Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Care-giver identity impacts offspring development and performance in an annually social bumble bee

Abstract

Background

The developmental fates of offspring have the potential to be influenced by the identity of their care-givers and by the nature of the care that they receive. In animals that exhibit both parental and alloparental care, such as the annually eusocial insects, the influence of care-giver identity can be directly assessed to yield mechanistic and evolutionary insights into the origins and elaboration of brood care. Here, we performed a comparative investigation of maternal and worker brood care in bumble bees, a pollinator group where mothers (queens) rear the first offspring in the nest, and then daughters (workers) assume this role upon their emergence. Specifically, we compared the effects of queen and worker brood care on offspring development and also offspring performance, for a set of traits related to sensory biology, learning, and stress resistance.

Results

We found that queen-reared workers were smaller-bodied than worker-reared offspring, suggesting that bumble bee queens influence body size determination in their offspring. We also found that queen-reared workers were more resistant to starvation, which might be beneficial for early nesting success. These maternal influences could not be explained by feeding rate, given that we detected a similar offspring feeding frequency in both queens and workers.

Conclusion

Bumble bee queens have a unique influence on the development of the first offspring in the nest, which they rear, relative to worker-reared workers. We propose that bumble bee brood care has been shaped by a suite of evolutionary and ecological factors, which might include a maternal influence on traits that promote survival of incipient colonies.

Background

Extended parental care is widespread in the Animal Kingdom, but is relatively rare in insects, where it has evolved in some groups such as burying beetles, earwigs, and several lineages in the order Hymenoptera [1,2,3]. The eusocial insects express an additional, alloparental form of care, in which some daughters remain in nests as workers and care for their young, developing siblings. In these systems, workers are largely sterile caregivers [4]. Theoretical explanations have been developed for the evolution of brood care. Parental investment is predicted to evolve when it increases parental fitness, even if future reproductive costs are incurred [5,6,7,8], whereas the evolution of sibling care can be explained by the benefits it confers to individuals or social groups, which can be explained by inclusive fitness or multi-level selection theory [9-11; reviewed by 12]. However, despite these theoretical advances, improving our understanding of the origins, mechanistic basis, and consequences of brood care remains a key goal in social insect research [13].

A small subset of eusocial insect species exhibit both maternal and sibling care, albeit at different life history stages. This is seen in the eusocial species where nests are founded by a solitary queen, which includes some primitively eusocial bees, including bumble bees (genus Bombus, family Apidae), and others hymenopterans, such as paper wasps (genus Polistes, family Vespidae) and the solitary founding ants. In these systems, queens care for their brood during initial stages of colony development, then cease providing care around the time that workers eclose in the nest and begin to care for their siblings [14,15,16,17]. Solitary founding queens face unique challenges at the nest-founding stage, as they physiologically prepare for the onset of egg-laying, tend to the nest, and then care for offspring as they develop. At this stage, the success of the nest rests entirely on a single individual, the queen. In species that found nests in spring, such as bumble bees, climate change-driven mismatches with food resource availability can be more detrimental at this stage relative to later in the nesting season [18, 19]. Nests are also subject to other stressors, such as pesticides, and many solitarily-founded nests likely fail at the initiation stage, which precludes the production of new reproductives (new queens and males) [20, 21].

Many forms of brood care include progressive provisioning [22], which refers to the continuous feeding of larvae by adults, usually through close contact of the mouthparts and regurgitation of food. This dynamic social feeding interaction is a nexus of cooperation and conflict between provisioning adults and brood. Developing larvae can produce signals that elicit regurgitation by adults [23, 24]; produce priming pheromones that stimulate foraging activity [25, 26]; and communicate information to provisioning adults about nutritional status and needs [23, 27, 28]. These larval hunger signals can be honest signals that reflect nutritional needs or they can be manipulative and uncoupled from nutritional state [24]. Progressive provisioning also creates an opportunity for the manipulation of offspring by care-givers. Food amount and composition are key determiners of adult body size and direct fitness in insects [29,30,31]. In social insects, larval diet can also influence female caste determination [32, 33]. Thus, the transition to producing new queens in the nest is partially under the control of those who feed brood, and its timing subject to evolutionary pressures related to queen-worker conflict [34,35,36]. However, in the young nests of solitary-founding queens, where workers have not yet emerged, larval feeding might be more directly limited by the amount of feeding that individual queens can provide.

Here, we report results of an experimental study on the development of the first brood in the bumble bee Bombus impatiens. We performed an experiment to determine how queen and worker brood care uniquely impact the developmental fates of offspring, by manipulating young nests such that the first cohort of female brood was either reared solely by a queen or by a small cohort of workers. We then examined how care-giver identity impacted offspring developmental duration and body size. We predicted that queen-reared workers would be fed less frequently and be smaller-bodied, based on the hypothesis that bumble bee queens are limited in the amount of brood care that they can provide, relative to a group of workers. This prediction was also based on a previous study in Bombus terrestris that found that female brood reared by the queen develop for a shorter duration and may be smaller than worker-reared offspring [14]. We also explored differences in the quality or performance of adult offspring reared by either a single queen or a set of workers, for a set of traits related to sensory biology, learning, and stress resistance. The three traits that we examined are each intricately linked to colony development and survival. Sucrose sensitivity and learning can be associated with foraging specialization [37, 38] and efficiency [39], and thus have implications for colony success, whereas resistance to starvation might allow colonies to survive during periods of nutritional stress [40]. Here, our predications were based on the hypothesis that rearing by the queen may have been influenced by selective pressures related to the challenges of solitary nest founding. Specifically, we predicted that we would observe characteristics that are beneficial for early nest establishment in queen-reared workers, such as improved learning abilities and greater resistance to starvation. With respect to sucrose responsiveness, we predicted that queen-reared workers would possess lower response thresholds. This prediction was based on a previous study that found that in honey bees, workers with lower sucrose response thresholds prioritize pollen collection over nectar [41]. If this is also true in bumble bees, such a bias might be advantageous for increasing early colony growth rates [42] and improving the likelihood of nesting success.

