Skip to main content

Foraging for high caloric anthropogenic prey is energetically costly

Abstract

Background

Several generalist species benefit from food provided by human activities. Food from anthropogenic sources is often high in caloric value and can positively influence reproductive success or survival. However, this type of resource may require specific foraging skills and habitat experience with related costs and benefits. As a result, not all individuals utilize these resources equally, with some individuals preferentially foraging in habitats where natural resources of lower energy content are predominant, possibly due to lower energy expenditure of the specific foraging strategy.

Methods

Here we investigate whether foraging in habitats which contain high caloric resources of anthropogenic origin is energetically costlier than foraging in habitats with low caloric resources such as intertidal areas or agricultural and natural areas, for example due to increased flight costs, in a generalist seabird, the herring gull Larus argentatus. We use data from GPS trackers with tri-axial acceleration measurements that allow us to quantify time-energy budgets, representing energy expenditure during foraging trips of herring gulls for each habitat.

Results

We show that the rate of energy expenditure is on average 34% higher when individuals forage for high caloric prey in marine and urban areas compared to foraging for low caloric prey in intertidal and agricultural areas. Energetic estimates suggest that if birds would feed completely on these resources, they have to gather ~ 400 kJ per day more to compensate for the higher foraging costs.

Conclusions

Energy expenditure differs among foraging habitat and may thereby influence foraging decisions of individual herring gulls. As management of anthropogenic resources changes, so too may the costs and potential benefits of foraging strategies which are strongly tied to human activities.

Background

Many species experience a loss in resource availability due to human influences in their environment, but some species take advantage of resources that comes available due to human activities [1]. For example, predators such as red foxes Vulpes vulpes and coyotes Canis latrans have expanded their foraging activities to urban areas in recent decades to profit from anthropogenic resources [2,3,4,5]. Generalist species are especially suited to exploit human refuse, as they have a broad prey spectrum and exhibit flexibility in their behavior [5,6,7,8,9,10].

The resources or foraging patches individual animals choose to forage on may depend on the trade-off between costs and benefits of different foraging strategies [11,12,13]. For instance, some prey might have benefits like a high energetic value or they are beneficial for breeding success, but they might be energetically costly to forage on due to special foraging skills that need to be learned [14], a long searching or handling time [15] or a high level of predation [16] or competition [17].

A specific example of a generalist species that has to make foraging decisions in a landscape which has changed by humans is the herring gull Larus argentatus. Herring gulls have adapted their foraging behavior to human activities, and forage at refuse dump sites, waste treatment centers and on fisheries discards from commercial fisheries [18,19,20,21], profiting from relatively high caloric prey. Fishery discards and refuse were found to have a beneficial effect on reproductive success in several populations of gulls [19, 21,22,23], which suggests that animals foraging on these prey have a higher net energy intake and are thereby able to offer more food in terms of kilojoules to their offspring. The net energy intake is determined by the energy intake per unit time as well as the energetic costs of foraging per unit time [24], which might differ per foraging habitat. However, assessing and comparing foraging costs in terms of energy expenditure associated with different foraging habitats in the wild remains challenging. Even in relatively well studied species, such as the herring gull, little is known about the energetic investments in different foraging strategies and how differential costs may influence foraging decisions.

In this paper, we investigate whether energetic costs of foraging varies between different foraging habitats. We conducted our study in a population of herring gulls that is studied thoroughly over the last 10 years on the island of Texel, the Netherlands [25]. Intertidal areas provide bivalves, the predominant prey type for birds within this colony, but during chick rearing the diet becomes more diverse [23]. As shown in other gull studies [1, 26, 27], high caloric prey are important to ensure sufficient chick growth in this population [23]; breeding pairs that provision their chicks more regularly with refuse and fishery discards fledged more and larger chicks. Still, many individuals continue to forage mainly in intertidal areas even during chick rearing, suggesting that there are higher costs involved in foraging for the more beneficial high caloric prey (Table 1). We hypothesize that foraging for high caloric prey of anthropogenic origin is energetically more costly than foraging for the more predominant low caloric prey, as foraging for anthropogenic prey might require costly flight and competitive behavior. The higher energetic costs of foraging for anthropogenic prey may at least partially explain why some individuals prefer other foraging habitats.

Table 1 Overview of the diet of Herring Gulls breeding on Texel during the chick care of the breeding season

Bird-borne GPS trackers with tri-axial accelerometers, make it possible to measure behavior and estimate energy expenditure. Using more than 10 years of dietary data and color ring recordings, we link habitat use to prey types most likely to be acquired in each habitat (Table 1) [28, 29]. We tested whether the energy invested in foraging is higher in habitats containing prey of anthropogenic origin (marine and built up areas) than when foraging in habitats containing low caloric prey (intertidal areas and non-built up terrestrial areas) by comparing habitat use with energy expenditure during foraging trips. We quantified time energy budgets of herring gulls using GPS tracking and concomitant acceleration measurements. We show that more energy is invested in foraging in habitats with anthropogenic prey than when foraging in habitats with intertidal or terrestrial prey in our study system and we discuss the consequences of energetic costs of different foraging strategies in the context of a food landscape strongly influenced by humans considering what gulls have experienced over the past 40 years and what is expected in the coming decades.

