Skip to main content

Evolution of contact and alarm calls in the Kenyan endemic Hinde’s babbler (Aves: Passeriformes)

Abstract

Background

Spatial isolation, diverging environmental conditions and social structures may lead to the differentiation of various traits, e.g. molecules, morphology and behaviour. Bird calls may provide important information on effects of geographic isolation and may reflect diverging ecological conditions related to altitude. Furthermore, bird calls are strongly shaped by the social behaviour of species. The Kenyan endemic bird Hinde’s Babbler, Turdoides hindei, is a cooperative breeder existing in distinct family groups. The species occurs in five isolated population groups at different altitudes across its distribution range in south-eastern Kenya. With this model species we test for potential effects of geographic isolation, diverging altitudes, and social structures. We recorded and analysed contact and alarm calls of T. hindei, including its entire distribution range and all existing population groups.

Results

Our data show significant differentiation of call characteristics among population groups across the species’ distribution range. This differentiation is correlated with geographical distance, but also with altitude. We also found strong call differentiation among neighbouring family groups. Call differentiation of contact calls was comparatively high in comparison to alarm calls, which showed a lower degree of divergence.

Conclusion

Our data show that call differentiation is governed by geographic isolation as well as altitude. Diverging degrees of call differentiation in contact and alarm calls suggests that both call types are under different selective pressures. Alarm calls are required to be understood by all members of the species across the entire distribution range and thus call differentiation is lower. In contrast, contact calls are more specific and differ even among neighbouring families supporting the maintenance of distinct bird families and groups.

Background

Species demanding specific habitat characteristics frequently occur in geographically isolated population groups, and thus provide the prerequisite for long-term geographic isolation of populations [1]. In addition, diverging environmental conditions may also drive the isolation of populations [2]. Both drivers may lead to the differentiation of taxa, at molecular, morphological and behavioural level [1, 2]. The interplay of both forces, geographic isolation and diverging environmental conditions, is complemented by the effects of social structures, which may further drive intraspecific differentiation across distribution ranges of taxa [3,4,5]. Yet, depending on the trait under study, one evolutionary force may be more important than the other.

Traits involved in communication, such as vocalisations are assumed to be under strong selection [3]. For example, bird calls are important behavioural evolutionary traits. It has been shown that call structure is partially heritable, but also culturally transmitted via learning and frequently diverges among local populations, especially when these are geographically or ecologically isolated [4, 5]. Bird vocalisations have an important function in intraspecific communication and may be used to hold contact to other members within a family group [6], or during mating [7]. However, they are also frequently used as alarm signals to warn for predators [8, 9].

In this study we recorded bird calls for the Kenyan endangered endemic Hinde’s Babbler Turdoides hindei (Aves, Passeriformes). We considered its entire distribution range across south-eastern Kenya [10] and recorded individuals belonging to 20 families representing the five still existing population groups [10]. The species can be found at humid and cool habitats at higher elevations at the foothills of Mt. Kenya, the Aberdares and the Thika Plateau, but also in the dry and hot lowlands around Machakos, and Kitui with Makueni [11]. Hence, our sampling represents different altitudes and geographically isolated population groups.

Turdoides hindei is a cooperative breeder and thus forms distinct family groups with sizes ranging between 2 and 10 individuals [12]. This allows to also study family specific call characteristics, which may play an outstanding role in social species, such as cooperative breeding birds. However, acoustic datasets for such social species across their whole range are rare, but may provide insights into the importance of geographic isolation, environmental conditions (highland versus lowland), and social structures for divergence in acoustic traits.

We observed families and the behaviour of single individuals and recorded contact and alarm calls. In addition, we recorded individuals being members of neighbouring family groups inhabiting the riparian forests of three river systems in the semiarid dry lands in south-east Kenya. We combined the obtained bioacoustics data with characteristics of sites, including geographic isolation, altitude, habitat type, and precipitation. Based on the results we attempt to address the following research questions:

  1. (i)

    Do call characteristics differ across the distribution range of T. hindei?

  2. (ii)

    Do geographic isolation and/or altitude cause call differentiation?

  3. (iii)

    Do contact calls and alarm calls vary in their degrees of differentiation?