Results

Influence of care-giver identity on offspring developmental duration and body size

We generated sets of nests that contained brood reared either solely by the queen (hereafter, queen-reared or “QR” nests; n = 13) or by workers (worker-reared or “WR” nests; n = 9). Next, we compared offspring developmental durations (time from egg laying until adult eclosion) and body sizes (estimated from the lengths of the second marginal cells of the wings) for these nests (n = 104 bees total from 22 nests). The best-fit model explaining body size included care-giver identity as a fixed effect [GLMM: significant group term (QR and WR), comparison of the model and null model: LRT: χ2 = 8.629, d.f. = 1, p = 0.003]; Table 1. Offspring reared solely by a queen were significantly smaller than those reared by a group of five workers (mean ± s.e.m. marginal cell length of 2.25 ± 0.03 mm versus 2.59 ± 0.02 mm, respectively; Tukey’s post doc: QR versus WR treatment groups: p < 0.001); Table 1; Fig. 1a. Care-giver identity did not impact developmental durations (Table 1; Fig. 1b). Body sizes and developmental durations were negatively correlated (Spearman: rho = -0.328, p < 0.001; Fig. 1c). One gyne pupal cell (determined based on its size) was observed in a worker-reared nest. This gyne was not included in any statistical analyses because the developmental patterns of queen and worker larvae are not comparable with respect to developmental duration or adult body size [43, 44].

Table 1. Predictors of offspring body size and developmental duration
Fig. 1.
figure 1

Effects of care-giver identity on body size, developmental duration, and feeding events. Young nests were manipulated such that the first cohort of female brood was either reared solely by a queen (QR nests) or by a small cohort of five workers (WR nests). a Body size as a function of care-giver identity; x-axis, length of the second marginal cell of the wing as a proxy for body size; y-axis, total number of bees; dashed lines represent the mean body sizes for each group (QR nests: mean ± s.e.m. 2.25 ± 0.03 mm; WR nests: 2.59 ± 0.02 mm). Care-giver identity impacted body size (Tukey’s post doc: QR versus WR: p < 0.001). b Developmental duration as a function of care-giver identity; x-axis, egg to adult development duration; y-axis, total number of bees; dashed lines represent the mean development durations for each group (QR nests: mean 27.75 ± 0.12 s.e.m. days; WR nests: mean 24.67 ± 0.06 s.e.m. days). Care-giver identity did not impact developmental durations. c Relationship between body size and developmental duration; dashed lines represent regression lines for each group and the solid line represents the regression line for all samples combined. There was a significant negative correlation between bee body size and developmental duration (rho = − 0.328, p < 0.001). d The total number of brood-feeding events exhibited, as a function of nest type; x-axis, day after larvae were first present in the nest (e.g., larval ages of 3 and 5 days); y-axis, number of brood-feeding events observed per day (mean ± s.e.m), for a total of 60 minutes of observation per day. Values are not corrected by the number of larvae in the nest, but these are assumed to be ~5 in all nests (based on Ref. 81 and S. H. W., unpublished data). Comparison performed by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) groups, care-giver, (N.S.).

Differences in queen and worker brood-feeding behavior

We compared the number of brood-feeding events exhibited in a subset of queen-reared versus worker-reared nests (n = 5 nests per group). There was no difference in the number of feeding events performed in QR nests (mean ± 1 s.e.m. events = 3.4 ± 0.28 and 4.4 ± 0.24 for days 3 and 5, respectively) and WR nests (3.4 ± 0.37 and 4.2 ± 0.28 for days 3 and 5, respectively) during the 120 min of observation per nest (ANOVA: F = 0, p = 1 and F = 0.067, p = 0.803 for days 3 and 5, respectively; Fig. 1d).

Influence of care-giver identity on offspring sucrose responsiveness

We then subjected a randomly selected subset of bees (adult age three days) from the QR (n = 25) and WR (n = 26) nests (from 11 and 8 nests, respectively) to a sucrose responsiveness assay. Care-giver identity did not impact whether or not bees responded to sucrose, at any concentration, or sucrose response threshold (Table 2; see Additional file 1: Fig. S3). The best-fit model for sucrose responsiveness included developmental duration as a predictor variable, but this predictor did not significantly impact sucrose responsiveness when the model was employed (GLMM: no significant development duration term; comparison of the model with factor versus null model: LRT: χ2 = 3.831, d.f. = 1, p = 0.051); Table 2.

Table 2. Predictors of offspring performance, including sucrose responsiveness, sucrose concentration, color training, and starvation resistance

Influence of care-giver identity during development on adult color training and associative learning

We also subjected a subset of offspring from QR (n = 27) and WR (n = 26) nests (from 11 and 8 nests, respectively) to an assessment of color learning. This set of offspring was different from the set of offspring used for the sucrose responsiveness assays. Care-giver identity did not affect either color training or learning (Table 2; see Additional file 1: Fig. S4). The best-fit model for the response of successful color training included body size as a predictor variable, but body size did not significantly impact color training when the model was employed (GLMM: no significant body size term; comparison of the model with a null model: LRT: χ2 = 3.261, d.f. = 1, p = 0.071); Table 2.

