Skip to main content

Attenuated relationship between salivary oxytocin levels and attention to social information in adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder: a comparative study

Abstract

Background

Previous research studies have assessed the relationship between attention to social information and peripheral (e.g., plasma and salivary) oxytocin (OT) levels in typically developing (TD) children and children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). A relationship between them was observed in TD children, but not in children with ASD. However, this relationship remains unexamined in other age groups. To clarify whether this lack of association is maintained throughout development in individuals with ASD, we aimed to assess the relationship between salivary OT levels and attention to social information in adolescents and adults with and without ASD.

Methods

We recruited male adolescents and adults with ASD (n = 17) and TD participants (n = 24). Using the all-in-one eye-tracking system Gazefinder, we measured the percentage fixation time allocated to social information. We also measured the salivary OT levels and Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) of participants. Subsequently, we confirmed group differences and conducted a correlation analysis to investigate the relationships between these three measures.

Results

Salivary OT levels did not show any significant difference between the ASD and TD groups and were negatively correlated with the AQ in the whole-group analysis, but not in within-group analysis. Individuals with ASD had significantly lower percentage fixation times than did TD individuals for eye regions in human faces with/without mouth motion, for upright biological motion, and for people regions in the people and geometry movies. The percentage of fixation for geometric shapes in the people and geometry movies was significantly higher in the ASD than in the TD group. In the TD group, salivary OT levels were positively correlated with percentage fixation times for upright biological motion and people and negatively correlated with inverted biological motion and geometry. However, no significant correlations were found in the ASD group.

Conclusions

Our exploratory results suggest that salivary OT levels in adolescents and adults with ASD are less indicative of attention to social stimuli than they are in TD adolescents and adults. It is suggested that their association is slightly weaker in adolescents and adults with ASD and that this attenuated relationship appears to be maintained throughout development.

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by a “deficit of social communication and social interaction” and “restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities”, which comprise various specific symptoms [1]. Although the clinical presentation of ASD is modified by appropriate support and reasonable accommodation for certain difficulties, features persist throughout life. Previous studies have assessed these features across development to better understand the clinical presentation of ASD.

Abnormal attention to social information, a component of sociality, is observed in ASD throughout various developmental stages. As first reported by Kanner [2], gaze fixation deficits are one of the most representative features of ASD and many researchers have since confirmed this objectively using eye-tracking methodology. For example, individuals with ASD tend to gaze less at the face [3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10], especially at the eye region [3, 10,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19], look less at biological motion [20,21,22], and pay greater attention to geometric shapes when people and geometric shapes are presented simultaneously [11, 23,24,25,26] than do typically developed (TD) individuals. These unique gaze patterns for the face, eyes, and geometry have been reported in both children [3,4,5, 7,8,9,10, 13,14,15, 19, 23,24,25,26] and adults [6, 11, 12, 16,17,18,19], although the unique gaze pattern for biological motion was reported only in children. Furthermore, fixation times for social information have also been shown to be negatively correlated with the severity of social deficits [12, 14, 15, 25]. Therefore, abnormal attention to social information is observed over a wide age range and reflects the severity of social deficits.

Oxytocin (OT), a neuropeptide released from the posterior pituitary gland, is likely to be associated with sociality, including abnormalities in attention to social information. OT underlies maternal behavior, sexual behavior, anxiolysis, social preference and recognition [27], and symptoms of mental disorders [28]. Central OT is measured in the cerebrospinal fluid collected by a lumbar puncture [29, 30]. However, this method is highly invasive. Therefore, many researchers have measured peripheral OT levels in the blood (plasma and serum) or saliva as a proxy for central OT levels [31,32,33]. In fact, significant correlations have been observed between the central OT and plasma OT levels [30] and between plasma OT and salivary OT levels [31, 34,35,36]. Note, however, that a meta-analysis reported that the relationship between the central OT and plasma OT levels was limited [37], and one study failed to show any significant correlation between plasma OT and salivary OT levels [38]. Furthermore, studies have reported inconsistent results regarding the relationship between central and peripheral OT levels [30, 39], and the validity of this proxy remains under debate [40]. Despite these limitations, plasma and salivary OT levels have been found to be positively correlated with sociality scores (the higher the score, the higher the sociality) using various tests in both children [41] and adults [31, 42]. Furthermore, in TD adults, serum OT levels were found to be negatively correlated with the imagination subscale score of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) [43]. These findings suggest that OT plays a role in the systems that regulate sociality and that peripheral OT levels also reflect the degree of social deficits.

An abnormal relationship between peripheral OT levels and sociality has been reported in individuals with ASD. One study reported that the more severe the social deficits, the lower the plasma OT levels in children with ASD [44]. However, in children with ASD, other studies have shown no correlations or an inverse relationship between plasma OT levels and the severity of social deficits [41, 45,46,47,48]. In adults with ASD, the plasma OT levels were not correlated with ASD traits [49]. In sum, these results suggest that the association between OT and the degree of social deficits seen in TD individuals appears to be attenuated in individuals with ASD.

The relationship between attention to social information, an aspect of sociality, and peripheral OT levels has only been investigated in children. Such studies have reported that the salivary OT levels are positively correlated with gaze fixation on the eye area in TD infants and children [50] and on object areas in a finger-pointing movie in preschool TD children [51]. However, only one study investigated individuals with ASD, reporting that salivary OT levels did not correlate with attention to social information in preschool children with ASD [51]. These results suggest that the association between peripheral OT levels and gaze patterns for social information may be attenuated in children with ASD.