Methods

The study was carried out between May 2013 and August 2016 at a breeding colony of approximately 4000 pairs of herring gulls which breed sympatrically with approximately 11,000 pairs of lesser black-backed gulls Larus fuscus at the island of Texel, the Netherlands (53°00′N, 04°43′E; 615,201.4 E, 5873649 N UTM zone 31; Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Map of the study system and habitat use (a) Overview of the study area, latitude and longitude are indicated on the x and y axis in UTM zone 31 (× 10.000), breeding colony is indicated with an asterix (615,201.4 E, 5873649 N). Foraging areas of category urban which contain refuse are indicated in dark grey. Another source of human waste is the closest waste treatment center to the colony, indicated with a circle. Foraging area of category marine with main prey fishery discards are North Sea and Wadden Sea. Foraging areas of category intertidal are indicated in the dashed area, but herring gulls also forage in the Wadden Sea on intertidal mudflats indicated in light grey. Terrestrial foraging areas are found on Texel and the mainland and are indicated in grey. b Space use of all the herring gulls used in the study (n = 17) during chick care in breeding seasons of 2013–2016, expressed in total time spent in minutes per square kilometer

GPS tracking

Thirty-one adult herring gulls (17 males and 14 females) were caught with walk-in traps during incubation between 2013 and 2015. Solar-powered GPS trackers of the UvA Bird Tracking System [30, 31] were mounted to the birds with a 3-g non-flexible Teflon harness on the back of the birds. As recommended for seabirds [32] GPS-tracker and harness together weighted less than 3% of the body mass of the birds which was on average 2.4% of female body mass and 2.1% of male body mass. These trackers measure, among others, the geographic location and time (UTC), and acceleration in three directions (surge, sway and heave). Tracking devices were calibrated to convert surge, sway and heave acceleration data to g-force (1 gn = 9.81 m/s2). At time of capture, body mass (g), wing (mm), tarsus (mm), head (mm) and bill (mm) lengths were taken. The birds were sexed on the basis of head plus bill length (mm) [33]. Birds were released after attaching the GPS tracker and taking body measurements, which took approximately 20 min. The tracking frequency was set to every 10 min inside the breeding territory and every 5 min outside the breeding territory. As we had the possibility to change measurement frequency while the tracker was on the bird, we took occasionally higher resolution measurements. For better comparison, we resampled the data in this case to the standard measurement frequency. Tri-axial acceleration was periodically measured, only outside the breeding colony, at 20 Hz for 1 s directly following a GPS fix.

Data selection and processing

For our analysis, we used GPS data of individuals that had a nest with chicks for at least 5 days after hatching of the first egg and we only used data up to 10 days after hatching to control for differences in demands when chicks grow bigger. We studied the costs of foraging during chick care, as prey choice have shown to be important for reproductive success in this period [23].

We compared the costs of foraging for different resources by analyzing foraging trips. We defined a foraging trip as a continuous period beginning when an individual travelled more than 100 m from its nest and ending when the individual returned to within 100 m of the nest. For each GPS location we attributed a ‘centered duration’ which was calculated by averaging the backward and forward time intervals between locations. The centered duration was used in further analysis to calculate trip duration and time spent in flight.

To calculate energy expenditure of foraging, we made use of acceleration data. Each acceleration measurement was attributed to one of 11 behaviors. We classified behaviors by training a random forest machine-learning algorithm for the classification of accelerometer data [31, 34]. We used two datasets to train the model on 11 different behaviors. The first is based on annotated accelerometer data of lesser black-backed gulls [34], which is a species that is comparable in size and morphology, and most behavior with the herring gull. The second dataset contained accelerometer data of herring gull specific foraging behavior which was annotated with synchronized video recordings [31]. The final random forest model used had an accuracy for predicting the 11 behaviors of 94%. The 11 behaviors were then aggregated into four behaviors, which are inactive behavior (sitting, standing or floating), terrestrial movement (terrestrial locomotion, looking and standing while looking for food, handling prey and other), soaring flight (soaring and maneuvering) and flapping flight (regular and extreme flapping flight).

Gaps in the GPS measurements occurred, and we excluded trips from analysis when gaps were bigger than 20 min when outside the breeding colony. Besides, we only used trips of which at least 80% of the GPS fixes were accompanied with acceleration measurements. After data selection, 605 trips were included in the analysis of 17 different individual herring gulls. One herring gull was included in the analysis for three consecutive years.

Energetic costs

To compare energy expenditure of foraging in different habitat, we used three proxies for energy expenditure: (1) trip duration (h), (2) duration spent on flapping flight per trip (h) and (3) average estimated hourly energy expenditure per trip (kJ h− 1). Trip duration was calculated by summing all the ‘centered durations’ per trip.

The duration spent on flapping flight per trip was calculated by summing the ‘centered duration’ of measurements which were assigned to flapping flight. The duration of flapping flight per trip was used as a proxy for energy expenditure as flapping flight is thought to be the most energetically expensive form of locomotion compared to other behaviors [35].