Influence of care-giver identity on offspring starvation resistance

Following the sucrose responsiveness and color learning assays, workers subjected to either of these tests (n = 104) were examined for their ability to withstand starvation. Here, in our GLMM, body size and care-giver identity were both predictors of starvation resistance in the best-fit model, but only care-giver identity significantly impacted this variable [GLMM: no significant body size and significant group (QR and WR) terms; comparison of the model with and null model: LRT: χ2 = 6.179, d.f. = 2, p = 0.045]; Table 2. Specifically, offspring reared by queens survived the longest durations under starvation conditions (mean hours ± s.e.m. of 28.66 ± 0.21 for QR and 25.61 ± 0.14 hours for WR; Tukey’s post doc: QR versus WR: p = 0.009; Fig. 2a). The maximum amount of time a bee survived during the assay was 72 hours (n = 2); these two outliers (from one QR and one WR nest) and another one outlier based on interquartile range (from a WR nest) were removed from the analyses. Time until death and body size were not correlated (Spearman: rho = -0.056, p = 0.580; Fig. 2b).

Fig. 2.
figure 2

Effects of care-giver identity and body size on starvation resistance. Offspring reared by a queen (QR nests) or a small group of workers (WR nests) were subjected to assay where food was removed and time until death (starvation) was recorded. a Starvation resistance as a function of care-giver identity; x-axis, time until starvation; y-axis, total number of bees. Dashed lines represent the mean survival hours for each group, care-giver (QR nests: mean ± s.e.m. 28.66 ± 0.21 h; WR nests: 25.60 ± 0.14 h). Care-giver identity impacted starvation resistance (Tukey’s post doc: QR versus WR: p = 0.009). b Relationship between starvation resistance and body size (rho = − 0.056, p = 0.580). Dashed lines represent regression lines for each group and the solid line represents the regression line for all samples combined.

Discussion

The evolution of extended parental care and cooperative alloparental brood care are foundational for understanding the evolution of sociality [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. In the subset of eusocial insect lineages that express both of these forms of care, they can be directly compared to explore their unique mechanistic bases [15,16,17]. Here, we show that in the bumble bee B. impatiens, queen and worker brood care uniquely impact the developmental trajectory and traits of offspring. Queen-reared workers in our study were smaller-bodied and also able to survive for a longer period under starvation conditions. These findings could not be explained by limitation in the frequency of brood feeding by queens, given that we detected no differences in the feeding rates of queen and worker care-givers.

We propose that our finding that queen-reared workers are more resistant to starvation might be related to the action of selection favoring worker traits that are beneficial during the early nesting period, a time when nests might be sensitive to food shortages [42] and when many wild bumble bee nests likely fail [20, 21, 45]. Annually social (or recently-formed) societies might experience intense selection for the first brood to be particularly capable of carrying out tasks related to nest development and survival. This is because the success of nests rests more heavily on these early emerging individuals, versus later in colony development when the social group is larger and there are more individuals present to carry out tasks [46]. For social species that initiate nests in early spring (including bumble bees), the first offspring may also need to withstand periods of nutritional dearth that are longer or more frequent. Floral food resources can be less consistent early in the season in some environments [18, 19], and fluctuating weather regimes may make it impossible to forage at times [47]. Further, food stores are less abundant at this stage in bumble bee colony development [47], and even mature colonies typically contain only enough stores to survive up to a few days without replenishment [47, 48]. In previous studies of B. impatiens, Couvillon and Dornhaus [40] found that smaller-bodied workers are also hardier against starvation in mature colonies, in part because they have higher lipid levels than larger-bodied workers [49; but see 50]. Our finding that queen-reared workers are more resistant to starvation, irrespective of body size, suggests that a complex suite of factors control this trait in workers, and that these factors potentially change across the course of bumble bee colony development [40, 49, 50].

Our finding that queens rear smaller-bodied offspring than workers is consistent with a previous study in the bumble bee B. terrestris, which found that rearing by the queen results in a shorter developmental duration in workers, and can cause a decrease in body size, during the establishment period of newly-formed nests [14]. The maternal manipulation hypothesis for the evolution of sociality posits that maternal control of direct fitness outcomes of offspring (in part through feeding and diet) played a role in the origins of social life, such that offspring were manipulated to remain within their natal nests as helpers, rather than leave and initiate their own nests [51, 52]. One of the key predictions of this hypothesis is that mothers should rear female daughters that are smaller or otherwise less able to successfully carry out nesting on their own [51]. Evidence for maternal manipulation has been found in a number of insect lineages, including weakly or flexibly social bees [53,54,55,56], and in many vertebrate species [e.g., 57,58]. In eusocial insects, maternal manipulation theory has been extended to include caste bias, such that in the systems with both maternal and alloparental care, queens are predicted to rear offspring that are more likely to develop into sterile workers versus reproductive queens [59]. This extension of maternal manipulation theory was developed because eusocial insect workers do not require manipulation to remain as helpers in the nest, but development into a queen might allow an individual to leave and initiate a new nest. This theoretical prediction has been met in paper wasps [59], and was not explicitly met in our study, because we saw smaller-sized workers in our queen-reared, but did not see more gynes.

As an alternative to the maternal manipulation hypothesis, the smaller body sizes we observed in queen-reared workers might be more related to colony efficiency, or some other beneficial outcome of having smaller-sized workers in young nests. In bumble bees, there is considerable variation in worker body sizes within mature nests (reviewed in ref. 33), which is considered adaptive in that it contributes to social organization and overall colony success [60]. Larger workers forage at an earlier age and are more frequent [61, 62] and efficient [63,64,65] foragers. They have a greater density of olfactory sensilla and sensitivity to odorants [66], which may impact learning, and are more positively phototactic [67], which is consistent with their increased propensity to forage. However, smaller workers are more likely to be found in the nest, are more resistant to starvation in previous studies [40], and are more likely to incubate brood [68]. Collectively, the maintenance of this worker variation in colonies is thought to improve colony efficiency [69]. However, empirical studies have shown that colonies can perform equally successfully when they are comprised of skewed or more limited worker body size distributions [69] and may produce skewed size distributions under some conditions, such as when resources are plentiful [42]. Based on our current understanding of bumble bee biology, the significance of having smaller-bodied workers in young, recently-founded nests is unclear. Our finding that variation in associative learning and sucrose sensitivity could not be attributed to body size (or care-giver identity) suggests that any benefit of smaller-sized workers is not related to these specific cognitive and sensory traits in B. impatiens. However, given that size-related differences in learning abilities have been detected in other studies [70,71,72], we suggest that further studies are required in this avenue of research. Additionally, further explorations of how maternal and sibling care shape worker characteristics, and the adaptive benefits of reduced body sizes in young nests, are important for fully understanding the functional and evolutionary significance of the patterns that we have detected.