However, no previous study has assessed the relationship between peripheral OT levels and gaze patterns in other age groups. Therefore, as the relationship between attention to social information and peripheral OT levels was not investigated other than in preschool children in a previous study [51], it remains unclear whether the attenuated association that is observed in children with ASD persists into adolescence and adulthood or whether it changes throughout development. As attention to social information is a lifelong prominent feature of ASD, understanding the relationship between peripheral OT and gaze for social information at various developmental stages could shed light on deficits characterizing ASD in individuals of different ages. Therefore, to clarify whether adolescents and adults with ASD also show an attenuated association between attention to social information and peripheral OT levels, as observed in children with ASD, we aimed to investigate the extent to which salivary OT levels affect the patterns of gazing at social information in adolescents and adults with and without ASD.

Methods

Participants

We recruited 17 adolescent and adult men with ASD and 24 TD male participants. In this study, we only included men because reported variations in OT levels in women are higher [52].

Participants with ASD (aged 16 to 39 years old) were recruited from the Department of Neuropsychiatry, University of Fukui Hospital, Japan, and the Department of Psychiatry and Neurobiology, Kanazawa University Hospital, Japan. Two authors (TM and HK) diagnosed the participants based on the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edition (DSM-5) [1] and the standardized criteria of the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders (DISCO) [53]. As OT appears to be associated with symptoms of various mental disorders [28], we recruited individuals who were only diagnosed with ASD; thus, participants were not included if they met the diagnostic criteria for any other psychopathology, e.g., attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, schizophrenia spectrum disorders, depressive disorders, anxiety disorders, or obsessive–compulsive disorder. Fifteen individuals with ASD completed the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition (WAIS-III) [54] and all scored ≥ 70 on the full-scale IQ (FSIQ). Two individuals with ASD refused to complete the WAIS-III, but were considered to have an average IQ based on their past or current academic performance.

TD participants (aged 20 to 53 years) were recruited from the local community. Individuals with a history of major medical or neurological illnesses, including epilepsy, significant head trauma, or a lifetime history of alcohol or drug dependence were excluded from the study. Although we did not measure the IQ in the TD group, all TD participants had graduated from high school or college.

Salivary OT measurement

Saliva samples were collected using Salivettes® (Sarstedt, Rommelsdorft, Germany). Saliva samples were frozen and stored at − 80 °C in the laboratory. Before assaying, saliva samples were lyophilized overnight and incubated at − 20 °C to concentrate them four times. The dry samples were reconstructed in the assay buffer immediately before analysis using an OT enzyme immunoassay commercial kit without extraction (Assay Designs Inc., Ann Arbor, MI). Although extraction is considered necessary to measure peripheral OT levels using an enzyme immunoassay [55], measurement without extraction was validated using a solid methodology [56, 57]. The kit’s sensitivity was 15 pg/ml. Each sample was duplicated and levels were calculated using the SpectraMax® micro-plate reader (Molecular Device, Sunnyvale, CA), according to relevant standard curves. The average intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation (CV) were 4.6% and 6.1%, respectively.

Attention to social information measurement

We used Gazefinder to assess attention to social information. Gazefinder is an all-in-one eye-tracking system used to evaluate the percentage fixation times allocated to specific objects on a video monitor [11, 50, 51]. Participants’ eye positions were measured using infrared light sources and cameras located below a 19-in. thin-film transistor (1280 pixels × 1024 pixels). Using corneal reflection techniques, eye position was recorded as (X, Y) coordinates at a frequency of 50 Hz (i.e., 3000 data collections/min). Calibration of eye position recordings was performed using a five-point method.

Following calibration, the Gazefinder presented four types of movies: (A) human faces without mouth motion; (B) human faces with mouth motion; (C) biological motion of a human, and (D) people and geometry. (A) Human faces without mouth motion included movies of a still face (7 s), of eyes blinking (an actress repeatedly opens and closes her eyes for 7 s), and of a silent face (an actress with a still face appears for 3 s, and this movie is presented after the mouth moving face movie described below). (B) Human faces with mouth motion included movies of a mouth moving face (an actress repeatedly opens and closes her mouth for 4 s) and of a talking face (7 s). In the talking face movie, the actress says, “Konnichiwa” (“Hello”), “Onamaewa?” (“What is your name?”), and “Issyoniasobouyo” (“Let’s play together”). (C) Biological motion presented upright and inverted biological motion simultaneously for 20 s. The movie was accompanied by the song “Under the Big Chestnut Tree” to which an upright human danced. (D) The people and geometry movies consisted of movie of people and geometric shapes presented at the same time and at the same size (16 s) and a movie of geometric shapes depicted in small-frame images in a small window on a movie of people (16 s). The former contains four pairs of people and geometric shape movies, each lasting 4 s, and the latter contains two types of movies lasting 8 s. The presentation order of these stimuli was randomly predetermined; all participants saw the stimuli in the same order. Figure 1 presents samples of the stimuli.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Gazefinder® movie samples and their areas-of-interest (AoIs). a Screenshot of human face without mouth motion, AoI-1 and AoI-2 include the eye and mouth regions, respectively; b screenshot of human face with mouth motion, AoI-1 and AoI-2 include the eye and mouth regions, respectively; c screenshot of biological motion, AoI-1 and AoI-2 are the upright and inverted images, respectively; d screenshot of people and geometry, AoI-1 and AoI-2 are people and geometry, respectively

Percentage fixation times allocated to areas-of interest (AoIs) on the video monitor, which are the areas considered to contain social information and set by default, were automatically calculated (time allocated to a particular area/duration of stimulus presentation), and the results could be shown immediately after the videos were viewed. Figure 1 shows the AoIs for each stimulus. Human face movies included two AoIs (i.e., AoI-1 was the eye region and AoI-2 was the mouth region). Biological motion movies included two AoIs (i.e., AoI-1 was upright biological motion involving dancing to the song, and AoI-2 was inverted biological motion that was presented upside down and played backward relative to the upright biological motion). For the movie of people and geometry, there were two AoIs (i.e., AoI-1 was the area with people, and AoI-2 was the area with geometry). In addition to these AoIs, the percentage fixation time allocated to areas other than AoI(s) (i.e., area outside the AoI) was also calculated. Figure 1 presents the AoIs of the stimuli.