We estimated the rate of energy expenditure per trip by estimating metabolic rates in kilojoules for the four classified behaviors per individual herring gull [31]. We calculated the basal metabolic rate (BMR) per individual in kJ day− 1 as 2.3 × body mass(g)0.774 at catching per individual (mean ± standard deviation of 19.64 ± 1.31 kJ h− 1 for all animals in our study) [36]. As the BMR does not account for thermoregulation when temperature is lower or higher than the thermo-neutral zone, digestion or little body movements, we calculated resting metabolic rate (RMR) as 1.7 × BMR [37, 38], with an average of 33.39 ± 2.22 kJ h− 1 over all individuals. For the energetic cost of the behavior ‘inactive’ we used RMR. We estimated the energetic cost of ‘terrestrial movement’ as 2 × BMR. This estimation was based on a formula of costs for terrestrial movement of Bautista et al. (1998) [39]; costs terrestrial movement (kJ day− 1) = (5.6 × Wkg0.246 + 11.4 × Wkg-0.285 × v) × 86.4, where Wkg is body mass (kg) and v is velocity in m s− 1. As v we used 0.4 m s− 1 which is the average velocity while walking of herring gulls with GPS trackers in this study. The formula of Bautista et al. is based on data of starlings Sturnus vulgaris, but two studies in barnacle geese Branta leucopsis show similar energy expenditure of terrestrial locomotion compared to basal metabolic rate [40, 41]. The cost of soaring flight was estimated as 2 × RMR [37] and the cost of flapping flight was estimated as 7 × RMR [35]. We calculated energy expenditure by summing the ‘centered duration’ for the four classified behaviors per trip and multiplying these with the energetic estimations (hour− 1) of these four behaviors. To calculate energetic costs per hour, we divided energy expenditure of the whole trip by its trip duration.

Habitat use

To compare trips with different habitat use, we calculated the percentage of time gulls spent per trip in four foraging habitats which are termed (1) urban (2) marine, (3) intertidal and (4) terrestrial. We expect urban and marine environments to include predominantly high caloric prey (e.g. refuse and fishery discards) whereas intertidal and terrestrial include mainly low caloric prey such as bivalves and crabs and terrestrial invertebrates (Table 1)(Fig. 1).

To calculate habitat use, we took all the GPS positions outside the colony into account, apart from when animals are commuting (i.e. when an individual is flying in a straight line from one place to the other). To select the commuting GPS positions, we made use of an expectation maximization binary clustering for behavioral annotation developed by Garriga et al. 2016 [42]. This clustering algorithm uses turning angle and velocity obtained from successive locations to cluster GPS positions in four behavioral categories which are High velocity/Low turn (HL), High velocity/High turn (HH), Low velocity/Low turn (LL), Low velocity/High turn (LH). We assumed that an animal is commuting when velocity is high and turning angle low (HL category). We applied the clustering algorithm per individual in a given year using the r package EmbC and applied a pre-smoothing procedure which is provided by the packages to account for temporal associations.

Subsequently, we coupled every non-commuting GPS position to one of the four foraging habitats using several shapefiles of foraging areas around the colony and the behavioral classifications. We used the following shapefiles of foraging areas: North Sea and Wadden Sea, urban areas, breakwaters, beach & intertidal mudflats, agriculture & natural land (Additional file 1: Table S1). Often, the area around the breakwater, beach and intertidal mudflats is also available for foraging during low tide (personal observations) and therefore we also assigned GPS points which were assigned to North Sea or Wadden Sea closer than 50 m to breakwater, beach or intertidal mudflats to the intertidal habitat type. GPS positions where assigned to urban habitat when in urban areas. GPS positions where assigned to the marine when in the North Sea and Wadden Sea and on intertidal mudflats with the behavioral mode flying or floating. Almost all fish that the herring gulls of this colony consume originates from fishery discards [25] (p.337). GPS positions were assigned to intertidal habitat when on breakwaters and beach and on intertidal mudflats when the behavioral mode was resting or terrestrial movement which indicates foraging on intertidal mudflats. GPS positions where assigned to terrestrial habitat when in agricultural or natural areas.

Statistical analysis

To test whether foraging costs differ depending on the foraging habitat, we divided all foraging trips in five habitat categories based on the time an animal spent in every foraging habitat. A foraging trip with 50% or more of its non-commuting GPS positions in one of the four foraging habitats, was assigned to category urban, marine, intertidal or terrestrial. A foraging trip with less than 50% of its non-commuting GPS positions in one of the four foraging habitats was assigned to a fifth category; mixed. Figure 2 shows examples of foraging trips of four categories.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Examples of four foraging trips which are indicated in four different colors. Latitude and longitude are indicated on the x and y axis in UTM (× 10.000). Every point corresponds to one GPS fix and the shape of the points indicates the behavior of the animal (Flapping flight, soaring flight, terrestrial activity, inactive or not annotated). In dark red; a foraging trip of category urban. In light red; a foraging trip of category marine. In blue; a foraging trip of category intertidal. In green a foraging trip of category terrestrial

We tested the hypothesis whether trips of the five habitat categories differed in energy expenditure by fitting linear mixed-effect models. We fitted a separate model for each of the response variables: trip duration in hours, duration spent on flapping flight in hours and rate of energy expenditure per hour. We included habitat categories as fixed effect and bird ID as random intercept in the models. Response variables in the models were transformed to obtain normality and homogeneity of variance; trip duration and energy expenditure were transformed with the natural logarithm, duration in flapping flight was transformed with the square root. Commuting GPS fixes were included in the calculation of these response variables. The models were tested against a null model which only contained the random factor. When the model was significantly better than the null model, we performed post hoc Tukey testing for the fixed part of the model to test which habitat category differed using the lsmeans function from the lmer Test library [43]. P-values, ∆AICc and the model estimates and standard errors were reported, as well as marginal and conditional R squared values for mixed models [44]. Although we did not have specific hypotheses about the role of sex, mass or sampling year in this study, we did explore these factors in the models by comparing the residuals of the model with sex, sampling year and mass. After this analysis, we concluded that we could ignore these factors in our analyses.