Conclusions

Here, we examined how brood care by a queen or workers impact offspring development in a bumble bee species, and found distinct differences in how these two forms of care influence offspring size and resistance to starvation. We observed that queen-reared larvae were fed no less frequently, as has also been reported for B. terrestris [14]. Given this, there may be other differences in the quality or nature of maternal and sibling care in bumble bees, such as the quantity of food delivered per feeding event or the composition of larval food [73], or in non-nutritive signals such as contact or volatile pheromones [14] or the frequency or quality of brood incubation [74]. Thus, a critical remaining question is how queens influence offspring development. Broadly, our study provides additional evidence that maternal interactions can influence early development [75, 76], including in eusocial lineages [59]. Further, despite their phenotypic similarity, maternal and sibling care appear to have been subject to unique selective pressures during the evolution of the bumble bee lineage.

Methods

Bee rearing and behavioral observations

All bees originated from mature colonies (queenright with > 50 workers) supplied by Koppert Biological Systems (Howell, MI, USA). We housed these source colonies in the Entomology Building at the University of California, Riverside, maintained at room temperature (~23°C) and uncontrolled (but > 40%) RH. Colonies were fed a syrup solution (provided with colonies by Koppert Biological Systems) and mixed-source, honey bee-collected pollen (Biobest USA, Inc.) provided ad libitum. All individual queens and nest cages were kept at the University of California, Riverside’s Insectary and Quarantine Facility in a room maintained at 27-30°C and 72-80% RH. Individual bees were fed the same pollen diet described above and an artificial nectar solution based on [77].

Nests were created to obtain brood reared solely by a queen (hereafter, queen-reared or “QR” nests; n = 13) or by a set of five workers (worker-reared or “WR” nests; n = 9). To create these two nests types, first, newly eclosed (callow) queens were removed from their natal colonies and mated with males from different source colonies, based on methods in Röseler [78], with the following differences. Callow queens (< 24 hours old; identified by their silvery appearance and inability to fly) were placed in individual plastic queen rearing cages obtained from Biobest USA, Inc. (approximately W15 x D15 x H10 cm). At ages 3-8 days, the queens were placed in mating cages (W60 x D60 x H120 cm; BugDorm-6S620) from approximately 900-1700 hours with males (2-3 per queen). After mating once [79, 80], queens were placed in new plastic rearing cages (as described above). At ages 12 and 13 days, queens received a CO2 treatment (30 minutes per day) to stimulate nest initiation [78]. All queens used in the experiment initiated nests within a mean (± 1 s.e.m.) of 16 ± 1.59 days after the CO2 treatment.

Five days after eggs were first observed in a nest (the time until eggs become larvae; ref. 44), queens were either left in their cage (for QR nests) or were removed and five callow workers were added (WR nests). This is the average number of eggs that a queen lays in her first egg cell, and thus the average number of individuals in the first brood in B. terrestris [81] and B. impatiens (unpublished data, S.H.W.). All workers introduced to a nest came from the same source colony, which differed from the queen’s natal colony. To ensure that the correct ages of eclosed adults were determined, we destroyed any additional eggs laid on the following five days after the first set of eggs were detected. This is conservative in that if it was unclear whether a larva originated from the first brood cohort or not, it was destroyed. This allowed us to have high confidence in brood ages, at the cost of likely reducing the number of brood obtained from some nests. All data reported hereafter are related only to the first brood cohort, which consisted solely of female brood; no male offspring were observed in these nests or included in our analyses. We placed infrared security cameras (VIGICA Peashooter QD520) above both the QR and WR nests for the duration of the experiment, which was done to generate video data for estimating feeding frequency and the time of eclosion for all offspring (see below). Nests were inspected daily and the dates of adult emergence were recorded. No queens died during the experiment. Any adult workers (i.e., care-givers) that died (n = 4) were replaced with callow workers from the same source colony.

Developmental duration and body size

To quantify the adult (final) body size of offspring, we measured the length of the second marginal cell of the wing for all offspring from the first brood cohorts in our QR and WR nests (n = 104 bees). This metric is highly correlated with body size in bumble bees [14, 61, 82, 83]. To quantify developmental duration for all offspring (n = 104 bees), we calculated the number of days between when eggs were laid until adult eclosion. This duration was determined based on video data obtained from infrared security cameras.

Estimation of feeding frequency

To estimate larval feeding frequency in our QR and WR nests, we selected a subset of five nests from each of the two treatment groups for observational analysis. Nests were selected for this analysis if they contained brood with the shortest or longest developmental durations, or they were randomly selected from the middle of this distribution. This was done to maximize our ability to detect differences in feeding rates between the two nest types. Brood-feeding is a discrete behavior that involves piercing the larval wax envelope, placing mouthparts through the opening, and regurgitating with an abdominal contraction [81] (see video in Dryad). For each nest, we observed five minutes per hour within a 12-h period on day 3 and again on day 5 after the first larvae were present in the nest, for a total of 120 min of observation per nest. This amount of observation was based on previous studies that examined brood-feeding behavior in bumble bees [14, 81]. A single observer viewed videos to avoid introducing observer bias. In our results, we show the rate of feeding events performed by a single queen or by a group of workers, not corrected by the number of larvae in the nest. Here we assume that there were equal numbers of larvae (~5; ref. 44) in all nests irrespective of nest type, given that the number of eggs in the first cohort was fixed before nests were assigned to a specific treatment group.