Procedures

Testing was carried out in a silent room in the Department of Neuropsychiatry, University of Fukui Hospital. First, saliva samples were collected only once. Participants then watched the movies while their gaze was detected using Gazefinder. Finally, all participants completed the AQ to determine the severity of ASD [58]. The AQ comprises 50 questions, with scores ranging from 0 to 50. Higher AQ scores indicate higher severity of ASD (Table 1). The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Fukui and conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki (as revised in 2000). After a complete explanation of the study, all participants or their parents/legal guardians provided written informed consent.

Table 1 Participant characteristics

Data analysis

According to previous research [51], we excluded participants for whom the available percentage fixation time was < 50% (i.e., Gazefinder could not detect eye position more than 50% of the time). One participant with ASD, for whom the available percentage fixation time was 31%, was excluded. Therefore, data were available for statistical analysis for 16 individuals with ASD and 24 TD individuals.

At first, we carried out the Shapiro–Wilk test of normality for the salivary OT level and found that it was normally distributed (W = .952, p = .092). Therefore, we used parametric tests in the subsequent analysis. To clarify group differences in salivary OT levels and the percentage fixation times for each AoI in Gazefinder, we investigated differences between the ASD and TD groups using two-tailed t test. For the AoIs, as in the previous study using Gazefinder with adults with ASD [11], to avoid type 1 statistical errors, we applied Bonferroni corrections and set .05 divided by the number of AoIs as the significance level for each item; we set .025 (.05/2) for all stimuli. We subsequently conducted a correlation analysis to clarify the relationships among salivary OT levels, AQ, and the percentage fixation times to the AoI for each movie. For AoIs, to avoid type 1 statistical error, we applied Bonferroni correction and set .025 (.05/2) as the significance level for the correlation analysis as described above.

Subsequently, we investigated whether there were significant differences in correlation coefficients between the ASD and TD groups. All statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS statistical software, version 24 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).

Results

Demographic data

There was no significant difference in the mean ages between the ASD and TD groups (t (39) = .54, p = .590). The AQ scores were significantly different between the ASD and TD groups (t (39) = 7.00, p < .001). These scores for both groups and the IQ scores for the ASD group are summarized in Table 1.

Between-groups analysis

There was no significant difference in salivary OT levels between the groups (ASD 36.2 ± 13.2 pg/ml, TD 43.6 ± 17.0 pg/ml, t (38) = 1.45, p = .154, Cohen’s d = .49). Individuals with ASD had significantly lower percentage fixation times than did TD individuals for eye regions in human faces with/without mouth motion and for people regions in the people and geometry movies. On the one hand, the percentage of fixation for geometric shapes in the people and geometry movies was significantly higher in the ASD than in the TD group (Table 2 and Fig. 2).

Table 2 Group differences in mean fixation percentages
Fig. 2
figure 2

Bar graphs of the percentage fixation times and standard errors of each area-of-interest (AoI). The red and blue bars indicate the percentage fixation times of the ASD group and TD group, respectively. Error bars indicate standard errors of the mean. *p < .025 (= .05/2)

Correlation analysis of salivary OT levels

First, salivary OT levels and AQ for the whole group were found to be significantly correlated (r = − .398, p = .011). However, correlation analysis revealed no significant correlation between salivary OT level and AQ for both the ASD and TD groups (r = − .361, p = .169 and r = − .349, p = .095, respectively). The results of the correlation analysis between salivary OT levels and the AoIs in Gazefinder are shown in Table 3, and Fig. 3 shows the scatter plots of these results. Salivary OT levels were significantly positively correlated with percentage fixation times for upright biological motion and the people region in the people and geometry movies. These significant correlations were observed in an analysis of the whole group, as well as in a subgroup analysis of the TD group. Furthermore, there were significant negative correlations between salivary OT levels and the percentage fixation times for inverted biological motion and the geometric regions in the people and geometry movies in the TD group.

Table 3 Correlations between salivary oxytocin (OT) level and the percentage fixation times
Fig. 3
figure 3

Scatter plots of the correlation between the percentage of fixation time to area-of-interest (AoI) and salivary oxytocin (OT) level. Red circles and lines are data from the ASD group, and blue triangles and lines are from the TD group. The black line is the approximate curve for all groups

Correlation analysis for AQ

For the whole-group correlation analysis, AQ was significantly negatively correlated with percentage fixation times for the eye region for faces without/with mouth motion and the people region in the people and geometry movies and positively correlated with the mouth region for faces without/with mouth motion and the geometry region in the people and geometry movies. For the ASD group, correlation analysis showed that the mouth region for faces with mouth motion and inverted biological motion were significantly positively correlated with AQ. For the TD group, AQ was negatively correlated with the people region and positively correlated with the geometry region in people and geometry movies. The results of the correlation analysis between AQ and the AoIs in Gazefinder are shown in Table 4.

Table 4 Correlations between the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) and the percentage fixation times

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the relationship between salivary OT levels and attention to social information in adolescents and adults with ASD and TD individuals. The salivary OT levels revealed no significant differences between the ASD and TD groups and were negatively correlated with the severity of ASD traits in the whole-group analysis, but not in the within-group analysis. The correlation analysis revealed that salivary OT level were positively correlated with percentage fixation times for upright biological motion and people regions in the people and geometry movies in whole-group and subgroup analyses of TD individuals. Moreover, OT levels were negatively correlated with percentage fixation times for inverted biological motion and geometric shapes in the people and geometry movies in TD individuals. In the ASD group, no significant correlations were observed between peripheral OT levels and percentage fixation times for any item.

