Skip to main content

Neuroprotective effects of magnesium l-threonate in a hypoxic zebrafish model

Abstract

Background

Hypoxia inhibits the uptake of glutamate (a major neurotransmitter in the brain closely related to cognitive function) into brain cells, and the initial response of cells to cortical hypoxia depends on glutamate. Previous studies have suggested that magnesium may have protective effects against hypoxic injuries. In particular, magnesium l-threonate (MgT) may increase magnesium ion concentrations in the brain better than MgSO4 and improve cognitive function.

Methods

We evaluated cell viability under hypoxic conditions in the MgT- and MgSO4-treated human SH-SY5Y neurons, in vivo behavior using the T-maze test following hypoxia in MgT-treated zebrafish, activity of brain mitochondrial dehydrogenase by 2,3,5-triphenyltetrazolium chloride (TTC) staining, and protein expression of the excitatory amino acid transporter (EAAT) 4 glutamate transporter by western blotting.

Results

Among the groups treated with hypoxia, cell viability significantly increased when pre-treated with 1 or 10 mM MgT (p = 0.009 and 0.026, respectively). Despite hypoxic insult, MgT-treated zebrafish showed preferences for the red compartment (p = 0.025 for distance and p = 0.007 for frequency of entries), suggesting memory preservation. TTC staining showed reduced cerebral infarction and preserved absorbance in the MgT-treated zebrafish brain after hypoxia (p = 0.010 compared to the hypoxia group). In addition, western blot showed upregulation of EAAT4 protein in the MgT treated group.

Conclusions

Pre-treatment with MgT attenuated cell death and cerebral infarction due to hypoxia and protected cognitive function in zebrafish. In addition, MgT appeared to modulate expression of the glutamate transporter, EAAT4.

Background

With a growing need to preserve cognitive function in an aging population, various drugs that can improve memory are being studied [1,2,3]. However, there is limited evidence of efficacy and limited indications for use of cognition-enhancing drugs including psychostimulants and glutamate activators. Misuse of such drugs may cause side effects and complications as well as social problems [3]. Therefore, it is necessary to establish more in-depth knowledge and scientific evidence to support the clinical applications of such drugs.

Hypoxic insults may result in rapid, irreversible, ischemic damage to neurons [4, 5], and a variety of functional deficits including cognitive impairments. Hypoxia is known to inhibit the migration of glutamate (one of the main neurotransmitters in the brain, closely related to cognitive function) into brain cells [1, 6, 7]. Previous studies confirmed that the initial response of cells to cortical hypoxia depends on glutamate [8]. Excitatory amino acid transporter 4 (EAAT4), a glutamate receptor, shows decreased immunoreactivity after hypoxic-ischemic damage [9].

Several studies have shown that magnesium sulfate (MgSO4) reduces levels of reactive oxygen species and inflammation following hypoxic injuries [10]. However, the increase in magnesium ion levels in the cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) following MgSO4 administration is limited by central nervous system regulation and the blood–brain barrier. Even in the case of a 150–200% increase in plasma magnesium concentrations in humans, that in the CSF increases by only 10–19% [11]. The relatively newly developed magnesium l-threonate (MgT) consists of a magnesium ion and threonate, which exists physiologically in the brain [12]. Unlike other magnesium compounds, MgT significantly increases magnesium ion levels in the CSF [13]. Previous studies have suggested several neuroprotective mechanisms of MgT in the rat using Alzheimer’s disease and neuropathy models [14,15,16]. Wei et al. showed that MgT prevented the reduction in glutamatergic synaptic transmission under Alzheimer’s disease-like pathological conditions [16]. We expected that their findings would be similarly applicable to the hypoxic model.

To assess the effects of magnesium on cognitive function, we used a hypoxic zebrafish model. Zebrafish demonstrate a 70% genetic similarity to human protein coding genes [17] and an 84% similarity in disease-related genes [18]; moreover, the zebrafish neurotransmitter system is similar to that of mammals [19]. This model has recently become regarded as an ideal vertebrate model that has a competitive edge in terms of time and cost for large-scale drug toxicity screening studies [20]. In our previous study, zebrafish were found to be useful in the evaluation of cognitive function including learning and memory [21].

In this study, we investigated whether MgT is associated with neuroprotection and improvement in cognitive function using a hypoxic zebrafish model. We hypothesized that magnesium would prevent hypoxia-induced cognitive dysfunction, decrease infarcted brain area, and upregulate the glutamate transporter, EAAT4. Further, we predicted that glutamate would have a role in the mechanism underlying MgT-induced neuroprotection.

Methods

Experimental animal

Adult zebrafish (4–6 months old, 2.5–3.5 cm long, 350 ± 50 mg of weight, wild type, Danio rerio) were used in the study. Animals were fed brine shrimp twice a day in a 28.5 °C tank, and were on a 14-h daytime and 10-h night cycle. The watercraft was equipped with a multi-stage filtration system with a sediment filter, post-carbon filter, fluorescent UV light, and sterilization filter (Zebrafish AutoSystem, Genomic Design, Daejeon, Korea). All procedures were reviewed and approved by the Committee on the Ethics of Animal Experiments of the Korea University Medical School (IACUC number KOREA-2018-0032). After behavior experiments, all zebrafish were anesthetized using MS-222 (tricaine, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, Mo., USA) and euthanatized by decapitation.