Sucrose responsiveness assay

On the third day after adult emergence, bees were removed from the nest and placed individually in a 14 ml centrifuge tube, modified to allow the worker to receive taste stimulation (modeled after [84]). Bees were starved of pollen and artificial nectar in these tubes for 3-5 hours prior to the assays. Then, we dipped a wooden toothpick into sucrose solutions at one of the following concentrations (w/v) and touched the antennae to attempt to elicit a proboscis extension reflex (PER): 20%, 25%, 30%, 35%, 40%, 45%, 50%. We administered concentrations in the order listed, with deionized water used as a negative control in between each concentration. No workers responded to this negative control. We used higher sucrose concentrations than are used for honey bee PER [85] (as have been used before in bumble bees, refs. [86, 87]) because in preliminary tests, we found that 20% sucrose was the lowest concentration that elicited a response. As a response variable (sucrose responsiveness), we recorded the minimum concentration at which a worker extended her proboscis for at least three seconds. Bees that respond to lower sucrose concentrations have a lower sucrose response threshold and are more sensitive to sucrose. We also considered whether or not a bee responded at all to sucrose (at any concentration) during this assay, as some bees (n = 22) did not. During the assay, bees were kept under controlled conditions in the insectary room described above. No bees died during this assay.

Color learning assay

To assess learning ability, we removed individual bees from their nests on the third day after adult emergence and subjected them to an assay based on [88]. During this period, individual bees were maintained in the insectary room described above. Workers were starved of pollen and artificial nectar for 3-5 hours prior to the assay in a 5 ml tube with holes for ventilation and two holes at the end to insert test strips. Our learning assay consisted of two phases: (1) color training and (2) color learning. During the training phase, bees were either given a yellow or blue strip of paper coated with 50% (w/v) sucrose solution to train them to associate the color with sucrose. Workers were then given a maximum of five minutes to respond to the stimulation and a response was recorded when a worker extended her proboscis and made contact with the paper for at least three seconds. After a successful response, the paper strip was removed from the tube and the bee was undisturbed for five minutes. This was repeated for a total of five times, alternating the hole that the paper strip was presented in to avoid spatial learning, and with a five-minute rest period between each stimulation. Here, our response variable was whether the bee successfully extended her proboscis to the sucrose coated strip each time (positive) or not (negative). Bees that did not respond successfully five times were marked as not being trained. We consider this training component of the color learning assay to be related to the propensity to respond repeatedly to sucrose as a stimulus, and not directly related to learning ability; although, previous studies have shown an association between sucrose sensitivity and learning (reviewed in ref. 41). During the subsequent learning phase, we introduced both blue and yellow strips into the tube simultaneously (both coated in distilled water) and bees were given one minute to extend their proboscis to a test strip. We then recorded whether the bee extended her proboscis for the color paper strip that she had been trained to during the learning training phase. This second component of the learning assay is directly related to learning ability, unlike the training component. No bees died during this assay. Unique subsets off bees were used in the sucrose responsiveness and color learning assays.

Starvation resistance assay

Following the sucrose responsiveness and color learning assays (no more than 4-5 hours after bees were removed from nests), we placed workers subjected to either of these tests (n = 104) in individual cages without pollen diet or artificial nectar solution in the insectary rearing room (described above). Cages were inspected at 3-h intervals from 900 to 1800 each day and the date and time that any worker was observed dead in the cage was recorded.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed in R version 3.6.1 [89] and only p-values < 0.05 were considered significant. All results were visualized with the ‘ggplot2’ package [90]. We employed the interquartile range technique and a 95% confidence interval to remove outliers, which resulted in the removal of three data points in the starvation data set (see results). After identifying non-normality of the data using the Shapiro-Wilk test, we used generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) to explore how factors such as care-giver identity, body size, and developmental duration contributed to responses. GLMMs were performed with the glmer function in the R package lme4 version 1.1-10 [91]. The best-fit model for our data was selected based on the Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC) using the “dredge” command within the MUMIn package [92]. Following model selection, factors of interest were analyzed by performing Likelihood Ratio Tests (LRT) comparing the models with factors to a null model without these factors. Post-hoc t-tests were conducted using Tukey’s multiple comparison of means. For responses, we considered body size, developmental duration, whether a bee responded to sucrose at all in the sucrose responsiveness assay (yes or no), sucrose responsiveness (lowest concentration a bee responded positively to), color training (whether the bee successfully extended her proboscis during training positively), color learning (bee extended her proboscis for the color paper strip that she had been trained to during the learning training phase), and survival time under starvation. Models could include as fixed effects care-giver identity, developmental duration, and/or body size, and their interactions. Queen source colony and individual nest were included as random factors in all models [92]. For starvation resistance, we included whether a bee was subjected to behavioral assays (sucrose or learning assays) as a random factor in GLMM. We used Spearman's correlation analyses (for non-parametric data) to examine correlations between body size and developmental duration, and between body size and time until starvation. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was employed to compare data on brood-feeding behavior in the nest, according to previous studies in bumble bees [14, 81].

Availability of data and materials

All analyses and pipelines can be found in the primary author’s GitHub (https://github.com/claudinpcosta/2021-BS.experiment-MaternalSiblingCare). Data are also available on Dryad at https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.6086/D1B37V [93].