  4. (iv)

    Do calls differ among neighbouring families?

Results

Principal component analysis (PCA) grouped the contact (Fig. 1a) and alarm (Fig. 1b) calls into distinct clusters (both one-way PERMANOVA P < 0.001). For both, contact and alarm calls, the first PC axis had the highest loading for gap length (loading > 0.9). For contact calls the second axis showed the strongest correlation with differences in highest and lowest frequencies (loading = 0.72); for alarm calls, call length was most important (loading = 0.89). PCA also grouped the local contact calls into three distinct non-overlapping clusters (Fig. 1a). Importantly, these three clusters did not reflect the spatial distribution of sites as cluster I combined individuals from the geographically distant sites Mukrueni and Kitui and cluster II combined individuals from Kitui and Kirinyaga.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Variance – covariance principal component analyses of (a) contact calls and (b) alarm calls identified significant (both one-way PERMANOVA: P < 0.001) differences of call characteristics among local populations across the species’ entire distribution range (colours are as given in Table 3). Ovals in a) denote the three well defined contact call clusters (I to III). PCA1 in a) and b) was highly correlated with gap length (loading > 0.9). PCA2 in a) correlated with the difference in highest and lowest frequency (loading = 0.72), in b) with call length (loading = 0.89); PCA = here Principal Component Axis

However, the family clusters exhibited a highly significant spatial component (one-way PERMANOVA: pseudo-F = 61.2, P < 0.001). A Mantel test suggested significant isolation-by-distance for both, contact and alarm calls; geographically close family groups share comparatively similar call characters (contact calls: r = 0.13, P < 0.001; alarm calls: r = 0.16. P < 0.001) compared to more distant families. The contact call clusters were neither related to altitude, nor to average precipitation (r2 < 0.05, ANOVA P (F1,12) > 0.5), but exhibited a moderate, although not significant, latitudinal gradient (contact call: r2 = 0.15, ANOVA P(F1,9) = 0.21). However, we found a significant difference in call characteristics for contact calls in relation to altitude of the population recorded (highland versus lowland, Table 1); different habitat types (swamp, riparian forest, thicket) (Table 1) led to differences in both, contact and alarm calls. Differences of contact calls among individuals, among family groups and among population clusters are stronger than differences found for alarm calls at all levels. However, we have to note that the geographical distributions of mountain regions (one-way PERMANOVA: pseudo-F = 506, P < 0.01) and habitat types (one-way PERMANOVA: pseudo-F = 191, P < 0.001) are not random and reflect a longitudinal gradient towards moist lowlands.

Table 1 Two-way PERMANOVA detected significant differences in contact and alarm calls between birds at different altitudes (low-, highlands) and living in habitats of different structure (swamp, riparian, thicket)

PCA (Fig. 2) and two-way PERMANOVA (Table 2) revealed significant differences (all P < 0.001) of contact calls (Fig. 2a, c) and alarm calls (Fig. 2b, d) among individuals of family groups along the three rivers (Nzeeu river, Kalundu river, Ithiani river) (Fig. 2a, b) and also among local family groups (Fig. 2c, d). Two-way PERMANOVA also pointed to highly significant differences in the calling patterns among families along one river (Table 2) and among the three rivers (Table 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Variance – covariance principal component analyses of contact (a, c) and alarm (b, d) calls identified significant (both one-way PERMANOVA: P < 0.001) differences of call characteristics from all individuals (irrespective of family membership) recorded along Nzeeu River (blue points), Kalundu River (red) and Ithani River (black) (a, b), and among single bird families recorded along these rivers (c, d: family colours are given in Table 3); PCA = here Principal Component Axis

Table 2 Two-way PERMANOVA detected significant differences in contact and alarm calls between birds at different rivers and of different family membership