Results

Habitat use and foraging trips

During the non-commuting phase of foraging trips, herring gulls spent most time in areas with relatively lower caloric prey; most time was spent in the intertidal habitat followed by terrestrial habitat (Table 2). Less time was spent in areas with relatively high caloric prey; gulls spent from these habitat types a bit more time in marine habitat than in urban habitat (Table 2). As a result, the number of foraging trips per category were also not evenly distributed; most trips were assigned to intertidal and least to category mixed (Table 2).

Table 2 Habitat use outside the breeding colony of non-commuting GPS data of all herring gulls used in this study (n = 17) and foraging trip details (n = 605)

To test whether habitat use differed in energy expenditure per trip, we made use of three proxies for energy expenditure which were trip duration, time spent on flapping flight and energy expenditure for which we also included the time spent on commuting. Duration of foraging trips varied widely (range: 0.6–14.4 h, mean ± SE: 2.8 ± 1.9 h) and mean foraging duration was highest for urban and mixed foraging trips. Similarly, we found high variation in the proportion of time spent on flapping flight per trip (range: 0–1 per h trip, mean ± SE: 0.21 ± 0.15 h) and the estimated energy spent per trip (range: 11 - 1006 kJ, mean ± SE: 224 ± 163 kJ) and they were both highest for urban trips. But the estimated energy spent per hour (range: 26 - 252 kJ, mean ± SE: 78 ± 29 kJ) was highest for marine foraging trips. Mean trip duration, time spent on flapping and the estimation of energy expenditure per trip were lowest for terrestrial trips, but estimated energy expenditure per hour was lowest for both terrestrial and intertidal trips.

Link between energetic costs and habitat use during foraging trips

Foraging costs were also significantly higher in urban and marine habitats than foraging in intertidal or terrestrial foraging habitats, while mixed foraging trips had similar energetic costs as trips to urban and marine habitat (Table 3). Duration of trips differed between the categories; urban and mixed trips were significantly longer than trips to marine and intertidal habitat and trips of category terrestrial were shorter than trips of all other categories (Fig. 4a). But the higher energetic costs of urban, marine and mixed trips are mainly caused by relatively more time spent on flapping flight per trip (Figs. 3 and 4). More specifically, the trips in category urban, marine and mixed included more time spent on flapping flight and a higher estimated energy expenditure per hour than trips in category intertidal and terrestrial (Table 3; p < 0.05), while energy expenditure per hour did not differ between intertidal and terrestrial trips (Fig. 4b and c).

Table 3 Model results of the relationship between habitat use and response variables the logarithm of trip duration in hours (Log (Duration)), the square root of duration of flapping flight in hours (sqrt (Flapping)), and the logarithm of energy expenditure in kJ per hour (log (energy))
Fig. 3
figure 3

Trip duration and time spent (h) on the different behavioral categories per habitat category (n = 605 foraging trips). Trips on which individuals spent more than 50% in one habitat were assigned to main habitat category urban, marine, intertidal or terrestrial. Trips on which individuals spent less than 50% in one habitat were assigned to main habitat category mixed

Fig. 4
figure 4

The relationship between habitat use per habitat category and energetic costs. The boxplots show the median values of energetic costs per habitat category. a-c represents habitat category in relation to (a) trip duration in hours (b) Proportion of time in flapping flight and (c) energy expenditure in kilojoules per hour

Discussion

By comparing foraging trips of different habitat use, we found, in line with our expectations, that foraging at sea for discards or in dumps or cities where most human refuse is obtained was energetically costlier (about 34 higher costs per hour) than foraging in other habitats. Mixed foraging strategies (the use of more foraging habitats) had a similar energy expenditure as foraging trips towards anthropogenic resources (urban and marine trips). Higher costs resulted in particular from the time spent in flapping flight, but mixed and urban foraging trips were also longer in duration. Foraging in terrestrial or intertidal habitats was comparable in energetic costs per hour, but foraging in terrestrial habitat was least costly per trip, because the duration of these trips was shorter. We discuss the consequences of these foraging costs considering the past and future changes in the food landscape and costs and benefits of prey.

We used rough estimates for energy expenditure in this study, and compare them with other studies to determine whether our estimates were biologically meaningful. These studies described below measured food intake or energy expenditure of adult seabirds. A study on lesser black-backed gulls in captivity measured a fish intake of 900–1400 kJ per day [45] and with an assimilation efficiency of 75% [46], these gulls used 28–44 kJ per hour. This gull species is smaller than the herring gull and the birds were not able to show energetically costly behavior like searching for food and flying. Another study on black-legged kittiwakes Rissa tridactyla, seabirds which are half the weight of herring gulls, found that birds used 41 kJ per hour during foraging trips [47]. Our estimations of an average energy use of 78 kJ per hour during foraging trips seem to be quite reasonable, compared to measurements from other seabirds.