Salivary OT levels in the ASD group

There was no significant difference in salivary OT levels between individuals with ASD and TD participants. Although some studies have reported lower peripheral OT levels in ASD [41, 44, 45, 59, 60], others have reported similar or higher peripheral OT levels in individuals with ASD than in TD individuals [46,47,48, 51, 61]. In particular, studies of adults with ASD have reported mixed results, with both lower [60] and higher [61] peripheral OT levels. A meta-analysis published in 2016 found no evidence of peripheral OT levels being impaired in ASD [62]. Summarizing the above, researchers have not obtained consistent results for peripheral OT levels in ASD, which appear to be affected by many factors other than diagnosis. This complexity may have influenced our results.

Several factors can affect OT levels in ASD. One factor may be the ASD subgroup, as suggested by Lee et al. [63], who indicated that “the decrease of the oxytocin receptor gene expression is related to some subgroups of ASD”. This statement was based on the results of Gregory et al. [64], which showed that in some samples of individuals with ASD, there was significantly greater methylation of the oxytocin receptor gene and reduced oxytocin mRNA at postmortem analysis. The report by Modahl et al. [41], in which plasma OT levels have been shown to correlate with some ASD traits in an “aloof” subgroup but not in an “active-but-odd” subgroup, may support the existence of subgroups of ASD. In summary, taken together, these findings indicate that some, but not all, individuals with ASD show abnormal peripheral OT levels. Therefore, the existence of ASD subgroups within our group of ASD study participants may have affected the results of the current study in which the ASD group showed moderately, but not significantly lower OT levels than the TD group. In addition to the potential ASD subgroups as a factor, some studies have found that OT levels are positively correlated with anxiety [65, 66] and negatively correlated with depression [67]. Although the underlying biological mechanisms remain unknown, OT appears to be released for the purpose of reducing the amygdala and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical reactivity to stressors [68]. Anxiety and depression disorders are highly comorbid with ASD [69, 70], and these factors may disrupt OT regulation in ASD. As previously mentioned, the traits of the ASD subgroups or those of other mental disorders may have affected the results of previous studies reporting on peripheral OT concentration levels.

What will be needed in future research based on this study’s results? Our participants in the ASD group were only diagnosed with ASD, and not with any other mental disorder, meaning that the participants in the ASD group did not show severe symptoms or traits of other mental disorders. Since we excluded participants with intellectual disability, our findings can be used to reflect characteristics exclusive to ASD, laying the foundation for future research. However, no genetic analysis or measures for ASD subgroups or psychological indices other than ASD were assessed in this study. Therefore, future studies are certainly needed to elucidate the influence of these factors.

Relationship between salivary OT levels and percentage fixation times for social information

In TD individuals, salivary OT levels were correlated with percentage fixation times for upright and inverted biological motion and people and geometric regions in the people and geometry movies. OT is thought to modulate prosocial behavior with anxiolytic and anti-stress effects on the brain, especially in the amygdala [68, 71, 72]. The mechanisms underlying the prosocial effects of OT have been investigated [68], and, while it appears that OT may not have major effects on prosociality because it is modulated by many factors [73], higher peripheral OT levels do seem to lead to more social behavior [41,42,43, 51, 74]. This finding is consistent with our results showing that TD participants with high peripheral OT levels tend to gaze at more social information.

Interestingly, however, although salivary OT levels were significantly associated with percentage fixation times on the eyes and mouth in TD infants and children in a previous study [50], these results were not replicated in our study with TD adolescents and adults. This may have been because the TD adolescents and adults in our study might have frequently gazed at the eye region, therefore gazing at the mouth region less frequently, regardless of the salivary OT levels. As shown in Fig. 3, this was especially notable in movies that showed human face without mouth motion, which thus did not strongly lead the attention to the mouth [11, 50]; for these movies, even TD adolescents and adults who had low salivary OT levels gazed into the eye region at high rates. In fact, the percentage fixation times to the eye region in this study’s participants were higher than those of male children from a previous study, in which these averages were 66.89 ± 20.26% for faces without a moving mouth, and 25.98 ± 17.58% for faces with a moving mouth [50]. Taken together, these findings may indicate that all TD adolescent and adult participants in our study tended to gaze at the eye region in human faces more frequently than did TD children in previous studies. The eye region, which is related to intuiting the mental states of others [75, 76] and joint attention [77, 78], provides salient social information. Adolescents and adults may need to perform complex interactions in everyday life; thus, TD individuals may need to regularly pay more attention to the eye region. Therefore, TD adolescent and adult participants may gaze at the eye region even if their salivary OT levels are low, and significant associations between salivary OT levels and percentage fixation times on AoIs on human faces might not be observed. However, our results are unable to confirm our hypothesis, and further studies are warranted to further investigate the factors that affect gaze when seeking out social information, particularly in the context of eye region-centric gazes.

For individuals with ASD, we observed no significant correlations between salivary OT levels and gazing social information. This result pointed to the weak relationships between salivary OT levels and attention given to social information in ASD, which may be due to the same reason underlying the group differences in peripheral OT levels between groups (i.e., ASD subgroup, anxiety, depression, etc.). As described above, although we excluded individuals with comorbidities in the ASD group, we did not completely rule out these factors in this study; hence, future studies are warranted to fully clarify their influence. Previous studies on children with and without ASD have also reported significant correlations between salivary OT levels and fixation duration in TD children, but not in children with ASD [51]. Our results may thereby point to the continued disturbance of the association between salivary OT levels and fixation duration from preschool ages into adolescence and adulthood.