K-means cluster analysis (Additional file 1) resulted in a close accordance of family and cluster membership and confirmed the family specific patterns in contact calls (Additional file 1: Figure S1) and alarm calls (Additional file 1: Figure S2), but also highlighted the variability and overlap among call patterns from local families (Fig. 3, Additional file 1: Figure S1, S2). The intra-family call variability increased with altitude, for both, contact and alarm calls (Fig. 3). Respective Mantel tests based on Euclidean distances in calling pattern and location revealed a significant positive correlation between call dissimilarity and geographical distance (contact calls: r = 0.17, permutation P < 0.001; alarm calls: r = 0.07, permutation P < 0.001). In turn, altitude and average precipitation (both r < 0.02, P > 0.05) did not significantly correlate with call dissimilarity.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The coefficients of variation (CV) of the first principle component (Fig. 1) increased for contact calls (circles) and alarm calls (triangles) with increasing altitude (in m) of study sites (colours are as given in Table 3). Black exponential regression line (contact calls: permutation P = 0.01). Red exponential regression line (alarm calls: permutation P = 0.13)

Discussion

We found a significant differentiation of call characteristics of T. hindei across the species’ distribution range. Previous studies showed that long-term isolation and subsequent neutral drift effects may drive the evolution of call characteristics of bird calls [13]. Call divergence found for T. hindei represents an isolation-by-distance pattern, suggesting an important role of geographic isolation for the evolution of bird call parameters. The evolution of call dialects in geographically isolated bird populations was intensively studied, mainly on bird species living on oceanic islands [14], and on isolated mountain tops [15]. For example, the East African Mountain White-eye Zosterops poliogaster occurs exclusively at higher elevations and thus forms mountain specific population groups. This caused long-term isolation with subsequent distinct evolutionary trajectories of bird calls [15]. In contrast, its lowland sibling, the Abyssinian White-eye Zosterops abyssinicus is homogenously distributed across interconnected savannahs and thus shows no significant call differentiation [16]. In addition, at localities where both, highland and lowland taxa occur sympatrically, bird call differentiation between both species is strongest. Such character displacement is interpreted as a pre-mating strategy to avoid hybridisation of closely related species when they become sympatric [17]. A study on call evolution of the Cuckoo-dove Turacoena manadensis showed significant call divergence across its entire distribution range due to population disjunction in the wake of geological changes accompanied by sea level fluctuations during Pleistocene glacial cycles [18].

In addition to drivers related to geographic isolation, ecological conditions may have strong impact on the evolution of distinct traits [19]. For T. hindei we found a significant relationship of call divergence and altitudes. Similar responses on environmental changes in relation to elevation were found in previous studies. For example, song divergence along an altitudinal gradient was detected for the Grey-breasted wood wren Henicorhina leucophrys [20]. However, elevation is itself correlated with other habitat parameters, such as humidity and temperature. But, our data do not underline further significant relationships between environmental conditions and call characteristics, or diverging habitat structures. This becomes contrasted by other studies, showing that bird calls differ depending on habitat structures [21]. This is likely at least in part the result of variation in sound propagation in different habitat types [22].

We even detected bird call differentiation between neighbouring bird families of T. hindei; differentiation of calls was more pronounced in contact calls than in alarm calls. Bird calls are important to maintain social structures [6, 23,24,25]. Further, calls are considered to represent an important pre-zygotic mating barrier (e.g. between closely related sibling species, see above) and play a pivotal role in sexual selection. The evolution of call differences within a species might also be of high relevance for species living in social structures, i.e. in family groups of cooperative breeders [26]. Local contact call differentiation may be an important mechanism to recognize the own family group and hence is a crucial cue in species with such social organisation [27]. The relevance of contact calls for the social organisation becomes further supported by our finding of a stronger call differentiation observed in contact calls compared to alarm calls. Alarm calls are considered an altruistic signal which may be under kin selection [28,29,30].

Conclusion

Our data suggests that contact calls originally evolved to keep members of a family together [29, 31] and thus often show regional dialects and are assumed to have a strong cultural component [32, 33]. In contrast, alarm calls are more conserved within the species. Thus, our study evidences that both call types are under different selective regimes resulting in different levels of call differentiation.