Abbreviations

QR:

Queen-reared

WR:

Worker-reared

References

  1. Tallamy DW. Insect Parental Care. Bioscience. 1984;34:20–4. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.2307/1309421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Zeh DW, Smith RL. Paternal investment by terrestrial Arthropods. Integr Comp Biol. 1985;25:785–805. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/icb/25.3.785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Trumbo ST. Parental care in invertebrates. In: Rosenblatt JS, Snowdon CT, eds. Advances in the study of behavior. New York, Academic Press, 1996. p. 3–51. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60329-0

  4. Wilson EO. The insect societies. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press; 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Hamilton WD. The genetical evolution of social behaviour. J Theor Biol. 1964;7:1–16.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Clutton-Brock TH. The evolution of parental care. New York: Princeton University Press; 1991.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  7. Queller DC. Extended parental care and the origin of eusociality. Proc Royal Soc Lond Ser B. 1994;256:105–11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1098/rspb.1994.0056.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Wong JWY, Meunier J, Kölliker M. The evolution of parental care in insects: the roles of ecology, life history and the social environment. Ecol Entomol. 2013;38:123–37. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/een.12000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Wilson EO, Hölldobler B. Eusociality: origin and consequences. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102:13367–71. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1073/pnas.0505858102.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Hughes WOH, Oldroyd BP, Beekman M, Ratnieks FLW. Ancestral monogamy shows kin selection is key to the evolution of eusociality. Science. 2008;320:1213–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1126/science.1156108.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Nowak MA, Tarnita CE, Wilson EO. The evolution of eusociality. Nature. 2010;466:1057–62. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1038/nature09205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Ratnieks FL, Helanterä H. The evolution of extreme altruism and inequality in insect societies. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2009;364:3169–79. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1098/rstb.2009.0129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Linksvayer T, Wade M. The evolutionary origin and elaboration of sociality in the aculeate hymenoptera: maternal effects, sib-social effects, and heterochrony. Quart Rev Biol. 2005;80(3):317–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Shpigler H, Tamarkin M, Gruber Y, Poleg M, Siegel AJ, Bloch G. Social influences on body size and developmental time in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2013;67:1601–12. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00265-013-1571-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Toth AL, Varala K, Newman TC, Miguez FE, Hutchison SK, Willoughby DA, et al. Wasp gene expression supports an evolutionary link between maternal behavior and eusociality. Science. 2007;318:441–4. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1126/science.1146647.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. West-Eberhard M. J., in Natural History and Evolution of Paper-Wasps, S. Turillazzi, M. J. West-Eberhard, Eds. (Oxford Univ. Press, New York, 1996), pp. 290–317.