Significant correlations in the whole-group analysis were found in upright biological motion and the people region in people and geometry movies. Based on the concept underlying the term ‘spectrum’, which suggests that every individual has ASD tendencies to a certain degree, the salivary OT levels are presumed to be correlated with sociality, which spontaneously directs one’s eyes to social information. However, there were no significant correlations in face stimuli. As aforementioned, the eyes may be salient social stimuli and have special significance for everyday interpersonal relationships; therefore, TD individuals may also have gazed at the eye region, regardless of their salivary OT level.

In summary, the salivary OT levels appeared to be related to attention to social information in TD adolescents and adults, and this relationship appeared to be weaker in adolescents and adults with ASD. In the TD group, however, the types of social information that showed significant correlations with the salivary OT levels were different from those reported in the previous studies with children [50] described above. Hence, it is possible that the factors or mechanisms underlying the relationship in TD individuals, and its disturbance in ASD, between salivary OT levels and social attention may vary over the course of development. Therefore, longitudinal research is warranted in order to fully clarify the factors affecting them (the genetic or psychiatric factors described above) at various ages.

Relationship between AQ and percentage fixation times for social information

Significant correlations in the whole-group analysis were found for all stimuli except for biological motion. These significant correlations may reflect group differences for AQ and percentage fixation times for social information. Regarding biological motion, there were no significant correlations despite the group difference. Biological motion was the only stimulus that did not include actual human beings in Gazefinder, and the quality of biological motion for social information stimuli may differ from that of others. It is a future task to clarify the factors related to the gaze of biological motion.

In the ASD group, AQ was significantly positively correlated with the percentage fixation times to the mouth region of faces with mouth motion and inverted biological motion. Previous studies have also reported that children and adults with ASD tend to pay attention to moving objects, not to salient social information [7, 11]. Consistent with previous findings, our results may reflect the fact that the stronger the characteristics of ASD, the more difficult it is to suppress attention to movement and redirect attention to social information.

In the TD group, AQ was significantly correlated with the percentage fixation times to people and geometry regions. In both cases, the stronger one’s ASD characteristics, the less the individual attended to social information. It was suggested that the simultaneous presentation of people and geometric patterns is a stimulus that can reflect the severity of ASD in TD adolescents and adults. Regarding the face stimuli, as described in “the relationship between salivary OT levels and percentage fixation times for social information”, TD adolescent and adult participants may gaze at the eye region, regardless of their salivary OT levels, and the association between ASD severity and social attention to the eyes may be disrupted. In biological motion, as described above, as the quality of the biological motion for social information stimuli may differ from that of others’, and some factors other than ASD traits more strongly affect attention than biological motion, the correlation between them may not be significant.

Relationship between salivary OT levels and AQ

A significant correlation between salivary OT level and AQ was observed in the whole-group analysis. The significant correlation in the whole-group analysis was presumed to reflect the fact that the ASD subgroup showed abnormal peripheral OT levels, that is, the result reflecting the difference between the ASD and TD groups. Based on the spectrum concept described above, the salivary OT level reflects the ASD characteristics in the whole-group analysis. Conversely, the significant correlations between salivary OT level and AQ were not observed in the within-group analysis. In a previous study, in TD adults, the salivary OT level was not correlated with the AQ total score, while it was negatively correlated with the AQ subscale “difficulties in imagination” [43]. Furthermore, in adults with ASD, the plasma OT levels were not correlated with ASD traits [49]. Our results of the within-group analysis are consistent with those reported by previous studies. Considering both the whole and within-group analyses in this study, it is appears that OT may not have major effect on autism traits or have major effect in only some individuals. Our results support the hypothesis that “it appears that OT may not have major effects on prosociality because it is modulated by many factors [73]”.

Conclusion

We observed significant correlations between salivary OT levels and percentage fixation times for social information in TD adolescents and adults, but not in a group of adolescents and adults with ASD. This study’s results failed to demonstrate any significant differences in salivary OT levels between adolescents and adults with ASD without other mental disorders and TD individuals; however, the regulatory mechanisms underlying salivary OT levels and fixation durations may be attenuated in adolescents and adults with ASD. The ASD subgroups showed abnormal peripheral OT levels and/or some factors that did not strongly affect the TD group, which may have affected our results. In addition, the relationship between salivary OT levels and attention to social information was also weaker in preschool children with ASD than in TD children [51]; this weakened relationship may continue beyond childhood. However, the factors or mechanisms underlying this relationship remain unclear and may change throughout development. Furthermore, the lack of correlation between the eye region in face stimuli and salivary OT levels both in the ASD and TD groups is another interesting result, showing that the eye area may have special significance for both ASD and TD individuals. In future research, we will need to investigate what affects this attenuation in the ASD group and whether the causes of this relationship between peripheral OT levels and social attention in ASD and TD at different developmental stages are the same. By elucidating the nature of these factors, we will be able to better understand ASD symptoms and their development.