Foraging for anthropogenic resources is energetically expensive in our system but how big are these costs in terms of quantity of prey? The average length of discarded flatfish that is found in the breeding colony is 12–13 cm with an energetic value of 84 kJ per fish [48, 49]. Daily energetic costs per day for an animal solely foraging for fishery discards are 1462 kJ per day, assuming 14 h in the colony spending 33 kJ per hour and 10 h outside the colony spending 100 kJ per hour (Table 2). The average assimilation efficiency is 75% [46], so such an animal has to catch 23 fishes (1949 kJ) for its own subsistence. The amount of prey that it catches should be higher, as birds also have to gather food for chicks. Compared to an animal foraging solely in intertidal or terrestrial habitat which costs about 70 kJ per hour (daily costs about 1162 kJ), it would need to catch 5 flatfish more per day to compensate for the higher foraging costs.

When animals forage for anthropogenic prey, they have to catch more prey to compensate for higher foraging costs. However, the differences in energetic costs per hour between resting metabolic rate and foraging are in fact considerably larger, respectively 33 kJ and 70–100 kJ depending on foraging habitat (Table 2). The total time a gull spends on foraging might actually be more important in terms of its energetic costs than in which habitat a gull forages. A gull that mostly forages in urban or marine habitat could compensate for its higher foraging costs by conducting fewer foraging trips per day and can thus have similar daily costs compared to a gull that mostly forages in terrestrial or intertidal habitats. Among individuals in our study, this does not seem to be the case. The average number of trips per day differs considerably per individual, but individuals that forage more for anthropogenic resources do not have less trips per day (Additional file 1: Table S1). Interestingly, there does seem to be a correlation between the number of trips per day and the time spent in intertidal area. Individuals that spent most of their trips in intertidal areas seem to have a higher amount of trips per day compared to individuals that do not forage often in intertidal habitat (Additional file 1: Table S2 and Figure S1), suggesting that the low hourly costs of foraging in intertidal area are cancelled out by foraging more times a day. This relationship is mainly caused by two individuals, so a larger sample size would be needed to investigate this relationship more thoroughly.

Individual animals have to make foraging decisions based on the advantages and disadvantages between different foraging strategies [11, 50, 51]. One of the main benefits of foraging for refuse and fishery discards for herring gulls is the high caloric value per gram prey which helps reaching energetic demands of growing chicks (Table 1) [19, 21,22,23]. We found that herring gulls spent more energy to obtain these high quality prey by making longer trips and flying more (Table 2). That central-place foragers like breeding gulls spent more energy to obtain prey of higher quality has been found before. For example, Ring-billed gulls Larus delawarensis travelled further for foraging patches that provided higher mean energy intake, like refuse dumps [11], while foraging patches with lower mean energy intake, like agricultural fields, were only visited closer to the breeding colony.

To understand the net energy gain, it is also important to learn about the food intake per unit time. Although we have indirect proof that animals foraging for high caloric prey like fishery discards and human waste have a higher net energy gain, because of their better growing and surviving chicks [23], we miss a direct measurement of food intake per unit time. In future, we hope to be able to measure food intake per habitat by using detailed accelerometer data and video recordings of the different habitats. Another possibility to estimate energy intake is to use the dynamic energy budget model (DEB) [52]. This model can be used to estimate energy intake of growing chicks based on their weight, which will give a more precise estimate of prey intake in terms of kilojoules brought to the nest, even on a daily basis [53].

Characteristics of prey other than energetic gain and loss are also important for gulls, like the availability and predictability of prey (Table 1). Prey in terrestrial habitat are not always available during the breeding season. Earthworms, for example, are only available when the soil is moist, and agricultural areas provide most food when farmers are ploughing [11, 54, 55]. This might explain the shorter time herring gulls spent in this habitat, despite the low foraging costs, compared to intertidal foraging habitat (Table 2; 21% of foraging time). On the contrary, prey of the intertidal habitat (mostly bivalves) are very predictable (during low tide) and available in large amounts every day which might be the reason that most of the foraging time is spent in this habitat.

Although herring gulls have been benefiting from human resources, this situation is changing and resources have been decreasing the last decades. National and international legislation caused the decrease of the number of refuse dumps and the amount of refuse in most European countries including the Netherlands [25, 56,57,58]. Furthermore, fishing fleet densities in the region have decreased and legislation towards discarding bycatch became stricter [59,60,61,62]. Resource availability for the herring gulls breeding on Texel have changed in a similar way as the number of fishing vessels and refuse dumps in the close surroundings decreased a lot [22, 25]. Only 30 years ago, there was still a refuse dump within 4 km of the breeding colony (Fig. 5), whereas now the closest waste treatment center is 35 km away. Unfortunately, we cannot compare foraging costs between the past and now, but using the knowledge we have gained in the present study, we could compare an hypothetical foraging trip to the disappeared refuse dump with a foraging trip to the closest waste treatment center now, which is presented in Fig. 5. When we assume that foraging time is similar between the two places (about 1.5 h), the costs of foraging on refuse dumps might have more than doubled for these gulls due to the increased cost of flight. In future, the amount of refuse and fishery discards will probably be even less. Fishing effort is still decreasing and a more sustainable way of fishing is in development [63]. This will probably increase time and energy needed for foraging because of higher competition.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Comparison between imaginary foraging trip to the closest refuse dump 30 years ago (4 km distance of the colony; in orange) and a foraging trip to the closest waste treatment center (35 km distance of the colony; in dark red). Latitude and longitude are indicated on the x and y axis in UTM (× 10.000). Breeding colony is situated at 615201.4 E, 5873649 N UTM. Every point corresponds to one GPS fix and the size of the points indicates the behavior of the animal (resting, terrestrial locomotion, soaring flight or flapping flight). Estimated energetic costs of these trips of an animal with a body mass of 1000 g are respectively 160 kJ and 362 kJ