  17. Woodard SH, Bloch GM, Band MR, Robinson GE. Molecular heterochrony and the evolution of sociality in bumblebees (Bombus terrestris). Proc Royal Soc B: Biol Sci. 2015;281:20132419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Kudo G, Ida TY. Early onset of spring increases the phenological mismatch between plants and pollinators. Ecology. 2013;94:2311–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Hegland SJ, Nielsen A, Lázaro A, Bjerknes AL, Totland Ø. How does climate warming affect plant-pollinator interactions? Ecol Lett. 2009;12:184–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Leza M, Watrous KM, Bratu J, Woodard SH. Effects of neonicotinoid insecticide exposure and monofloral diet on nest-founding bumblebee queens. Proc R Soc B. 2018;285(1880):20180761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Baron GL, Jansen VA, Brown MJ, Raine NE. Pesticide reduces bumblebee colony initiation and increases probability of population extinction. Nat Ecol Evol. 2017;1(9):1308–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Michener CD. Comparative social behaviour of bees. Annu Rev Entomol. 1969;14:299–342. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1146/annurev.en.14.010169.001503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. den Boer SPA, Duchateau MJH. A larval hunger signal in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Insectes Soc. 2006;53:369–73. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00040-006-0883-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Mas F, Kölliker M. Maternal care and offspring begging in social insects: chemical signalling, hormonal regulation and evolution. Anim Behav. 2008;76:1121–31. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.06.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Pankiw T, Page RE Jr, Kim Fondrk M. Brood pheromone stimulates pollen foraging in honey bees (Apis mellifera). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1998;44:193–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s002650050531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Le Conte Y, Mohammedi A, Robinson GE. Primer effects of a brood pheromone on honeybee behavioural development. Proc Royal Soc Lond Ser B. 2001;268:163–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1098/rspb.2000.1345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Mas F, Haynes KF, Kölliker M. A chemical signal of offspring quality affects maternal care in a social insect. Proc Biol Sci. 2009;276:2847–53. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1098/rspb.2009.0498.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. He XJ, Zhang XC, Jiang WJ, Barron AB, Zhang JH, Zeng ZJ. Starving honey bee (Apis mellifera) larvae signal pheromonally to worker bees. Sci Rep. 2016;6:22359. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1038/srep22359.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Bakker K. Feeding period, growth, and pupation in larvae of Drosophila melanogaster. Entomol Exp Appl. 1959;2:171–86. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1570-7458.1959.tb00432.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Davidowitz G, D’Amico LJ, Nijhout HF. Critical weight in the development of insect body size. Evol Dev. 2003;5:188–97. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1046/j.1525-142x.2003.03026.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Boggs CL, Freeman KD. Larval food limitation in butterflies: effects on adult resource allocation and fitness. Oecologia. 2005;144:353–61. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00442-005-0076-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Chole H, Woodard SH, Bloch G. Body size variation in bees: regulation, mechanisms, and relationship to social organization. Curr Opin Insect Sci. 2019;35:77–87. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.cois.2019.07.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Corona M, Libbrecht R, Wheeler DE. Molecular mechanisms of phenotypic plasticity in social insects. Curr Opin Insect Sci. 2016;13:55–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Duchateau MJ, Velthuis HHW. Development and reproductive strategies in Bombus terrestris colonies. Behaviour. 1988;107:186–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Bloch G. Regulation of queen–worker conflict in bumble bee (Bombus terrestris) colonies. Proc R Soc London B. 1999;1999(266):2465–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Owen RE, Plowright RC. Worker-queen conflict and male parentage in bumble bees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1982;11:91–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Mujagic S, Sarkander J, Erber B, Erber J. Sucrose acceptance and different forms of associative learning of the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) in the field and laboratory. Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4:46.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Chittka L, Raine NE. Recognition of flowers by pollinators. Curr Opin Plant Biol. 2006;9:428–35. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.pbi.2006.05.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Spaethe J, Brockmann A, Halbig C, Tautz J. Size determines antennal sensitivity and behavioral threshold to odors in bumblebee workers. Naturwissenschaften. 2007a;94(9):733–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Couvillon MJ, Dornhaus A. Small worker bumble bees (Bombus impatiens) are hardier against starvation than their larger sisters. Insectes Soc. 2010;57(2):193–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00040-010-0064-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Scheiner R, Page RE, Erber J. Sucrose responsiveness and behavioral plasticity in honey bees (Apis mellifera). Apidologie. 2004;35:133–42. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1051/apido:2004001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Malfi RL, Crone E, Williams N. Demographic benefits of early season resources for bumble bee (B. vosnesenskii) colonies. Oecologia. 2019;191(2):377–88. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00442-019-04472-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Cnaani J, Borst DW, Huang ZY, Robinson GE, Hefetz A. Caste determination in Bombus terrestris: differences in development and rates of JH biosynthesis between queen and worker larvae. J Insect Physiol. 1997;1997(43):373–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Cnaani J, Schmid-Hempel R, Schmidt JO. Colony development, larval development and worker reproduction in Bombus impatiens Cresson. Insectes Sociaux. 2002;49(2):164–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Mullins JL, Strange JP, Tripodi AD. Why Are Queens Broodless? Failed nest initiation not linked to parasites, mating status, or ovary development in two bumble bee species of pyrobombus (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Bombus). J Econ Entomol. 2020;113:575–81. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/jee/toz330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Dornhaus A, Powell S, Bengston S. Group size and its effects on collective organization. Ann Rev Entomol. 2012;57:123–41.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Heinrich B. Bumblebee Economics. Harvard: Harvard University Press; 2004.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Cartar RV, Dill LM. Colony energy requirements affect the foraging currency of bumble bees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1990;27:377–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Couvillon MJ, Jandt JM, Bonds J, Helm BR, Dornhaus A. Percent lipid is associated with body size but not task in the bumble bee Bombus impatiens. J Comp Physiol A. 2011;197(11):1097.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Treanore ED, Kiner JM, Kerner ME, Amsalem E. Shift in worker physiology and gene expression pattern from reproductive to diapause-like with colony age in the bumble bee Bombus impatiens. J Exp Biol. 2020;223:9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Alexander RD. The evolution of social behavior. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1974;5:325–83. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1146/annurev.es.05.110174.001545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Michener CD, Brothers DJ. Were workers of eusocial hymenoptera initially altruistic or oppressed? Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1974;71:671–4. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1073/pnas.71.3.671.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Lawson SP, Ciaccio KN, Rehan SM. Maternal manipulation of pollen provisions affects worker production in a small carpenter bee. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2016;70:1891–900. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00265-016-2194-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Rehan SM, Richards MH. The influence of maternal quality on brood sex allocation in the small carpenter bee Ceratina calcarata. Ethology. 2010;116:876–87. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Kapheim KM, Bernal SP, Smith AR, Nonacs P, Wcislo WT. Support for maternal manipulation of developmental nutrition in a facultatively eusocial bee, Megalopta genalis (Halictidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65:1179–90. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00265-010-1131-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Brand N, Chapuisat M. Born to be bee, fed to be worker? The caste system of a primitively eusocial insect. Front Zool. 2012;9:35. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1742-9994-9-35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Briga M, Pen I, Wright J. Care for kin: within-group relatedness and allomaternal care are positively correlated and conserved throughout the mammalian phylogeny. Biol Lett. 2012;8:533–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1098/rsbl.2012.0159.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Shine R. Manipulative mothers and selective forces: the effects of reproduction on thermoregulation in reptiles. Herpetologica. 2012;68:289–98. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1655/HERPETOLOGICA-D-12-00004.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Jandt JM, Suryanarayanan S, Hermanson JC, Jeanne RL, Toth AL. Maternal and nourishment factors interact to influence offspring developmental trajectories in social wasps. Proc Biol Sci. 2017. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1098/rspb.2017.0651.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Austin MW, Dunlap AS. Intraspecific variation in worker body size makes North American bumble bees (Bombus spp.) less susceptible to decline. Am Nat. 2019;194(3):381–94. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1086/704280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Yerushalmi S, Bodenhaimer S, Bloch G. Developmentally determined attenuation in circadian rhythms links chronobiology to social organization in bees. J Exp Biol. 2006;209(Pt 6):1044–51. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1242/jeb.02125.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Russell AL, Morrison SJ, Moschonas EH, Papaj DR. Patterns of pollen and nectar foraging specialization by bumblebees over multiple timescales using RFID. Sci Rep. 2017;7:42448. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1038/srep42448.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Brian AD. Division of labour and foraging in Bombus agrorum Fabricius. J Anim Ecol. 1952;21(2):223–40. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.2307/1959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Spaethe J, Weidenmüller A. Size variation and foraging rate in bumblebees (Bombus terrestris). Insectes Soc. 2002;49(2):142–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00040-002-8293-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Hagbery J, Nieh JC. Individual lifetime pollen and nectar foraging preferences in bumble bees. Naturwissenschaften. 2012;99(10):821–32. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00114-012-0964-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Spaethe J, Brockmann A, Halbig C, Tautz J. Size determines antennal sensitivity and behavioral threshold to odors in bumblebee workers. Naturwissenschaften. 2007b;94(9):733–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00114-007-0251-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Merling M, Eisenmann S, Bloch G. Body size but not age influences phototaxis in bumble bee (Bombus terrestris, L.) workers. Apidologie. 2020. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s13592-020-00759-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Gardner KE, Foster RL, O’Donnell S. Experimental analysis of worker division of labor in bumblebee nest thermoregulation (Bombus huntii, Hymenoptera: Apidae). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2007;61(5):783–92. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s00265-006-0309-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Holland, J. G., Nakayama, S., Porfiri, M., Nov, O., & Bloch, G. (2020). Is diversity in worker body size important for the performance of bumble bee colonies?. bioRxiv.