Drug administration

MgT (C8H14MgO10, molecular weight 294.50 g/mol, Doctor’s Best Inc., USA) and MgSO4 (anhydrous, molecular weight 120.37 g/mol, Biosesang Inc., Korea) were used in the study. MgT were centrifuged at 2000 revolutions per minute for 2 min in 30 mL of phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) to a concentration of 100 mM and a sample from the middle layer was diluted 10 times (10 mM) or 100 times (1 mM). MgSO4 was also centrifuged and diluted in PBS in the same way. Drug concentrations were determined based on physiological concentrations of magnesium [22, 23] and results of MgT toxicity experiments in zebrafish embryos described below.

Evaluation of toxicity of MgT in zebrafish embryos

Zebrafish embryos were treated with 0 (control), 5, 25, 50, and 100 mM MgT (diluted in water) in a 6-well plate (ten embryos per well) and incubated at 26 °C. Two days later, heart rate and development were measured with a microscope. After 6 days, survival rate was evaluated.

Hypoxic chamber

The hypoxic chamber consisted of a closed glass box attached to an inner lid with a pack of Gaspak™ (Becton, Dickinson and Company, USA) (Fig. 1) filled with water such that the Gaspak™ was not immersed. The hypoxic chamber was closed the night prior to the experiment so as to reach at 1.0 ± 0.5 mg/L of dissolved oxygen (DO) as measured by a portable DO analyzer.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Schematic of the hypoxic chamber

Effects of MgT and MgSO4 on neurons

The human neuroblastoma cell line SH-SY5Y (ATCC® CRL-2266™, American Type Culture Collection, USA) were used for in vitro experiments. After ensuring cells were healthy and free of contamination, and removing culture media, cells were treated with fetal bovine serum-free Dulbecco’s Modified Eagle Medium, and starved for two hours. Prewarmed 1 × trypsin solution was added to promote cell detachment in appropriate quantities (0.5 mL/10 cm2). When more than 90% of cells were detached, prewarmed complete growth media was added to inactivate the trypsin. After cell counting,  μL of cell suspension was added to a 96-well plate to obtain a final concentration of 7000 cells per well. The cells were treated with PBS (control), MgSO4 (1 or 10 mM) or MgT (1 or 10 mM) for 1 h. Media was changed, and the cells were incubated for two hours in a hypoxic chamber with no water (or a regular CO2 incubator as a control). To evaluate cell viability, 10 μL of cell counting kit-8 solution (CCK-8 solution, Dojindo Laboratories, Japan) was added to each well and absorbance was measured with a microplate reader (450 nm). To ensure reliability of the experimental results, experiments were repeated three times (each with 6 technical replicates per group).

Hypoxic zebrafish model

Zebrafish exposed to hypoxia were evaluated using a 4-stage behavioral repertoire as previously described [24]. Briefly, fish were classified as stage 1 (swimming on the surface of the water), stage 2 (failure to attain normal posture), stage 3 (intermittent maintenance of opercular beats with brief movements), and stage 4 (dead). When a hypoxic zebrafish reached stage 3 it was immediately transferred to a normoxic chamber (DO: 7.0 ± 0.5 mg/L).

Oral administration of PBS and MgT

Oral administration was performed by a professional after tricaine anesthesia [25]. Zebrafish were anesthetized in a water mixture containing 16.8 mg of tricaine per 100 mL, resuspended to minimize the anesthetic time, and administered 1 μL orally of PBS or MgT (10 mM) with a micropipette, taking care not to damage the mouth. This volume was chosen based on pilot data showing that zebrafish were likely to vomit solutions administered at volumes greater than 2 μL.

Classification of the experimental group

The experimental group was randomly divided into four groups: PBS = zebrafish that were orally administered 1 μL of PBS and remained in normoxia for 3 h; PBS + HYP = zebrafish that were orally administered 1 μL of PBS and placed in the hypoxic chamber for 1 h followed by normoxia for 2 h; MgT = zebrafish that were orally administered 10 mM of MgT and were in normoxia for 3 h; and the MgT + HYP group = zebrafish that were orally administered 10 mM of MgT and placed in the hypoxic chamber for 2 h followed by normoxia for 2 h (Fig. 2a). Randomization was performed using a web-based computer-generated list (www.randomization.com). The numbers were kept in opaque, sealed envelopes that were opened in the laboratory just before the experiment.

Fig. 2
figure 2

T-maze experiment. a Experimental groups and schedules. b Three-dimensional schematic of the T-maze. Colors indicate the two compartments: red for the target compartment and yellow for the opposite compartment

Sample size calculation

Power analysis revealed that a minimum sample size of 10 for each group would be required to achieve a power of 80% at a significance level of 5%. Power analysis was calculated from results of our prior experiment [26]. To allow for exclusions, total sample size was prospectively set at 48 zebrafish (12 for each group).