Limitations and future directions

This study has some limitations that should be considered when interpreting its results. First, the sample size was relatively small. Future studies with larger sample sizes are required to confirm these results. Second, we did not exclude confounding factors that could have affected peripheral OT levels and/or gaze to social information. We speculate that these confounders may include a certain degree of heterogeneity within the ASD subgroups associated with genetic factors, anxiety, and depression. Studies adjusting for these confounding factors are warranted in order to confirm this study’s results. With regard to the TD group, we did not measure IQ. As such, in addition to controlling for the factors described above, it may be necessary to repeat the analysis in this study while controlling for IQ. Finally, some researchers have suggested that extraction is needed to measure peripheral OT levels using an enzyme immunoassay [55]. Although the method used in this study, without extraction, is a valid method for measuring peripheral OT levels [56, 57] and is widely used [31, 51, 79,80,81,82], future confirmation using extraction processing is warranted.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ADOS:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

AoI:

Area-of-interest

AQ:

Autism Spectrum Quotient

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

CARS:

Childhood Autism Rating Scale

DISCO:

Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders

DSM:

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

FSIQ:

Full-Scale Intelligent Quotient

IQ:

Intelligent Quotient

OT:

Oxytocin

TD:

Typically developed

VABS:

Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale

WAIS-III:

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. New York: American Psychiatric Pub; 2013. p. 947.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  2. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Acta Paedopsychiatr. 1943;35(4):100–36.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Saitovitch A, Bargiacchi A, Chabane N, Phillipe A, Brunelle F, Boddaert N, et al. Studying gaze abnormalities in autism: which type of stimulus to use? Open Journal of Psychiatry. 2013;03(02):32–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Shic F, Macari S, Chawarska K. Speech disturbs face scanning in 6-month-old infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75(3):231–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Chawarska K, Shic F. Looking but not seeing: atypical visual scanning and recognition of faces in 2 and 4-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(12):1663–72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Sasson N, Tsuchiya N, Hurley R, Couture SM, Penn DL, Adolphs R, et al. Orienting to social stimuli differentiates social cognitive impairment in autism and schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(11):2580–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Chawarska K, Macari S, Shic F. Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(8):903–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Chawarska K, Macari S, Shic F. Decreased spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(3):195–203.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. von Hofsten C, Uhlig H, Adell M, Kochukhova O. How children with autism look at events. Res Autism Spect Disord. 2009;3(2):556–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Hosozawa M, Tanaka K, Shimizu T, Nakano T, Kitazawa S. How children with specific language impairment view social situations: an eye tracking study. Pediatrics. 2012;129(6):e1453–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Fujioka T, Inohara K, Okamoto Y, Masuya Y, Ishitobi M, Saito DN, et al. Gazefinder as a clinical supplementary tool for discriminating between autism spectrum disorder and typical development in male adolescents and adults. Mol Autism. 2016;7:19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Amestoy A, Guillaud E, Bouvard MP, Cazalets JR. Developmental changes in face visual scanning in autism spectrum disorder as assessed by data-based analysis. Front Psychol. 2015;6:989.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Dalton KM, Nacewicz BM, Johnstone T, Schaefer HS, Gernsbacher MA, Goldsmith HH, et al. Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(4):519–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Jones W, Carr K, Klin A. Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(8):946–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Speer LL, Cook AE, McMahon WM, Clark E. Face processing in children with autism: effects of stimulus contents and type. Autism. 2007;11(3):265–77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Corden B, Chilvers R, Skuse D. Avoidance of emotionally arousing stimuli predicts social-perceptual impairment in Asperger’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(1):137–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Sterling L, Dawson G, Webb S, Murias M, Munson J, Panagiotides H, et al. The role of face familiarity in eye tracking of faces by individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(9):1666–75.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Pelphrey KA, Sasson NJ, Reznick JS, Paul G, Goldman BD, Piven J. Visual scanning of faces in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2002;32(4):249–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Nakano T, Tanaka K, Endo Y, Yamane Y, Yamamoto T, Nakano Y, et al. Atypical gaze patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated from developmental changes in gaze behaviour. Proc Biol Sci. 2010;277(1696):2935–43.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Klin A, Lin DJ, Gorrindo P, Ramsay G, Jones W. Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature. 2009;459(7244):257–61.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Falck-Ytter T, Rehnberg E, Bolte S. Lack of visual orienting to biological motion and audiovisual synchrony in 3-year-olds with autism. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(7):e68816.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Annaz D, Campbell R, Coleman M, Milne E, Swettenham J. Young children with autism spectrum disorder do not preferentially attend to biological motion. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(3):401–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Pierce K, Conant D, Hazin R, Stoner R, Desmond J. Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(1):101–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Shi L, Zhou Y, Ou J, Gong J, Wang S, Cui X, et al. Different visual preference patterns in response to simple and complex dynamic social stimuli in preschool-aged children with autism spectrum disorders. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(3):e0122280.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Franchini M, Wood de Wilde H, Glaser B, Gentaz E, Eliez S, Schaer M. Brief report: a preference for biological motion predicts a reduction in symptom severity 1 year later in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Frontiers in psychiatry. 2016;7:143.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Moore A, Wozniak M, Yousef A, Barnes CC, Cha D, Courchesne E, et al. The geometric preference subtype in ASD: identifying a consistent, early-emerging phenomenon through eye tracking. Mol Autism. 2018;9:19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Neumann ID, Landgraf R. Balance of brain oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for anxiety, depression, and social behaviors. Trends Neurosci. 2012;35(11):649–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Jzendoorn MH. Sniffing around oxytocin: review and meta-analyses of trials in healthy and clinical groups with implications for pharmacotherapy. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3:e258.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Kagerbauer SM, Martin J, Schuster T, Blobner M, Kochs EF, Landgraf R. Plasma oxytocin and vasopressin do not predict neuropeptide concentrations in human cerebrospinal fluid. J Neuroendocrinol. 2013;25(7):668–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Carson DS, Berquist SW, Trujillo TH, Garner JP, Hannah SL, Hyde SA, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid and plasma oxytocin concentrations are positively correlated and negatively predict anxiety in children. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20(9):1085–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Feldman R, Gordon I, Zagoory-Sharon O. Maternal and paternal plasma, salivary, and urinary oxytocin and parent-infant synchrony: considering stress and affiliation components of human bonding. Dev Sci. 2011;14(4):752–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Taylor SE, Saphire-Bernstein S, Seeman TE. Are plasma oxytocin in women and plasma vasopressin in men biomarkers of distressed pair-bond relationships? Psychol Sci. 2010;21(1):3–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Nagasawa M, Mitsui S, En S, Ohtani N, Ohta M, Sakuma Y, et al. Social evolution. Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science. 2015;348(6232):333–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Martin J, Kagerbauer SM, Gempt J, Podtschaske A, Hapfelmeier A, Schneider G. Oxytocin levels in saliva correlate better than plasma levels with concentrations in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients in neurocritical care. J Neuroendocrinol. 2018;30:e12596.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Feldman R, Gordon I, Schneiderman I, Weisman O, Zagoory-Sharon O. Natural variations in maternal and paternal care are associated with systematic changes in oxytocin following parent-infant contact. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(8):1133–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Grewen KM, Davenport RE, Light KC. An investigation of plasma and salivary oxytocin responses in breast- and formula-feeding mothers of infants. Psychophysiology. 2010;47(4):625–32.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Valstad M, Alvares GA, Egknud M, Matziorinis AM, Andreassen OA, Westlye LT, et al. The correlation between central and peripheral oxytocin concentrations: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;78:117–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Javor A, Riedl R, Kindermann H, Brandstatter W, Ransmayr G, Gabriel M. Correlation of plasma and salivary oxytocin in healthy young men—experimental evidence. Neuro Endocrinol Lett. 2014;35(6):470–3.