Methods

Study species

Hinde’s Babbler, Turdoides hindei, is a bird species endemic to Kenya and is exclusively found in the south-east of the country [11]. The species occurs in the highlands and at lower elevations along rivers and forms disjunct population groups. It is found at the cool and moist Aberdares and the foothills of Mt. Kenya, but also in the dry lowlands around Machakos, Kitui, and Makueni. Some populations occur at intermediate elevations around Thika, Sagana, and Oldonjo Sabuk [10]. Turdoides hindei is a cooperative breeder and forms distinct families consisting of two to 10 individuals [12]. The call repertoire includes a contact and an alarm call (JCH, own observations). Typical habitat is dense riparian thicket (including invasive exotic Lantana camara shrubs, see Teucher et al. 2015 [34]), in highland and lowland regions [35]. At higher elevations, the species can also be found at coffee plantations (JCH, own observations). The distribution of suitable habitat creates a long-term disjunction of this species. Agricultural intensification and clearance of shrubs along rivers with subsequent loss of habitats and the fragmentation of the remaining thicket has caused a severe decline of T. hindei populations within population groups, mainly across the semiarid lowland populations around Kitui [10]. Due to this negative population trend, T. hindei is classified as globally vulnerable according the IUCN Red List [36, 37].

Study region

Our study region covers the entire distribution range (south-eastern Kenya) with populations found along rivers, but also in agricultural land. Highland populations are represented by the following sites: Kirinyaga, Sagana, Meru, Meru swamp, Mukrueni, Thika, Thika plantation, Oldonyo Sabuk. These sites are characterised by comparatively cool and wet climate. Populations from the hot dryland region are Mutitu, Machakos, Makrueni, Kitui (including the following three rivers: Nzeeu, Kalundu, and Ithiani River). Further details are given in Table 3. Sites are displayed in Fig. 4.

Table 3 Overview of all populations of Turdoides hindei for which bird calls were recorded. Given is the locality name, colour code (coinciding with Figures), GPS coordinates, altitude, habitat type, average precipitation, estimated number of members of the respective family recorded, number of contact calls and alarm calls analysed
Fig. 4
figure 4

Study region in Kenya (small inlet map of Kenya, shaded in grey), and geographic locations where bird calls of Turdoides hindei were recorded, including a detailed map showing the three rivers Nzeeu, Kalundu, Ithiani, along which families were observed and bird calls were recorded. Names and colors are congruent with other figures and tables

Data collection

Bird calls were recorded in 2013 and 2014 year round (including the dry and rainy season), between 6 am and 6 pm with a NTG2 Røde (Australia) dual powered directional condenser microphone. The input level was operated manually and adjusted to 100%. A digital Zoom-H4 recorder was used to save the calls as stereo wav-files. Calls of the birds were recorded with approximately 10 m distance between the microphone and the target individual or family group. While recording we observed the members of a family group and assigned calls to individuals, as far as possible. Furthermore, we classified bird calls into two call types, according the activity of the bird observed, and the sound of the call: contact calls, being emitted from members while feeding and foraging to keep contact with members of the same family; alarm calls, being emitted when an individual got lost and isolated from the family group. Both contact and alarm calls are mostly emitted in series and regular intervals. We recorded bird calls of 20 families.

Recordings of high quality were processed with the programme AUDACITY v. 2.01 (Audacity Development Team). Calls affected by strong background noise or overlapping with other calls from the same (or other) species were excluded from further analyses. After deleting calls of bad quality, a total number of 2177 calls remained (at least 9 records of each call type per family emitted from different individuals, except of two families without contact call recording). We analysed the following call characteristics: lowest frequency, highest frequency, frequency range (distance between lowest and highest frequency), and the length of each call. We neither took into consideration the total number of bouts emitted, nor the total length of series of bouts or the duration of single gaps between bouts, as these parameters are strongly influenced by the level of excitement of individuals (JCH, observations), and thus the temporary state of individuals of a family may overlay general call characteristics. Spectral analyses were performed blind to site (i.e. population) using the programme PRAAT v. 5.2.15 [38]. Range of frequency (Hz) and the dynamic range (dB) were adjusted depending on the background noise in the spectrogram settings. Typical sonograms of contact calls and alarm calls are presented in Fig. 5.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Typical sonogram of an alarm call (above) and a contact call (below) of Turdoides hindei, recorded south of Kitui. The following call characteristics were analysed: highest and lowest frequency (circles), and frequency range and length of single calls (arrows)

Statistics

Due to the pseudo-replicated data structure (non-independence among calls within each family and possible spatial autocorrelation of data) we used permutation tests based on one- and two-way PERMANOVA [39] and Mantel correlation (Euclidean distances) to link individual bird vocalisations (lowest and highest frequency, frequency range and call length) for both call types (contact call and alarm call) to geographic distances among populations (isolation by distance), family membership, average precipitation (data on precipitation from local weather stations), habitat type (swamp, riparian forest, thicket) and altitude. We note that family membership, and climatic and habitat variables were not spatially autocorrelated at the 5% error level. Consequently, we did not include spatial distance into the PERMANOVA and Mantel analyses.