Color-added T-maze test

A T-shaped maze containing two arms and one stem was used to measure learning using color preference or compensation. A starting box (10 × 10 × 10 cm) was located at the bottom of the stem (50 × 10 × 10 cm) and two compartments (the “target” and “opposite” compartment; 10 × 10 × 10 cm each) were located at the ends of the maze arms (each 20 × 10 × 10 cm). Transparent sliding doors were used to separate the start box and the arms of the maze from the stem. During the training periods, sleeves made of red or yellow cellophane were fitted around the target and opposite compartments, respectively (Fig. 2b). To minimize bias, all the experiments started at 1–2 pm in a quiet dedicated place in the laboratory and all zebrafish were subjected to a habituation trial for 2 h prior to testing. Each zebrafish was trained once a day for four consecutive days. 20 µL of food (brine shrimp) was placed in the red cellophane compartment and each zebrafish was placed in the start box. On the fifth day, all zebrafish underwent memory testing, wherein all cellophane and food rewards were removed from the maze. All processes were recorded with a 4 K camcorder (Sony FDR-AX33, Sony Corporation, Japan) and analyzed using EthoVision XT software (Noldus Information Technology, Netherlands).

Evaluation of MgT effects on zebrafish behavior

The evaluation indices for zebrafish behavior were as follows: time spent = total time spent in one of the compartments of the T-maze; distance moved = total horizontal distance moved in a compartment; and frequency of entries = the number of times the zebrafish entered one of the compartments.

Compartment preference was calculated using the following equation: preference = log(target compartment/opposite compartment). Therefore, 0 indicated no preference, and 1 indicated an index value in the target compartment 10 times higher than that of the opposite compartment. Conversely, a negative preference value indicated that the opposite compartment was preferred.

2,3,5-triphenyltetrazolium chloride (TTC) staining

TTC staining is a widely used method to measure hypoxic brain damage [27] and evaluate activity of brain mitochondrial dehydrogenase. To remove the brain, zebrafish were anesthetized using MS-222 (tricaine) and euthanatized by decapitation upon completion of behavior testing. For gross infarct size assessment, the brain was incubated in 1 mL of PBS containing 2% TTC (Sigma-Aldrich) for 40 min, followed by overnight incubation in 4% paraformaldehyde. The following day, the brain was imaged with a microscope. For absorbance measurement, the brain was incubated in TTC solution in a CO2 incubator for 100 min. The TTC solution was discarded and the brain was gently rinsed with 2–3 drops of dimethyl sulfoxide (DMSO)/ethanol (1:1 solution) and stored overnight in a 1.5 mL tube containing 1 mL of DMSO/ethanol solution. The following day, the absorbance of the DMSO/ethanol solution was measured with a spectrophotometer (Epoch, BioTek instruments, USA) and corrected by the brain weight.

Western blot

To determine the mechanism of MgT in neuroprotection during hypoxia, we measured the expression of a glutamate transporter, excitatory amino acid transporter (EAAT) 4, by western blot. Following behavioral testing, zebrafish brains were homogenized in a lysis buffer (radio-immunoprecipitation buffer; Sigma) containing a protease inhibitor cocktail (Roche). Protein concentration was determined using the Bradford method. Proteins (20 μg) were separated by 8% sodium dodecyl sulfate polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis and transferred to membranes. The membranes were blocked with 5% skim milk in 1 × tris-buffered saline (TBS) at room temperature for 1 hour. The membranes were then incubated with a rabbit anti-EAAT4 antibody (ab41650, Abcam, Cambridge, MA) or rabbit anti-beta actin (A5441, Sigma-Aldrich) overnight at 4 °C. Membranes were washed three times in 1 × TBS + 0.05% Tween 20 and incubated with a 1:2000 (EAAT4) or 1:5000 (beta actin) dilution of horseradish peroxidase (HRP)-conjugated anti-rabbit immunoglobulin G (IgG) secondary antibody. An electro-chemiluminescence kit was used to develop the western blots (Amersham, Boston, MA, USA). Quantitative analysis of densitometry was performed using ImageJ (v. 1.52a, National Institutes of Health, USA).

Statistical analysis

All data were analyzed using SPSS 22 (IBM, USA) and GraphPad Prism 6.0 (GraphPad, USA). Data are expressed as mean with standard error of the mean. Data were tested for normality using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Parametric or non-parametric analysis was performed as appropriate. For MgT toxicity and survival data, a Kaplan–Meier survival curve was evaluated using the log-rank (Mantel-Cox) test. Cell viability was compared using one-way analyses of variance, and Tukey’s multiple comparison tests were used for post hoc analysis among the treated and untreated hypoxia groups. T-maze results were compared using a paired T test or Wilcoxon matched-pair signed rank test to determine differences in compartment preference. P-value < 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

Evaluation of toxicity of MgT in zebrafish embryos

Two embryos in the 100 mM group died on the day of MgT treatment. Two days after drug treatment, most zebrafish in the 100 mM group showed abnormal cardiac development. The remaining embryos that survived past the third day hatched. On the sixth day, all zebrafish in the control, MgT (5 mM), and MgT (25 mM) groups survived (10 zebrafish in each group, survival rate of 100%). Two survived in the MgT (50 mM) group (survival rate of 20%) and no zebrafish survived in MgT (100 mM) group (survival rate of 0%) Kaplan–Meier survival analysis showed significant differences between doses (p < 0.001) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Kaplan Meier Survival Analysis. The log-rank (Mantel-Cox) test showed significant differences among the survival curves (p < 0.001)