  70. Mares S, Ash L, Gronenberg W. Brain allometry in bumblebee and honey bee workers. Brain Behav Evol. 2005;66:50–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Worden BD, Skemp AK, Papaj DR. Learning in two contexts: the effects of interference and body size in bumblebees. J Exp Biol. 2005;208:2045–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Riveros AJ, Gronenberg W. Olfactory learning and memory in the bumblebee Bombus occidentalis. Naturwissenschaften. 2009;96:851–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. Pereboom JJM. The composition of larval food and the significance of exocrine secretions in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Insectes Soc. 2000;47(1):11–20. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s000400050003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Heinrich B. Physiology of brood incubation in the bumblebee queen, Bombus vosnesenskii. Nature. 1972;239(5369):223–5. https://doi.org/10.1038/239223a0.pdf.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Wade MJ. Maternal effect genes and the evolution of sociality in haplo-diploid organisms. Evolution. 2001;55(3):453–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.0014-3820.2001.tb00779.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Wolf JB, Wade MJ. What are maternal effects (and what are they not)? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364(1520):1107–15. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1098/rstb.2008.0238.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Boyle NK, Tripodi AD, Machtley SA, Strange JP, Pitts-Singer TL, Hagler JR. A Nonlethal Method to examine non-Apis bees for mark-capture research. J Insect Sci. 2018;18:3. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/jisesa/iey043.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  78. Röseler P-F. A technique for year-round rearing of Bombus terrestris (Apidae, Bombini) colonies in captivity. Apidologie. 1985;16(2):165–70. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1051/apido:19850206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Woodard SH, Duennes MA, Watrous KM, Jha S. Diet and nutritional status during early adult life have immediate and persistent effects on queen bumble bees. Conserv Physiol. 2019;7:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Schmid-Hempel R, Schmid-Hempel P. Female mating frequencies in Bombus spp. from Central Europe. Insectes Soc. 2000;47(1):36–41. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s000400050006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Woodard SH, Bloch G, Band MR, Robinson GE. Social regulation of maternal traits in nest-founding bumble bee (Bombus terrestris) queens. J Exp Biol. 2013;216(Pt 18):3474–82. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1242/jeb.087403.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Knee WJ, Medler JT. The seasonal size increase of bumblebee workers (Hymenoptera: Bombus). Can Entomol. 1965;97(11):1149–55. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4039/Ent971149-11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Owen RE. Body size variation and optimal body size of bumble bee queens (Hymenoptera: Apidae). Can Entomol. 1988;120(1):19–27. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4039/Ent12019-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Ma C, Kessler S, Simpson A, Wright G. A novel behavioural assay to investigate gustatory responses of individual, freely-moving bumble bees (Bombus terrestris). J Vis Exp. 2016. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3791/54233.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Pankiw T, Page RE Jr. Response thresholds to sucrose predict foraging division of labor in honeybees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2000;47(4):265–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s002650050664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Smith AR, Graystock P, Hughes WOH. Specialization on pollen or nectar in bumblebee foragers is not associated with ovary size, lipid reserves or sensory tuning. PeerJ. 2016;4:e2599. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.7717/peerj.2599.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  87. Graystock P, Blane EJ, McFrederick QS, Goulson D, Hughes WOH. Do managed bees drive parasite spread and emergence in wild bees? Int J Parasitol Parasites Wildl. 2016;5(1):64–75. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.ijppaw.2015.10.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Muth F, Cooper TR, Bonilla RF, Leonard AS. A novel protocol for studying bee cognition in the wild. Methods Ecol Evol. 2018;9(1):78–87. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/2041-210X.12852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  90. Wickham H. ggplot2. WIREs Comp Stat. 2011;3(2):180–5. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/wics.147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2014:67, 1–48. Available from: http://arxiv.org/abs/1406.5823

  92. Barton K. MuMIn: Multi - Model Inference. R package version 1.15.1. 2015. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=MuMIn.

  93. Costa CP, Fisher K, Guillén BM, Yamanaka N, Bloch G, Woodard SH. Data from: Care-giver identity impacts offspring development and performance in an annually social bumble bee. Dryad Digital Repos. 2021. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.6086/D1B37V.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Felicity Muth and Laura Leger for providing feedback on the learning assay, Kristal Watrous for statistical assistance, Yadira Diaz for assistance with carrying out the experiment, Christie Miranda for assistance with analysis of brood-feeding behavior, and Leif Richardson for providing feedback on the manuscript. We also thank Koppert Biological Systems, Inc. for providing bee colonies. 

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute of Food and Agriculture (2017-06558 to SHW and NY), the US-Israel Binational Agricultural Research and Development Fund (IS-5077-18 R to SHW and GB), and the US-Israel Binational Science Foundation (2017188 to SHW, GB, and NY).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

CPC led the experimental design, carried out the experiments, and led authorship of the manuscript. KF co-designed and helped carry out the experiments and assisted with statistical analyses. BMG led the sucrose responsiveness and learning assays. GB and NY assisted with experimental design. SHW co-designed the experiment. All co-authors contributed to writing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to S. Hollis Woodard.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Additional materials and methods.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Costa, C.P., Fisher, K., Guillén, B.M. et al. Care-giver identity impacts offspring development and performance in an annually social bumble bee. BMC Ecol Evo 21, 20 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-021-01756-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12862-021-01756-2

Keywords