Variance-covariance based principal components analysis (PCA) served to discriminate bird vocalisations among population clusters and local families. Among and within family variability in bird call patterns were studied using k-means clustering. K-means grouped all individuals according to their contact and alarm calls into 14 predefined clusters, the number of observed families. If families have distinct calling structures a close accordance between cluster and family membership should result. Calculations were done with Primer 7 [40] and Statistica 12 (Statsoft, Tolsa, OK, USA).

References

  1. Sutton JT, Nakagawa S, Robertson BC, Jamieson IG. Disentangling the roles of natural selection and genetic drift in shaping variation at MHC immunity genes. Evolution. 2011;20:4408–20.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Orr MR, Smith TB. Ecology and speciation. Trends Ecol Evol. 1998;13:502–6.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Ryan MJ, Rand AS. Species recognition and sexual selection as aunitary problem in animal communication. Evolution. 1993;47:647–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Podos J, Huber SK, Taft B. Bird song: the interface of evolution and mechanism. Ann Rev Ecol Evol Sys. 2004;35:55–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Patten MA, Rotenberry JT, Zuk M, Shaw K. Habitat selection, acoustic adaptation and the evolution of reproductive isolation. Evolution. 2004;58:2144–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Kondo N, Watanabe S. Contact calls: information and social function. Japan Psych Res. 2009;51:197–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Baker MC. Differentiation of mating vocalizations in birds: acoustic features in mainland and island populations and evidence of habitat-dependent selection on songs. Ethology. 2006;112:757–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Griesser M. Referential calls signal predator behaviour in a group-living bird species. Curr Biol. 2008;18:69–73.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Suzuki TN. Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls. Anim Behav. 2014;87:59–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Shaw P, Njoroge P, Otieno V, Mlamba E. Detecting change in the status and habitat of Hinde's babbler Turdoides hindei: 2000 to 2011. Ibis. 2013;155:428–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Zimmermann DA, Turner DA, Pearson DJ, Willis I, Pratt HD: The birds of Kenya and northern Tanzania. Russel Friedman books CC, halfway house, South. Africa, 1996.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Habel JC, Teucher M, Pschonny S, Rost S, Fischer C. Beyond prime areas of nature protection in East Africa: conservation ecology of a narrowly distributed Kenyan endemic bird species. Biodivers Conserv. 2015;24:3071–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Kroodsma DE, Miller EH (eds): Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds. 1996, Comstock publishing, Sacramento, Califormia, USA, 1996.