Effects of MgT and MgSO4 on neuron cells

In the groups that underwent hypoxia, cell viability was significantly decreased compared to the groups without hypoxic treatment. Among the normoxic groups, cell viabilities decreased when pretreated with MgT or MgSO4 (F (2, 51) = 84.65, p < 0.001 and F (2, 51) = 81.18, p < 0.001, respectively) (Fig. 4). On the contrary, among the hypoxic groups, cell viability significantly increased when pretreated with 1 mM or 10 mM MgT (F (2, 51) = 6.837, p = 0.0023) (Fig. 4). In the groups with 1 mM or 10 mM MgSO4, cell viability was not affected (F (2,51) = 0.6304, p = 0.5365). In both MgT and MgSO4, Concentration itself (1 or 10 mM) did not affect any significant difference, regardless of oxygen condition.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Effects of MgT and MgSO4 on neurons after normoxia (left) or hypoxia (right) in the presence or absence of MgT or MgSO4. MgT: magnesium l-threonate. The cells were treated with PBS (control), MgSO4 (1 or 10 mM) or MgT (1 or 10 mM) for 1 h. After the culture media were exchanged, the cells were incubated for 2 h in a CO2 incubator or a hypoxic chamber. The cellular experiments were repeated three times (each individual experiment contained 6 technical replicated, totaling 180 wells). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. p < 0.05 compared to the PBS group, p < 0.05 compared to the PBS + HYP group. PBS: phosphate-buffered saline; MgT, magnesium l-threonate; HYP, hypoxia

Evaluation of MgT effects in Zebrafish behavior

Twelve zebrafish in the PBS group, 11 in the PBS + HYP group, 11 in the MgT group, and 11 in the MgT + HYP group were included in the behavior testing. In the hypoxia groups, the average time for the zebrafish to reach stage 3 of hypoxia was 10 ± 0.5 min.

The PBS group showed significantly longer time spent and distance moved in the target compartment than the opposite compartment (t (11) = 2.420, p = 0.034 and t (11) = 2.243, p = 0.046, respectively) while the frequency of entries was not significantly different (t (11) = 1.267, p = 0.231). There was no significant difference in time, distance, or frequency of entries between the target and opposite compartments in the PBS + HYP group (t (10) = 0.201, p = 0.845, t (10) = 0.011, p = 0.991, and t (10) = 0.686, p = 0.509, respectively) (Table 1). The MgT group also showed a significantly longer time and distance in the target compartment compared to the opposite compartment (t (10) = 2.901, p = 0.016 and t (10) = 2.507, 0.031, respectively) and the frequency of entries was unchanged (t (10) = 1.894, p = 0.088) (Table 1). The MgT + HYP group showed significantly higher distance and frequency of entries in the target compartment compared to the opposite compartment (t (10) = 2.641, p = 0.025 and (t (10) = 3.373, p = 0.007, respectively), and the difference in time was not significant (t (10) = 2.140, p = 0.058) (Table 1).

Table 1 T-Maze results

Figure 5 shows the target compartment preference for time spent, distance moved, and frequency of entries. For all measures, the PBS + HYP group showed no compartment preference.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Preference for (a) time spent, (b) distance moved, and (c) frequency of entries into each compartment. Preferences were calculated as ‘Log (Target compartment/opposite compartment)’ from the zebrafish behavior test (see Table 1). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. The PBS + HYP group showed no preference in the behavior test. PBS, phosphate-buffered saline; MgT, magnesium l-threonate; HYP, hypoxia

TTC staining

Visualization with a microscope showed that the infarct area was reduced in the MgT + HYP group compared to the PBS + HYP group (Fig. 6a, Additional file 1). In addition, the PBS + HYP group showed significantly lower absorbance than the other groups (F (4,30) = 24.34, p < 0.001, adjusted p = 0.001 for PBS + HYP vs PBS, < 0.001 for PBS + HYP vs MgT and 0.002 for PBS + HYP vs MgT + HYP). There was no significant difference in absorbance between the PBS, MgT, and MgT + HYP groups (Fig. 6b).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Zebrafish brain injury detected by TTC staining. a TTC-stained zebrafish brain sections: PBS, PBS + HYP, MgT, and MgT + HYP. Scale bar = 1000 um. The square indicates an unstained area. bSpectrophotometric measurement. Compared to the PBS group, the PBS + HYP group showed significantly low absorbance, while the MgT or MgT + HYP group did not. Eight brains in each group were analyzed. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. p < 0.05 compared to the PBS group, p < 0.05 compared to the PBS + HYP group. PBS, phosphate-buffered saline; MgT, magnesium l-threonate; HYP, hypoxia. TTC, 2,3,5-triphenyltetrazolium chloride

