequential hand or scanner entry followed by data checking routines.

Means with 95% confidence intervals were reported.

Simple correlations between our outcome measure (CRHCSF) and other objective and subjective patient variables were conducted as an exploratory tool. Given the small sample size (n = 9), both parametric and non-parametric correlations were used to evaluate the robustness of the correlation coefficients. Partial and intra-class correlations examined the structure of the relationships between CRHCSF and the independent variables. Original data were always checked to make sure that correlations were not spurious, due to outliers, colinearity, or sets of virtually identical scores.

Our data included a very wide range of covariates which spanned the physical and psychological attributes of the patients. Scatter plots were used to determine the response slopes with CRHCSF. Independent variables with flat slopes or high scatter that did not generate correlations with CRHCSF, that were highly correlated with each other (collinear), or that had < 5 recorded values were excluded from further analysis.

The remaining suitable independent variables fell into 3 categories: metabolic, autonomic and psychometric function. Three 3 separate linear regression models [8] were constructed to predict CRHCSF, the dependent variable. Separate models were required because of the low degrees of freedom available for the analyses, and to ensure that the explained variance (R2) was not inflated due to overloading the model with potentially collinear variables. Variance inflation factors and tolerance values were incorporated to optimize the selection of the most independent combinations of variables that also maximized the R2 and p values. The linear regression procedure took into account the multiple comparisons for each model.

Abbreviations

CESD:

Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale

CFS:

Chronic Fatigue Syndrome

CIDI:

Composite International Diagnostic Interview

CRH:

corticotropin-releasing hormone

CSF:

cerebrospinal fluid

MFI:

Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory

MIQ:

Meaning of Illness Questionnaire

NPY:

neuropeptide Y

R2:

explained variance

SCID II:

Structured Clinical Interview for the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV)

SF-36:

Short Form 36

SES:

Self Efficacy Scale

References

  1. Bakshi VP, Kalin NH: Animal models and endophenotypes of anxiety and stress disorders:. In: Neuropsychopharmacology: 5th Generation of Progress. Edited by: David KL, Charney D, Coyle JT, Nemeroff C. 2002, Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, New York, 883-900.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Campbell RE, Grove KL, Smith MS: Distribution of corticotropin releasing hormone receptor immunoreactivity in the rat hypothalamus: coexpression in neuropeptide Y and dopamine neurons in the arcuate nucleus. Brain Res. 2003, 973: 223-32. 10.1016/S0006-8993(03)02487-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Lin Q, Zou X, Ren Y, Wang J, Fang L, Willis WD: Involvement of peripheral neuropeptide Y receptors in sympathetic modulation of acute cutaneous flare induced by intradermal capsaicin. Neuroscience. 2004, 123: 337-347. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.09.017.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Gracely RH, Geisser ME, Geisecke T, Grant MA, Petzke F, Williams DA, Clauw DJ: Pain castastrophizing and neural responses to pain among persons with fibromyalgia. Brain. 2004, 127: 835-843. 10.1093/brain/awh098. Epub 20004 Feb 11

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Wolfe F, Smythe HA, Yunus MB, Bennett RM, Bombardier C, Goldenberg DL, Tugwell P, Campbell SM, Abeles M, Clark P: The American College of Rheumatology 1990 Criteria for the Classification of Fibromyalgia. Report of the Multicenter Criteria Committee. Arthritis Rheum. 1990, 33: 160-172.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fukuda K, Straus SE, Hickie I, Sharpe MC, Dobbins JG, Komaroff A: The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach to its definition and study. International Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group. Ann Intern Med. 1994, 121: 953-959.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Fukuda K, Nisenbaum R, Stewart G, Thompson WT, Robin L, Washko RM: Chronic mutlisymptom illness affecting air force veterans of the gulf war. JAMA. 1999, 280: 981-988. 10.1001/jama.280.11.981.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Petrie A, Sabine C: Medical Statistics at a Glance. 2000, Oxford. Blackwell Science, 75-77.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Koepsell H, Spangenberg J: Function and presumed molecular structure of Na+ -D-glucose cotransport systems. J Membr Biol. 1994, 138: 1-11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Fulton S, Richard D, Woodside B, Shizgal P: Interaction of CRH and energy balance in the modulation of brain stimulation reward. Behav Neurosci. 2002, 116: 651-659. 10.1037//0735-7044.116.4.651.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Pelleymounter MA, Joppa M, Ling N, Foster AC: Pharmacological evidence supportinga role for central corticotropin-releasing factor(2) receptors in behavioral, but not endocrine, response to environmental stress. J Pharmacol Exp Therap. 2002, 302: 145-152. 10.1124/jpet.302.1.145.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Hand GA, Hewitt CB, Fulk LJ, Stock HS, Carson JA, Davis JM, Wilson MA: Differential release of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) in the amygdala during different types of stressors. Brain Res. 2002, 949: 122-130. 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02972-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Makino S, Hashimoto K, Gold PW: Multiple feedback mechanisms activating corticotropin-releasing hormone system in the brain during stress. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002, 73: 147-158. 10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00791-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Swiergiel AH: Effects of infusion of corticotropin-releasing factor antagonist into the locus coeruleus on freezing behavior and brain catecholamines in rats. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Warsz). 2003, 63: 9-16.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Bakshi VP, Smith-Roe S, Newman SM, Grigoriadis DE, Kalin NH: Reduction of stress-induced behavior by antagonism of corticotropin-releasing hormone 2 (CRH2) receptors in lateral septum or CRH1 receptors in amygdala. J Neurosci. 2002, 22: 2926-2935.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Pongratz G, Zietz B, Gluck T, Scholmerich J, Straub RH: Corticotropin-releasing factor modulates cardiovascular and pupillary autonomic reflexes in man: is there a link to inflammation-induced autonomic nervous hyperreflexia?. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002, 966: 373-383.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Dirks A, Groenink L, Bouwknecht JA, Hijzen TH, Van Der Gugten J, Ronken E, Verbeek JS, Veening JG, Dederen PJ, Korosi A, Schoolderman LF, Roubos EW, Olivier B: Overexpression of corticotropin-releasing hormone in transgenic mice and chronic stress-like autonomic and physiological alterations. Eur J Neurosci. 2002, 16: 1751-1760. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.02245.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Strome EM, Wheler GH, Higley JD, Loriaux DL, Suomi SJ, Doudet DJ: Intracerebroventricular corticotropin-releasing factor increases limbic glucose metabolism and has social context-dependent behavioral effects in nonhuman primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002, 99: 15749-15754. 10.1073/pnas.232480899.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Nelson-Gray RO: DSM-IV: empirical guidelines from psychometrics. J Abnorm Psychol. 1991, 100: 308-315. 10.1037//0021-843X.100.3.308.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV Axis I disorders-patient edition (SCID-I/P, Version 2.0). 1996, New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute

    Google Scholar 

  21. Rosenman SJ, Korten AE, Levings CT: Computerised diagnosis in acute psychiatry: validity of CIDI-Auto against routine clinical diagnosis. J Psychiatr Res. 1997, 31: 581-592. 10.1016/S0022-3956(97)00032-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Thornton C, Russell J, Hudson J: Does the Composite International Diagnostic Interview underdiagnose the eating disorders?. Int J Eat Disord. 1998, 23: 341-345. 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199804)23:3<341::AID-EAT11>3.0.CO;2-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Cherpitel CJ: Analysis of cut points for screening instruments for alcohol problems in the emergency room. J Stud Alcohol. 1995, 56: 695-700.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Ware JE, Kosinski M, Keller SD: Physical and mental health summary scales: a user's manual. 1994, Boston: The Health Institute

    Google Scholar 

  25. Ware JE, Sherbourne CD: The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I. Conceptual framework and item selection. Med Care. 1995, 30: 473-483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Lorig K, Chastain RL, Ung E, Shoor S, Holman HR: Development and evaluation of a scale to measure perceived self-efficacy in people with arthritis. Arthritis Rheum. 1989, 32: 37-44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Weir R, Browne G, Roberts J, Tunks E, Gafni A: The Meaning of Illness Questionnaire: further evidence for its reliability and validity. Pain. 1994, 58: 377-386. 10.1016/0304-3959(94)90132-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Smets EM, Garssen B, Bonke B, De Haes JC: The Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI) psychometric qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. J Psychosom Res. 1995, 39: 315-325. 10.1016/0022-3999(94)00125-O.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology: Heart rate variablility Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation. 1996, 93: 1043-1065.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Berntson GG, Bigger JT, Eckberg DL, Grossman P, Kaufmann PG, Malik M, Nagaraja HN, Porges SW, Saul JP, Stone PH, van der Molen MW: Heart rate variability: origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology. 1997, 34: 623-648.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Stein PK: Assessing heart rate variability from real-world Holter reports. Card Electrophysiol Rev. 2002, 6: 239-244. 10.1023/A:1016376924850.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Gracely RH, Lota L, Walter DJ, Dubner R: A multiple random staircase method of psychophysical pain assessment. Pain. 1988, 32: 55-63. 10.1016/0304-3959(88)90023-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Gracely RH, Petzke F, Park K, Grant M, Clauw DJ: Functional MRI evidence of augmented pain processing in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 2002, 46: 1333-1343. 10.1002/art.10225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Amieva H, Letenneur L, Dartigues JF, Rouch-Leroyer I, Sourgen C, D"Alchee-Biree F, Dib M, Barberger-Gateau P, Orgogozo JM, Fabrigoule C: Annual rate and predictors of conversion to dementia in subjects presenting mild cognitive impairment criteria defined according to a population-based study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004, 18: 87-93. 10.1159/000077815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Khurana RK, Setty A: The value of the isometric hand-grip test – studies in various autonomic disorders. Clin Auton Res. 1996, 6: 211-218.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Baraniuk JN, Castellino S, Goff J, Lundgren JD, Mullol J, Merida M, Shelhamer JH, Kaliner MA: Neuropeptide Y (NPY) in human nasal mucosa. Am J Respir Cell Mol Biol. 1990, 3: 165-173.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Liu Z, Welin M, Bragee B, Nyberg F: A high-recovery extraction procedure for quantitative analysis of substance P and opioid peptides in human cerebrospinal fluid. Peptides. 2000, 21: 853-860. 10.1016/S0196-9781(00)00219-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Supported by Department of Defense Award DAMD 170020018, Public Health Service Award RO1 AI42403, and 1 M01-RR13297-01A1 from the General Clinical Research Center Program of the National Center for Research Resources, National Institutes of Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James N Baraniuk.

Additional information

Authors' contributions

JNB directed the RIA studies, correlation of clinical variables, and wrote the manuscript. HM confirmed the accuracy of the study results by double data entry methods, vetted the SAS database, performed the statistical analysis, and co-wrote the manuscript. GW performed the CRH and NPY assays and maintained the repository of these samples. DJC was the Principal Investigator for the overall study and helped review the draft and final manuscript.

Table 2 Psychometric data for each individual stratified by CRHCSF

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Baraniuk, J.N., Maibach, H., Whalen, G. et al. Covariates of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) from healthy humans. BMC Neurosci 5, 58 (2004). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2202-5-58

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2202-5-58

Keywords