If the foraging costs for these resources become higher than the reproductive gains, gulls might have to change their behavior. Gulls could decrease foraging costs, for instance by decreasing the distance that they have to commute between breeding territory and foraging areas by breeding closer to urban areas or refuse dumps. Gulls do breed on rooftops in increasing numbers, with sometimes higher breeding performance [64,65,66,67]. Although the foraging behavior of these gulls is largely unknown, the proximity of foraging possibilities to a colony does affect the composition of the diet of that colony [68,69,70]. A recent study on breeding colonies of herring gulls in the USA, showed that birds breeding closer to urbanization had shorter foraging trips [70]. But whether gulls will move between breeding locations is questionable, as gulls of this colony rarely move to another breeding colony, even though breeding success is low [25] (p.326). The location of the breeding site on Texel does not seem very favorable anymore in terms of food sources compared to 30 years ago [22]. Still, there are no indications of a decrease in breeding pairs over the last decade. Whether individual herring gulls of this colony will adapt their behavior is unclear, but these human induced changes in the environment create a natural experiment to study the effect on animals’ behavior. In future, we could use these environmental changes to look into whether individuals will respond to these changes by adapting their foraging or breeding behavior.

Conclusions

We studied costs of foraging in different habitats in the herring gull, to get a better understanding of the factors that can shape animals’ foraging decisions in an environment which is highly affected by humans. We found that foraging for high caloric prey of anthropogenic origin is costly in terms of foraging effort compared to other foraging options, but these prey are beneficial for chick growth and survival. Foraging for less beneficial prey for reproduction, like terrestrial and intertidal prey, were less costly in terms of foraging effort. Recent and future alterations in fishery discards and garbage management will increase foraging costs of these prey, which will affect the balance between costs and benefits. Gulls might have to adapt or change foraging or breeding behavior, although it is not clear whether they will be able to do this due limited flexibility.

References

  1. Weiser EL, Powell AN. Does garbage in the diet improve reproductive output of glaucous gulls? Condor. 2010;112:530–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Chautan M, Pontier D, Artois M. Role of rabies in recent demographic changes in red fox (Vulpes vulpes) populations in Europe. Mammalia. 2000;64:391–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Contesse P, Hegglin D, Gloor S, Bontadina F, Deplazes P. The diet of urban foxes (Vulpes vulpes) and the availability of anthropogenic food in the city of Zurich, Switzerland. Mamm Biol. 2004;69:81–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Gompper ME. Top in the Carnivores Suburbs? Ecological by Colonization of North-eastern North America by Coyotes. Bioscience. 2002;52:185–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Murray M, Cembrowski A, Latham ADM, Lukasik VM, Pruss S, St Clair CC. Greater consumption of protein-poor anthropogenic food by urban relative to rural coyotes increases diet breadth and potential for human-wildlife conflict. Ecography (Cop). 2015;38:1235–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Angert AL, Crozier LG, Rissler LJ, Gilman SE, Tewksbury JJ, Chunco AJ. Do species’ traits predict recent shifts at expanding range edges? Ecol Lett. 2011;14:677–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Gompper ME, Vanak AT. Subsidized predators, landscapes of fear and disarticulated carnivore communities. Anim Conserv. 2008;11:13–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Micheli F. Behavioural plasticity in prey-size selectivity of the blue crab Callinectes sapidus feeding on bivalve prey. J Anim Ecol. 1995;64:63–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. O’Brien EL, Burger AE, Dawson RD. Foraging decision rules and prey species preferences of northwestern crows (Corvus caurinus). Ethology. 2005;111:77–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Plumer L, Davison J, Saarma U. Rapid urbanization of red foxes in Estonia: distribution, behaviour, attacks on domestic animals, and health-risks related to zoonotic diseases. PLoS One. 2014;9:1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Patenaude-Monette M, Bélisle M, Giroux J-F. Balancing energy budget in a central-place forager: which habitat to select in a heterogeneous environment? PLoS One. 2014;9:e102162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Hutchings MR, Gordon IJ, Kyriazakis I, Jackson F. Sheep avoidance of faeces-contaminated patches leads to a trade-off between intake rate of forage and parasitism in subsequent foraging decisions. Anim Behav. 2001;62:955–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Houston AI, McNamara JM, Hutchinsom JMC. General results concerning the trade-off between gaining energy and avoiding predation. Philos Trans R Soc B-Biol Sci. 1993;341:375–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hand CE, Sanders FJ, Jodice PGR. Foraging proficiency during the nonbreeding season of a specialized forager : are juvenile American oystercatchers “ bumble-beaks ” compared to adults ? Condor. 2010;112:670–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Hawlena D, Pérez-Mellado V. Change your diet or die : predator-induced shifts in insectivorous lizard feeding ecology. Oecologia. 2009;161:411–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Houston AI. Diet selection. In: R.N. H, editor. The importance of state. London: Blackwell Scientific Publications; 1993. p. 10–31.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Slotow R, Paxinos E. Intraspecific competition influences food return-predation risk trade-off by white-crowned sparrows. Condor. 1997;99:642–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Pons J. Effects of changes in the availability of human refuse on breeding parameters in a herring gull Larus argentatus population in Brittany, France. Ardea. 1992;80:143–50.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Hunt GLJ. Influence of food distribution and human disturbance on the reproductive success of herring gulls. Ecology. 1972;53:1051–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Camphuysen CJ. Herring gull Larus argentatus and lesser black-backed Gull L fuscus feeding at fishing vessels in the breeding season: competitive scavenging versus efficient flying. ARDEA. 1995;83:365–80.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Pons J, Migot P. Life-history strategy of the herring gull: changes in survival and fecundity in a population subjected to various feeding conditions. J Anim Ecol. 1995;64:592–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Spaans AL. On the feeding ecology of the herring gull Larus argentatus Pont. In the northern part of the Netherlands. Ardea. 1971;59:73–188.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Van Donk S, Camphuysen CJ, Shamoun-Baranes J, van der Meer J. The most common diet results in low reproduction in a generalist seabird. Ecol Evol. 2017;7:4620–9.