Western blot

Western blot revealed upregulation of EAAT4 in the MgT group. Compared to the PBS group, an 18% decrease was observed in the PBS + HYP group, a 110% increase was found in the MgT group, and a 35% increase was present in the MgT + HYP group (this was a 65% increase compared to the PBS + HYP group and a 36% decrease compared to the MgT group) (Fig. 7, Additional file 2).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Upregulation of EAAT4. a Western blotting of EAAT4. b Optical density, normalized to beta actin. The predicted size of the EAAT4 protein was 61 kDa and that of beta actin was 42 kDa. Compared to the PBS group, an 18% decrease was observed in the PBS + HYP group, a 110% increase in the MgT group, and a 35% increase in the MgT + HYP group (representing a 65% increase compared to the PBS + HYP group and a 36% decrease compared to the MgT group). Data are shown as mean ± SEM. PBS, phosphate-buffered saline; MgT, magnesium l-threonate; HYP, hypoxia

Discussion

In this study, we showed MgT may prevent hypoxia-induced cognitive dysfunction with decreased brain infarction and upregulated glutamate transporter (EAAT4). In addition to other studies which showed neuroprotective effects of MgT [12,13,14,15,16], our experiment suggested a new mechanism of MgT using a novel hypoxic zebrafish model.

Cerebral hypoxia–ischemia models have been widely used to evaluate mechanisms of neuroprotection [4]. The pathophysiology underlying hypoxic brain damage includes complex mechanisms including loss of ATP, excitotoxicity, production of free radicals, inflammation, over activation of the immune system, and cell death [28]. Although numerous mammalian studies have extensively investigated ischemic stroke, most have failed to develop therapeutic treatments for ischemia. Therefore, some progress may be made by studying hypoxic-tolerant organisms such as a fish [28].

The zebrafish is a relatively small and simple vertebrate organism, who’s genetic composition is similar to that of mammals, including humans. Thus, similar genes are likely to be associated with similar functions in humans [17]. Braga et al. reported spontaneous behavioral recovery in zebrafish after hypoxia [24]. In addition, several zebrafish models have been developed to demonstrate the usefulness of assessing cognitive function, learning, and memory [27, 29]. The zebrafish T-maze is based on visual discrimination learning [30]. Sison and Gerlai evaluated associative memory using visual perception in zebrafish [31]. Notably, zebrafish have specific color preferences [32]. We previously employed a similar zebrafish behavior model using color preferences in the absence of food rewards [21, 26]. Similar to our previous findings, a four-day training period was sufficient to allow zebrafish to develop a preference for a particular target compartment, even in the absence of colored cellophane and food during the testing period. Approximately 10 min of hypoxia reversed the effects of training and induced cerebral injury, consistent with our previous results [21, 26].

Magnesium is important for proper functioning in many tissues and organs including those of the cardiovascular, neuromuscular, and nervous systems. It plays an important role in synaptic plasticity [33] by reducing the calcium dependent post-burst after hyperpolarization of membrane potential, and regulating voltage-dependent blockade of N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) glutamate receptors [23, 34]. Previous studies have shown that an increase in magnesium ion concentration in the extracellular fluid causes long-term enhancement of synaptic plasticity in hippocampal neurons [35]. Thus, increased magnesium in the brain may improve cognitive function.

Nonetheless, McKee et al. [11] reported limited neuroprotective effects of MgSO4 in patients with acute brain injury. Intravenous MgSO4 administration appeared to be hindered by the blood–brain barrier, leading to low levels of magnesium in the CSF, reflective of brain bioavailability [11, 36]. Compared with MgSO4, MgT has a different molecular structure that consists of a magnesium ion and threonate. Interestingly, Slutsky et al. [13] showed that sodium-L-threonate with/without magnesium chloride did not affect memory, while MgT enhanced memory recall. Sun et al. [12] reported several threonate effects, including increased mitochondrial function, glutamatergic synapse density, and neuronal intracellular magnesium ions in hippocampal neuronal cultures. They suggested that threonate may induce magnesium ion transport into hippocampal neurons.

Although reliable increases in magnesium levels are mostly safe [13], magnesium overdose may result in adverse events including lowered blood pressure, slowed heart rate, and cardiac arrhythmia or arrest. Because there was no prior study regarding MgT concentration, we first evaluated MgT toxicity in zebrafish embryos. After confirming that an MgT concentration below 25 mM showed no adverse effect on development or survival, we then performed a cellular experiment. In aerobic conditions, MgT and MgSO4 did not alter viability of human neuronal cell cultures. However, in hypoxic conditions, MgT treatment showed significantly improved cell viability, while MgSO4 treatment was not significant. In addition, the concentration (1 mM or 10 mM) of either MgT or MgSO4, did not affect outcome. This finding suggests that the regulation of the magnesium occurs at a cellular level at a relatively wide concentration range. We then performed an in vivo experiment with MgT. As expected, zebrafish pretreated with MgT maintained a preference in time, distance, and frequency of entries to the target compartment after hypoxic insult. The absorbance of zebrafish brain after TTC staining in the MgT + HYP groups was significantly higher than those in the PBS + HYP group. This indicates that MgT preconditioning reduced brain infarction and protected against hypoxic insult, in agreement with the behavioral results.