  14. Baker MC, Baker MSA, Tilgham LM. Differing effects of isolation on evolution of bird songs: examples from an island-mainland comparison of three species. Biol J Linn Soc. 2006;89:331–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Husemann M, Ulrich W, Habel JC. The evolution of contact calls in isolated and overlapping populations of two white-eye congeners in East Africa (Aves, Zosterops). BMC Evol Biol. 2014;14:115.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Habel JC, Husemann M, Ulrich W. Patterns of contact call differentiation in the panmictic east African Abyssinian white-eye Zosterops abyssinicus (Aves: Passeriformes). Ecol Evol. 2015;5:5974–82.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Marshall DC, Cooley JR. Reproductive character displacement and speciation in periodical cicadas, with description of a new species, 13-year Magicicada neotredecim. Evolution. 2009;54:1313–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Ng NSR, Rheidt FE. Species delimitation in the white-faced cuckoo-dove (Turacoena manadensis) based on bioacoustic data. Avian Res. 2016;7:2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Slabberkoorn H, Smith T. Habitat-dependent song divergence in the little Greenbul: an analysis of environmental selection pressures on acoustic signals. Evolution. 2002;56:1849–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Caro LM, Caycedo-Rosales PC, Bowie RCK, Salbbekoorn H, Cadena CD. Ecological speciation along an elevational gradient in a tropical passerine bird. J Evol Biol. 2013;26:357–74.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Handford P. Trill rate dialects in the rufous-collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, in northwestern Argentina. Can J Zool. 1988;66:2658–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Cosens SE, Falls JB. A comparison of sound propagation and song frequency in temperate marsh and grassland habitats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1984;15:161–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Robertson BC. Vocal mate recognition in a monogamous, flock-forming bird, the silvereye Zosterops lateralis. Anim Behav. 1996;51:303–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Irwin DE. Song variation in an avian ring species. Evolution. 2000;54:998–1010.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Gerhardt HC. Evolution of acoustic communication: a multi-level analysis of signal variation. Bioacoustics. 2012;21:9–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Hatchwell BJ, Komdeur J. Ecological constraints, life history traits and the evolution of cooperative breeding. Anim Behav. 2000;59:1079–86.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Baker MC, Cunningham MA. The biology of bird-song dialects. Behav Brain Sci. 1985;8:85–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Smith JM. The evolution of alarm calls. Am Nat. 1965;99:59–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Trivers RL. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol. 1971;46:35–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Charnov EL, Krebs JR. The evolution of alarm calls: altruism or manipulation. Am Nat. 1975;109:107–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Goodale E, Kotagama SW. Alarm calling in Sri Lankan mixed-species bird flocks. Auk. 2005;122:108–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Wright TF. Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot. Proc Roy Soc B. 1996;263:867–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. MacDougall-Shackleton EA, MacDougall-Shackleton SA. Cultural and genetic evolution in mountain white-crowned sparrows: song dialects are associated with population structure. Evolution. 2001;55:2568–75.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Teucher M, Fischer C, Busch C, Horn M, Igl J, Kerner J, Müller A, Mulwa RK, Habel JC. A Kenyan endemic bird at home in invasive thickets. J Basic Appl Ecol. 2015;16:180–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Njoroge P, Bennun LA, Lens L. Habitat use by the globally endangered Hinde's babbler Turdoides hindei and its sympatric relative, the northern pied Babbler T. hypoleucus. Bird Conserv Int. 1998;8:59–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Njoroge P, Bennun LA. Status and conservation of Hinde's babbler Turdoides hindei, a threatened species in an agricultural landscape. Ostrich. 2000;71:69–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Bird Life International: Turdoides hindei, in: IUCN 2013. IUCN Red List of threatened species, version 2013.2. www.iucnredlist.org (accessed 30 Apr 2014).

  38. Boersma PPG. Praat - a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot Int. 2002;5:341–45.

  39. Anderson MJ. A new method for non-parametric multivariate analysisof variance. Austral Ecol. 2001;26:32–46.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Clarke KR, Gorley RN. PRIMER v7: User Manual/Tutorial. PRIMER-E, Plymouth. 2015.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Lydia Schübel (Freising, Germany), Onesmus Kioko and Ronald K. Mulwa (Nairobi, Kenya) for field assistance, and four anonymous referees for fruitful and critical comments.

Funding

Financial support was granted by the German Academic Exchange Service. WU was supported by an institutional university grant of UMK. The funding body had no role in the design of the study, and collection, analysis and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript.

Availability of data and materials

All bird calls recorded and analyzed in this study are available as wav-files, accessible under www.figshare.com, under the following link: https://figshare.com/s/ab27b9a4c1aca6897825.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JCH recorded bird calls and analysed the sonograms, WU performed statistical analyses, JCH, WU and MH interpreted the data and have written the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jan Christian Habel.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable

Consent for publication

Not applicable

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Numbers of classification within each family of a k-means clustering of contact calls. Colors coincide with the colors given in the main text. Raw data (wav-files) of bird calls recorded and analyzed are available at figshare.com, https://figshare.com/s/ab27b9a4c1aca6897825. (DOCX 144 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Habel, J.C., Husemann, M. & Ulrich, W. Evolution of contact and alarm calls in the Kenyan endemic Hinde’s babbler (Aves: Passeriformes). BMC Evol Biol 18, 112 (2018). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-018-1222-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-018-1222-1

Keywords