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Jokinen J, Chatzittofis A, Hellstrom C, Nordstrom P, Uvnas-Moberg K, Asberg M. Low CSF oxytocin reflects high intent in suicide attempters. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37(4):482–90.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Valstad M, Alvares GA, Andreassen OA, Westlye LT, Quintana DS. The relationship between central and peripheral oxytocin concentrations: a systematic review and meta-analysis protocol. Syst Rev. 2016;5:49.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Modahl C, Green L, Fein D, Morris M, Waterhouse L, Feinstein C, et al. Plasma oxytocin levels in autistic children. Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43(4):270–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Feldman R, Zagoory-Sharon O, Weisman O, Schneiderman I, Gordon I, Maoz R, et al. Sensitive parenting is associated with plasma oxytocin and polymorphisms in the OXTR and CD38 genes. Biol Psychiat. 2012;72(3):175–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Crespi BJ, Hurd PL. Genetically based correlates of serum oxytocin and testosterone in autism and schizotypy. Pers Indiv Differ. 2015;79:39–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Alabdali A, Al-Ayadhi L, El-Ansary A. Association of social and cognitive impairment and biomarkers in autism spectrum disorders. J Neuroinflamm. 2014;11:4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Green L, Fein D, Modahl C, Feinstein C, Waterhouse L, Morris M. Oxytocin and autistic disorder: alterations in peptide forms. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(8):609–13.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Jacobson JD, Ellerbeck KA, Kelly KA, Fleming KK, Jamison TR, Coffey CW, et al. Evidence for alterations in stimulatory G proteins and oxytocin levels in children with autism. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2014;40:159–69.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Miller M, Bales KL, Taylor SL, Yoon J, Hostetler CM, Carter CS, et al. Oxytocin and vasopressin in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: sex differences and associations with symptoms. Autism Res. 2013;6(2):91–102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Taurines R, Schwenck C, Lyttwin B, Schecklmann M, Jans T, Reefschlager L, et al. Oxytocin plasma concentrations in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: correlation with autistic symptomatology. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2014;6(3):231–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Althaus M, Groen Y, Noltes H, Tucha O, Sweep FC, et al. Do blood plasma levels of oxytocin moderate the effect of nasally administered oxytocin on social orienting in high-functioning male adults with autism spectrum disorder? Psychopharmacology. 2016;233(14):2737–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Nishizato M, Fujisawa TX, Kosaka H, Tomoda A. Developmental changes in social attention and oxytocin levels in infants and children. Scientific reports. 2017;7(1):2540.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Fujisawa TX, Tanaka S, Saito DN, Kosaka H, Tomoda A. Visual attention for social information and salivary oxytocin levels in preschool children with autism spectrum disorders: an eye-tracking study. Front Neurosci. 2014;8:295.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Salonia A, Nappi RE, Pontillo M, Daverio R, Smeraldi A, Briganti A, et al. Menstrual cycle-related changes in plasma oxytocin are relevant to normal sexual function in healthy women. Horm Behav. 2005;47(2):164–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Wing L, Leekam SR, Libby SJ, Gould J, Larcombe M. The diagnostic interview for social and communication disorders: background, inter-rater reliability and clinical use. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2002;43(3):307–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Wechsler D. The wechsler adult intelligence scale -3rd edition. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  55. McCullough ME, Churchland PS, Mendez AJ. Problems with measuring peripheral oxytocin: can the data on oxytocin and human behavior be trusted? Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(8):1485–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. MacLean EL, Gesquiere LR, Gee N, Levy K, Martin WL, Carter CS. Validation of salivary oxytocin and vasopressin as biomarkers in domestic dogs. J Neurosci Methods. 2018;293:67–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. MacLean EL, Gesquiere LR, Gee NR, Levy K, Martin WL, Carter CS. Effects of Affiliative Human-Animal Interaction on Dog Salivary and Plasma Oxytocin and Vasopressin. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1606.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31(1):5–17.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Al-Ayadhi LY. Altered oxytocin and vasopressin levels in autistic children in Central Saudi Arabia. Neurosciences (Riyadh). 2005;10(1):47–50.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Andari E, Duhamel JR, Zalla T, Herbrecht E, Leboyer M, Sirigu A. Promoting social behavior with oxytocin in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(9):4389–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Jansen LM, Gispen-de Wied CC, Wiegant VM, Westenberg HG, Lahuis BE, van Engeland H. Autonomic and neuroendocrine responses to a psychosocial stressor in adults with autistic spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36(7):891–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Rutigliano G, Rocchetti M, Paloyelis Y, Gilleen J, Sardella A, Cappucciati M, et al. Peripheral oxytocin and vasopressin: biomarkers of psychiatric disorders? A comprehensive systematic review and preliminary meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2016;241:207–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Lee SY, Lee AR, Hwangbo R, Han J, Hong M, Bahn GH. Is oxytocin application for autism spectrum disorder evidence-based? Exp Neurobiol. 2015;24(4):312–24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Gregory SG, Connelly JJ, Towers AJ, Johnson J, Biscocho D, Markunas CA, et al. Genomic and epigenetic evidence for oxytocin receptor deficiency in autism. BMC Med. 2009;7:62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Tops M, van Peer JM, Korf J, Wijers AA, Tucker DM. Anxiety, cortisol, and attachment predict plasma oxytocin. Psychophysiology. 2007;44(3):444–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Hoge EA, Pollack MH, Kaufman RE, Zak PJ, Simon NM. Oxytocin levels in social anxiety disorder. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2008;14(3):165–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Scantamburlo G, Hansenne M, Fuchs S, Pitchot W, Marechal P, Pequeux C, et al. Plasma oxytocin levels and anxiety in patients with major depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32(4):407–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Domes G, Kirsch P, Heinrichs M. Oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain: social neuropeptides for translational medicine. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(9):524–38.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. van Steensel FJ, Bogels SM, Perrin S. Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2011;14(3):302–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Sterling L, Dawson G, Estes A, Greenson J. Characteristics associated with presence of depressive symptoms in adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(6):1011–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Neumann ID. Brain oxytocin: a key regulator of emotional and social behaviours in both females and males. J Neuroendocrinol. 2008;20(6):858–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Macdonald K, Macdonald TM. The peptide that binds: a systematic review of oxytocin and its prosocial effects in humans. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2010;18(1):1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Shen H. Neuroscience: the hard science of oxytocin. Nature. 2015;522(7557):410–2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Light KC, Grewen KM, Amico JA. More frequent partner hugs and higher oxytocin levels are linked to lower blood pressure and heart rate in premenopausal women. Biol Psychol. 2005;69(1):5–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. BaronCohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psyc. 1997;38(7):813–22.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  76. Bal E, Harden E, Lamb D, Van Hecke AV, Denver JW, Porges SW. Emotion recognition in children with autism spectrum disorders: relations to eye gaze and autonomic state. J Autism Dev Disord. 2010;40(3):358–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Bruinsma Y, Koegel RL, Koegel LK. Joint attention and children with autism: a review of the literature. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2004;10(3):169–75.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Dawson G, Toth K, Abbott R, Osterling J, Munson J, Estes A, et al. Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Dev Psychol. 2004;40(2):271–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Carter CS, Pournajafi-Nazarloo H, Kramer KM, Ziegler TE, White-Traut R, Bello D, et al. Oxytocin—behavioral associations and potential as a salivary biomarker. Ann Ny Acad Sci. 2007;1098:312–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Feldman R, Golan O, Hirschler-Guttenberg Y, Ostfeld-Etzion S, Zagoory-Sharon O. Parent-child interaction and oxytocin production in pre-schoolers with autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2014;205(2):107–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Leeds A, Dennis PM, Lukas KE, Stoinski TS, Willis MA, Schook MW. Validating the use of a commercial enzyme immunoassay to measure oxytocin in unextracted urine and saliva of the western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Primates. 2018;59(6):499–515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Yuhi T, Ise K, Iwashina K, Terao N, Yoshioka S, Shomura K, et al. Sex differences in salivary oxytocin and cortisol concentration changes during cooking in a small group. Behav Sci (Basel). 2018;8:11.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We wish to express our sincere appreciation to the participants who generously and courageously participated in this study. We thank Kayo Mita for helping with data collection.