During hypoxia, many changes, including glutamate alteration, NMDA receptor stimulation, and neuronal degeneration occurred [37]. There are several explanations for neuroprotective effects of magnesium. Stevenson et al. [10] suggested that magnesium protects the central nervous system from hypoxic injuries through the prolyl hydroxylase or factor inhibiting hypoxia-inducible factor pathways. They focused on a specific genetic pathway (ephrinB2a with a hypoxia-inducible transcription factor 1 pathway) in neurodevelopment of zebrafish embryos. Others focused on tumor necrosis factor alpha to explain the neuroprotective effect of magnesium [14, 15]. Other plausible explanations include stabilization of the cell membrane, maintenance of ionic homeostasis by attenuating reductions in Na+K-ATPase activity, neuronal effects via reduction in NMDA receptor-mediated calcium entry into the cell, and vascular effects by improving cerebral blood flow [11, 37].

In this study, we investigated the role of EAAT4 as a neuroprotective mechanism of magnesium. Once released into the synapse, glutamate is rapidly cleared by transporters (high-affinity EAATs) to limit excitotoxicity [38]. Keeping a low concentration of extracellular glutamate is also required for high signal-to-noise ratios during synaptic transmission [6]. Among the glutamate transporters, EAAT4 is expressed in several sites, including the cerebellum, hippocampus, and spinal cord [39,40,41]. Compared to EAAT subtypes 1-3, EAAT4 is associated with higher chloride conductance, which is not coupled to glutamate uptake, and therefore acts as an inhibitory glutamate receptor [39, 42]. By switching glutamate transport and chloride channel activity, EAAT4 may dampen cellular excitability during glutamate uptake and prevent a reduction in transport rate [42]. A previous study found that Purkinje cells die more easily in the event of EAAT4 deficiency after global brain ischemia [43]. Yi et al. [40] reported upregulated EAAT4 in rat hippocampal astrocytes 3–7 days after traumatic brain injury. In addition, Sachs et al. [44] suggested a role of EAAT4 in neuroprotection using a mutant neurodegenerative rat model. These findings suggest that EAAT4 may play a role, although the neuroprotective mechanisms are complex [45]. In this study, MgT groups showed upregulation of EAAT4 protein, which is approximately 61 kDa in size. Based on our results, MgT upregulates EAAT4 by 65–110%, whereas hypoxia downregulates EAAT4 by 18–36%. Taken together with previous experimental results, hypoxia may initially deplete EAAT4 and then induce EAAT4 reactivity in a later recovery phase [40]. Upregulation of EAAT4 by MgT appeared to protect the brain from hypoxic injury. A schematic diagram of the proposed mechanism of hypoxia and magnesium interaction is described in Fig. 8.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Schematic of the proposed mechanisms of hypoxia and magnesium. Black lines with arrows indicate activation while blue lines indicate inhibition. The red-colored texts represented the key concepts in the schematic diagram

Western blotting demonstrated two unidentified protein bands expressed consistently in all groups. A consignment test also showed the two additional unknown bands mentioned above (data not shown). We referred to the antibody manufacturer’s guidelines to verify whether these bands may represent non-specific binding, or be due to incomplete antibody validation in the zebrafish. The antibody used was rabbit-derived, and validated in mice. One of the limitations of our study was that there was no suitable commercial antibody for zebrafish, making further evaluation of the mechanism limited. Moreover, it would be interesting to confirm the expression of EAAT4 several days following hypoxia. Another limitation of this study was that we performed tricaine anesthesia for oral administration of MgT or PBS prior to hypoxia and behavior testing. Although time under anesthesia was minimal, it may have influenced the outcomes via anesthetic preconditioning or toxic effects.

Despite these limitations, this study has several advantages. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first application of drug-induced memory preservation in the hypoxic zebrafish behavior model. Because zebrafish are inexpensive and easy to manage, using our protocol may be useful to apply similar experiments designed to confirm the protective or toxic effects of other drugs. This finding suggests that hypoxic-tolerant organisms appear to have adaptive mechanisms to overcome hypoxic damage. Further experiments and observations are required to evaluate the specific mechanisms in order to contribute to the clinical application of potential treatments.

Conclusion

This study showed that pretreatment with MgT, and subsequent upregulation of glutamate transporter EAAT4, has protective effects on neuronal survival, reduction in cerebral infarction, and preservation of learning and memory in zebrafish following hypoxia.

Availability of data and materials

Data are available from Pf. Il-Ok Lee, Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine, Korea University Guro Hospital, Seoul, Korea; E-mail: iloklee@korea.ac.kr for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data.

Abbreviations

CSF:

Cerebrospinal fluid

DMSO:

Dimethyl sulfoxide

DO:

Dissolved oxygen

EAAT:

Excitatory amino acid transporter

MgSO4:

Magnesium sulfate

MgT:

Magnesium l-threonate

PBS:

Phosphate-buffered saline

TBS:

Tris-buffered saline

TTC:

2,3,5-triphenyltetrazolium chloride

References

  1. Min TJ, Kim WY, Kim YS, Yoo JW, Lee HJ, Lee IO. Ulinastatin up-regulates glutamate transporters in glial cells. Int J Clin Exp Med. 2016;9(2):2367–74.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. McLean SL, Grayson B, Marsh S, Zarroug SH, Harte MK, Neill JC. Nicotinic alpha7 and alpha4beta2 agonists enhance the formation and retrieval of recognition memory: potential mechanisms for cognitive performance enhancement in neurological and psychiatric disorders. Behav Brain Res. 2016;302:73–80.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Frati P, Kyriakou C, Del Rio A, Marinelli E, Vergallo GM, Zaami S, et al. Smart drugs and synthetic androgens for cognitive and physical enhancement: revolving doors of cosmetic neurology. Curr Neuropharmacol. 2015;13(1):5–11.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Mandel M, Talamoni Fonoff E, Bor-Seng-Shu E, Teixeira MJ, Chadi G. Neurogenic neuroprotection: clinical perspectives. Funct Neurol. 2012;27(4):207–16.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Oechmichen M, Meissner C. Cerebral hypoxia and ischemia: the forensic point of view: a review. J Forensic Sci. 2006;51(4):880–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Danbolt NC. Glutamate uptake. Prog Neurobiol. 2001;65(1):1–105.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Gibbs ME, Hertz L, Ng KT. Inhibition of short-term memory formation in the chick by blockade of extracellular glutamate uptake. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2004;81(2):115–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Bickler PE, Fahlman CS, Ferriero DM. Hypoxia increases calcium flux through cortical neuron glutamate receptors via protein kinase C. J Neurochem. 2004;88(4):878–84.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Inage YW, Itoh M, Wada K, Takashima S. Expression of two glutamate transporters, GLAST and EAAT4, in the human cerebellum: their correlation in development and neonatal hypoxic-ischemic damage. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 1998;57(6):554–62.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Stevenson TJ, Trinh T, Kogelschatz C, Fujimoto E, Lush ME, Piotrowski T, et al. Hypoxia disruption of vertebrate CNS pathfinding through ephrinB2 is rescued by magnesium. PLoS Genet. 2012;8(4):e1002638.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. McKee JA, Brewer RP, Macy GE, Borel CO, Reynolds JD, Warner DS. Magnesium neuroprotection is limited in humans with acute brain injury. Neurocrit Care. 2005;2(3):342–51.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Sun Q, Weinger JG, Mao F, Liu G. Regulation of structural and functional synapse density by L-threonate through modulation of intraneuronal magnesium concentration. Neuropharmacology. 2016;108:426–39.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Slutsky I, Abumaria N, Wu LJ, Huang C, Zhang L, Li B, et al. Enhancement of learning and memory by elevating brain magnesium. Neuron. 2010;65(2):165–77.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Wang J, Liu Y, Zhou LJ, Wu Y, Li F, Shen KF, et al. Magnesium l-threonate prevents and restores memory deficits associated with neuropathic pain by inhibition of TNF-alpha. Pain Physician. 2013;16(5):E563–75.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Xu T, Li D, Zhou X, Ouyang HD, Zhou LJ, Zhou H, et al. Oral application of magnesium-L-threonate attenuates vincristine-induced allodynia and hyperalgesia by normalization of tumor necrosis factor-alpha/nuclear factor-kappaB signaling. Anesthesiology. 2017;126(6):1151–68.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Li W, Yu J, Liu Y, Huang XJ, Abumaria N, Zhu Y, et al. Elevation of brain magnesium prevents synaptic loss and reverses cognitive deficits in Alzheimer’s disease mouse model. Mol Brain. 2014;7:65.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Howe K, Clark MD, Torroja CF, Torrance J, Berthelot C, Muffato M, et al. The zebrafish reference genome sequence and its relationship to the human genome. Nature. 2013;496(7446):498–503.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Kalueff AV, Stewart AM, Gerlai R. Zebrafish as an emerging model for studying complex brain disorders. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2014;35(2):63–75.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Shams S, Rihel J, Ortiz JG, Gerlai R. The zebrafish as a promising tool for modeling human brain disorders: a review based upon an IBNS Symposium. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;85:176–90.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Collier AD, Khan KM, Caramillo EM, Mohn RS, Echevarria DJ. Zebrafish and conditioned place preference: a translational model of drug reward. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2014;55:16–25.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Kim YH, Lee KS, Park AR, Min TJ. Adding preferred color to a conventional reward method improves the memory of zebrafish in the T-maze behavior model. Anim Cells Syst. 2017;21(6):374–81.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Westermaier T, Stetter C, Kunze E, Willner N, Raslan F, Vince GH, et al. Magnesium treatment for neuroprotection in ischemic diseases of the brain. Exp Transl Stroke Med. 2013;5(1):6.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Do SH. Magnesium: a versatile drug for anesthesiologists. Korean J Anesthesiol. 2013;65(1):4–8.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Braga MM, Rico EP, Cordova SD, Pinto CB, Blaser RE, Dias RD, et al. Evaluation of spontaneous recovery of behavioral and brain injury profiles in zebrafish after hypoxia. Behav Brain Res. 2013;253:145–51.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kulkarni P, Chaudhari GH, Sripuram V, Banote RK, Kirla KT, Sultana R, et al. Oral dosing in adult zebrafish: proof-of-concept using pharmacokinetics and pharmacological evaluation of carbamazepine. Pharmacol Rep. 2014;66(1):179–83.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Kim YH, Lee KS, Kim YS, Kim YH, Kim JH. Effects of hypoxic preconditioning on memory evaluated using the T-maze behavior test. Anim Cells Syst. 2019;23(1):10–7.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Yu X, Li YV. Zebrafish as an alternative model for hypoxic-ischemic brain damage. Int J Physiol Pathophysiol Pharmacol. 2011;3(2):88–96.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Bickler PE. Clinical perspectives: neuroprotection lessons from hypoxia-tolerant organisms. J Exp Biol. 2004;207(Pt 18):3243–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Levin ED, Chen E. Nicotinic involvement in memory function in zebrafish. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2004;26(6):731–5.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Colwill RM, Raymond MP, Ferreira L, Escudero H. Visual discrimination learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Processes. 2005;70(1):19–31.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Sison M, Gerlai R. Associative learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio) in the plus maze. Behav Brain Res. 2010;207(1):99–104.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Avdesh A, Martin-Iverson MT, Mondal A, Chen M, Askraba S, Morgan N, et al. Evaluation of color preference in zebrafish for learning and memory. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;28(2):459–69.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Xu ZP, Li L, Bao J, Wang ZH, Zeng J, Liu EJ, et al. Magnesium protects cognitive functions and synaptic plasticity in streptozotocin-induced sporadic Alzheimer’s model. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(9):e108645.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Glasdam SM, Glasdam S, Peters GH. The importance of magnesium in the human body: a systematic literature review. Adv Clin Chem. 2016;73:169–93.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Slutsky I, Sadeghpour S, Li B, Liu GS. Enhancement of synaptic plasticity through chronically reduced Ca2+ flux during uncorrelated activity. Neuron. 2004;44(5):835–49.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Brewer RP, Parra A, Borel CO, Hopkins MB, Reynolds JD. Intravenous magnesium sulfate does not increase ventricular CSF ionized magnesium concentration of patients with intracranial hypertension. Clin Neuropharmacol. 2001;24(6):341–5.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Hoffman DJ, Marro PJ, McGowan JE, Mishra OP, Delivoria-Papadopoulos M. Protective effect of MgSO4 infusion on nmda receptor binding characteristics during cerebral cortical hypoxia in the newborn piglet. Brain Res. 1994;644(1):144–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Santello M, Volterra A. Synaptic modulation by astrocytes via Ca2+ -dependent glutamate release. Neuroscience. 2009;158(1):253–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Dalet A, Bonsacquet J, Gaboyard-Niay S, Calin-Jageman I, Chidavaenzi RL, Venteo S, et al. Glutamate transporters EAAT4 and EAAT5 are expressed in vestibular hair cells and calyx endings. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(9):e46261.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Yi JH, Herrero R, Chen G, Hazell AS. Glutamate transporter EAAT4 is increased in hippocampal astrocytes following lateral fluid-percussion injury in the rat. Brain Res. 2007;1154:200–5.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Hu WH, Walters WM, Xia XM, Karmally SA, Bethea JR. Neuronal glutamate transporter EAAT4 is expressed in astrocytes. Glia. 2003;44(1):13–25.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Mim C, Balani P, Rauen T, Grewer C. The glutamate transporter subtypes EAAT4 and EAATs 1-3 transport glutamate with dramatically different kinetics and voltage dependence but share a common uptake mechanism. J Gen Physiol. 2005;126(6):571–89.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Welsh JP, Yuen G, Placantonakis DG, Vu TQ, Haiss F, O’Hearn E, et al. Why do Purkinje cells die so easily after global brain ischemia? Aldolase C, EAAT4, and the cerebellar contribution to posthypoxic myoclonus. Adv Neurol. 2002;89:331–59.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Sachs AJ, David SA, Haider NB, Nystuen AM. Patterned neuroprotection in the Inpp4a(wbl) mutant mouse cerebellum correlates with the expression of EAAT4. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(12):e8270.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Vazana U, Veksler R, Pell GS, Prager O, Fassler M, Chassidim Y, et al. Glutamate-mediated blood–brain barrier opening: implications for neuroprotection and drug delivery. J Neurosci. 2016;36(29):7727–39.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