  24. Ydenberg RC, Welham CVJ. Time and energy constraints and the relationships between currencies in foraging theory. Behav Ecol. 1992;5:28–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Camphuysen CJ. A historical ecology of two closely related gull species (Laridae): multiple adaptations to a man-made environment. Groningen: Ph.D. Thesis, University of Groningen; 2013.

  26. Annett CA, Pierotti R. Long-term reproductive output in western gulls: consequences of alternate tactics in diet choice. Ecology. 1999;80:288–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Bukacińska M, Bukaciński D, Spaans AL. Attendance and diet in relation to breeding success in herring gulls (Larus argentatus). Auk. 1996;113:300–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Masello JF, Wikelski M, Voigt CC, Quillfeldt P. Distribution patterns predict individual specialization in the diet of dolphin gulls. PLoS One. 2013;8:e67714.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Woo KJ, Elliott KH, Davidson M, Gaston AJ, Davoren GK. Individual specialization in diet by a generalist marine predator reflects specialization in foraging behaviour. J Anim Ecol. 2008;77:1082–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Bouten W, Baaij EW, Shamoun-Baranes J, Camphuysen KCJ. A flexible GPS tracking system for studying bird behaviour at multiple scales. J Ornithol. 2013;154:571–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. van Donk S, Shamoun-baranes J, Bouten W, van der Meer J, Camphuysen KCJ. Individual differences in foraging site fidelity are not related to time-activity budgets in herring gulls. 2018 Early View.

  32. Phillips RA, Xavier JC, Croxall JP. Effects of satellite transmitters on albatrosses and petrels. Auk. 2003;120:1082–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Coulson J, Thomas CS, Butterfield JEL, Duncan N, Monaghan PC. The use of head and bill length to sex live gulls Laridae. Ibis (Lond 1859). 1983;125:549–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Shamoun-Baranes J, Bouten W, van Loon EE, Meijer C, Camphuysen CJ. Flap or soar? How a flight generalist responds to its aerial environment. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2016;371:415–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Tucker VA. Metabolism during flight in the laughing gull, Larus atricilla. Am J Phys. 1972;222:237–45.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Bryant DM, Furness RW. Basal metabolic rates of North Atlantic seabirds. Ibis (Lond 1859). 1995;137:219–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Baudinette RV, Schmidt-Nielsen K. Energy cost of gliding flight in herring gulls. Nature. 1974;248:83–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Furness RW. Energy requirements of seabird communities: a bioenergetics model. J Anim Ecol. 1978;47:39–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Bautista LM, Tinbergen J, Wiersma P, Kacelnik A. Optimal foraging and beyond: how starlings cope with changes in food availability. Am Nat. 1998;152:543–61.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Nudds RL, Gardiner JD, Tickle PG, Codd JR. Energetics and kinematics of walking in the barnacle goose (Branta leucopsis). Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol. 2010;156:318–24.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Nolet BA, Butler PJ, Masman D, Woakes AJ. Estimation of daily energy expenditure from heart rate and doubly labeled water in exercising geese. Physiol Zool. 1992;65:1188–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Garriga J, Palmer JRB, Oltra A, Bartumeus F. Expectation-maximization binary clustering for behavioural annotation. PLoS One. 2016;11:1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff P, Christensen R. lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J Stat Softw. 2017;82:1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Nakagawa S, Schielzeth H. A general and simple method for obtaining R 2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2013;4:133–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Hilton GM, Furness RW, Houston DC. The effects of diet switching and mixing on digestion in seabirds. Funct Ecol. 2000;14:145–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Castro G, Stoyan N, Myers JP. Assimilation efficiency in birds: a function of taxon or food type? Comp Biochem Physiol A Physiol. 1989;92:271–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Gabrielsen GW, Mehlum F, Nagy KA. Daily energy expenditure and energy utilization of free-ranging black-legged kittiwakes. Condor. 1987;89:126–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Camphuysen CJ, Henderson PA. North Sea fish and their remains. North Sea fish and their remains. Den Burg: Royal Netherlands Institute for Sea Research/Pisces Conservation Ltd: [s.l.]; 2017.

  49. Garthe S, Camphuysen CJ, Furness RW. Amounts of discards by commercial fisheries and their significance as food for seabirds in the North Sea. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 1996;136:1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Oudman T, Onrust J, de Fouw J, Spaans B, Piersma T, van Gils JA. Digestive capacity and toxicity cause mixed diets in red knots that maximize energy intake rate. Am Nat. 2014;183:650–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Rozen-Rechels D, van Beest FM, Richard E, Uzal A, Medill SA, Mcloughlin PD. Density dependent, central-place foraging in a grazing herbivore: competition and tradeoffs in time allocation near water. Oikos. 2015;124:1142–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Kooijman SALM. Dynamic Energy Budget theory for metabolic organisation. Third edition. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press; 2010.