Funding

This study was partly funded by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (15K08093, 16K17469), the Center of Community from MEXT, the Research on Development of New Medical Devices form AMED, and Research and development of supportive device technology for medicine using ICT from AMED and MEXT. Part of this study was the result of the project “Integrated Research on Neuropsychiatric Disorders”, which was conducted under the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences from MEXT and AMED.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

TF acquired the data, performed the statistical analyses, interpreted the clinical data, and drafted the manuscript. TXF acquired the data, collected and analyzed saliva samples, interpreted the clinical data, and drafted the manuscript. KI acquired the data and drafted the manuscript. YO acquired the data, collected and analyzed saliva samples, and drafted the manuscript. YM recruited and evaluated the participants. KJT conceived the study, performed the statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. TK conceived the study, interpreted the clinical data, and drafted the manuscript. TM recruited and evaluated the participants, and diagnosed the participants with ASD. AT recruited the participants and evaluated the participants, and drafted the manuscript. YW recruited and evaluated the participants, and interpreted the clinical data. HK conceived of the study, recruited and evaluated the participants, diagnosed the participants with ASD, interpreted the clinical data, and drafted the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to H. Kosaka.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of each university and conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki (as revised in 2000). After a complete explanation of the study, all participants or their parents/legal guardians provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

These data were anonymously collected after obtaining the consent of participants.

Competing interests

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Fujioka, T., Fujisawa, T.X., Inohara, K. et al. Attenuated relationship between salivary oxytocin levels and attention to social information in adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder: a comparative study. Ann Gen Psychiatry 19, 38 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12991-020-00287-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12991-020-00287-2

Keywords