YSK and IOL conceived the study, conceptualized the experimental design. YHK and TJM gave input to experimental design. YSK and YHK performed the imaging and clinical assessments and acquired the data. YSK, TJM and YHK recruited the subjects. YJW, BGL and IOL organized the study. YSK wrote the first draft and prepared the manuscript. YSK and YHK generated the figures. YJW, BGL and IOL analyzed the data. YSK, YJW, BGL, TJM and IOL interpreted the data. All authors revised and gave input to the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors information

First author, Young Sung Kim, M.D., Ph.D. Clinical Assistant Professor, Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine, Korea University Guro Hospital, Seoul, Korea; Corresponding author, Il ok Lee, M.D., Ph.D. Professor, Department of Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine, Korea University Guro Hospital, Seoul, Korea.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Il Ok Lee.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the Committee on the Ethics of Animal Experiments of the Korea University Medical School. (IACUC number KOREA-2018-0032).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1: Figure S1.

Original version of TTC-stained zebrafish brain sections: PBS, PBS + HYP, MgT, and MgT + HYP. Scale bar= 1000 um (See Figure 6a).

Additional file 2: Figure S2.

Original version of western blotting of EAAT4 (See Figure 7a).

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, YS., Won, Y.J., Lim, B.G. et al. Neuroprotective effects of magnesium l-threonate in a hypoxic zebrafish model. BMC Neurosci 21, 29 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12868-020-00580-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12868-020-00580-6

Keywords