  53. Teixeira CMGL, Sousa T, Marques GM, Domingos T, LM KS a. A new perspective on the growth pattern of the wandering albatross (Diomedea exulans) through DEB theory. J Sea Res Elsevier BV. 2014;94:117–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Sibly R, McCleery RH. The distribution between feeding sites of herring gulls breeding at Walney island, U.K. J Anim Ecol. 1983;52:51–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Kruuk H. Foraging and spatial organisation of the European badger, Meles meles L. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1978;4:75–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. European Environment Agency (EEA). Less household waste going to landfill in Europe. 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Kohler N, Perry E. Implementation of the landfill directive in the 15 member states of the european union; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Rijkswaterstaat. Afvalverwerking in Nederland : gegevens 2015 Werkgroep Afvalregistratie. Utrecht: Rijkswaterstaat; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Rijnsdorp AD, Poos JJ, Quirijns FJ, HilleRisLambers R, De Wilde JW, Den Heijer WM. The arms race between fishers. J Sea Res. 2008;60:126–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Poos J-J. Effort allocation of the Dutch beam trawl fleet. 2010;Ph.D.-thes.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Rijnsdorp AD, Poos JJ, Quirijns FJ. Spatial dimension and exploitation dynamics of local fishing grounds by fishers targeting several flatfish species. Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 2011;68:1064–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Borges L. The evolution of a discard policy in Europe. Fish Fish. 2015;16:534–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Bicknell AWJ, Oro D, Camphuysen KCJ, Votier SC. Potential consequences of discard reform for seabird communities. Blanchard J, editor. J Appl Ecol. 2013;50:649–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Rock P. Urban gulls: problems and solutions. Br Birds. 2005;98:338–55.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Soldatini C, Albores-Barajas YV, Mainardi D, Monaghan P. Roof nesting by gulls for better or worse? Ital J Zool. 2008;75:295–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Mitchell PI, Newton SF, Ratcliffe N, Dunn TE. Seabird populations of Britain and Ireland: results of the seabird 2000 census (1998–2002); 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Monaghan P. Aspects of the breeding biology of herring gulls (Larus argentatus) in urban colonies. Ibis (Lond 1859). 1979;121:475–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. O’Hanlon NJ, McGill RAR, Nager RG. Increased use of intertidal resources benefits breeding success in a generalist gull species. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2017;574:193–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Washburn BE, Bernhardt GE, Kutschbach-Brohl L, Chipman RB, Francoeur LC. Foraging ecology of four Gull species at a coastal–urban Interface. Condor. 2013;115:67–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Fuirst M, Veit RR, Hahn M, Dheilly N, Thorne LH. Effects of urbanization on the foraging ecology and microbiota of the generalist seabird Larus argentatus. PLoS One. 2018;13:1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

These studies are part of a long-term demographical and ecological study on sympatric breeding gulls by the Royal Netherlands Institute for Sea Research (NIOZ) at Texel. We thank Staatsbosbeheer Texel for permission to work in Kelderhuispolder, a nature reserve closed for the general public. Particularly, we thank Aris Ellen, Glenn van Ginkel, Marcel Groenendaal, and Erik van der Spek for help and cooperation. We thank Willem Bouten for his advice, Edwin Baaij for his technical support of UvA-BiTS and Jan Baert for helping with analytical problems. We thank all the volunteers that helped in catching the gulls and other fieldwork over the years.

The UvA-BiTS infrastructure was facilitated by Infrastructures for E-Science, developed with the support of the Netherlands eScience Centre (NLeSC) and LifeWatch, and conducted on the Dutch National E-Infrastructure with support from the SURF Foundation. We thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

Funding

Not applicable.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. When publication is considered, raw data files used in the analysis will be made public in 4TU.Centre for Research Data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

SvD carried out the statistical analysis, and drafted the manuscript with support from JSB, JvdM and KCJC; KCJC leads long term study on herring gulls; KCJC and SvD conducted field work, JSB assisted with GPS tracking. SvD, JSB, JvdM and KCJC discussed the analytical approach; All authors discussed results and contributed to the final manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Susanne van Donk.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

We have permission to work in the colony and catch, ring and tag Herring Gulls under the permit Art 75 of the Dutch ‘Flora & Faunawet’ FF/75A/2014003, an annually renewed ringing permit issued by Vogeltrekstation Wageningen for ringer-licensed E29 Camphuysen, an annually renewed research permit issued by Saatsbosbeheer Divisie grond en gebouwen, and a permit for animal experiments (tracking herring gulls) issued by the ‘Nederlandse Voedsel- en Warenautoriteit’, Ministry of Economics, TRC/VWA/20132090.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Table S1. for shapefiles used to determine habitat use. Table S2 and Figure S1 for showing the relationship between the total time foraging and habitat use per individual. (PDF 195 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

van Donk, S., Shamoun-Baranes, J., van der Meer, J. et al. Foraging for high caloric anthropogenic prey is energetically costly. Mov Ecol 7, 17 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40462-019-0159-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40462-019-0159-3

Keywords