Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Neuropsychological and cerebral morphometric aspects of negative symptoms in schizophrenia: negative symptomatology is associated with specific mnestic deficits in schizophrenic patients

Abstract

Background

The prevalence of negative symptoms in schizophrenic patients seems to be an important indicator for treatment response and prognosis. Although negative symptoms have often been attributed to frontal lobe anomalies, neuropsychological and anatomical findings do not explicitly support this assumption. Since knowledge about the cerebral correlate of negative symptoms in schizophrenia might have a strong impact on therapeutic and psychopharmacological interventions, we aimed to answer this question by investigating the relationship between negative symptoms, neuropsychological functioning and cerebral volumes in schizophrenic patients.

Methods

Twenty schizophrenic patients and 32 healthy controls were examined using a neuropsychological test battery for the assessment of temporal (mnestic) and frontal (executive) faculties. Volumetric measurements of temporal (hippocampus and amygdala) and frontal (orbitofrontal, dorsolateral prefrontal, and anterior cingulate area) brain areas were performed. Negative symptoms were assessed using the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS).

Results

Schizophrenic patients performed worse than healthy controls in tests assessing verbal and visuospatial learning and memory functions and on the Stroop interference task. After dividing the schizophrenic group in patients with high and low SANS scores almost all of these deficits were restricted to the former group. There were no overall group differences regarding cerebral subarea volumes. Overall negative symptoms were significantly correlated with verbal memory functions but not with frontal lobe faculties.

Conclusions

Negative symptoms in schizophrenia could specifically associated with verbal memory deficits.

Peer Review reports

Background

In 1985 Crow suggested a dichotomic concept for the diagnosis of schizophrenia by dividing schizophrenic disorders into type I- and type II-schizophrenia according to the prevalence of either positive or negative symptoms [1]. Since schizophrenic patients often show symptoms of both dimensions and the psychopathology tends to change in the course of disease [2], Crow’s approach did not suffice as a diagnosis system.

Nevertheless the concept of negative symptoms in schizophrenia has attracted much attention, since negative symptoms have been identified as important indicators for treatment response and prognosis. Negative symptoms, especially anhedonia and affective flattening, can be used as predictors of long-term outcome [3],[4]. Furthermore negative symptomatology in schizophrenia was found to intensify the severity of illness and strongly influence global functioning [5]. Some authors found a specific influence of negative symptoms on vocational (work performance) and social functioning as well as functional skills [6]-[8]. Furthermore negative symptoms have been associated with social problem solving [9]. Besides, they may also predict response to certain medication. Clozapine responders were identified by a lesser degree of negative symptomatology [10].

The first rating scale for the assessment of negative symptomatology, the “Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms” (SANS), was developed by Andreasen in the early 1980s [11],[12]. The SANS became the standard instrument for rating the extent of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. It defines five symptom complexes: affective flattening, impoverishment of language and thinking (alogia), reduced drive (avolition/apathy), the inability to feel happy (anhedonia/asociality) and disturbance of attention.

Negative symptoms might be differentiated from positive symptoms by a distinct pathological process. Traditionally negative symptoms in schizophrenia have been linked to frontal lobe anomalies, mainly due to the frequently described relationship between negative symptoms and hypofrontality while in a resting state or during the processing of cognitive tasks [13]-[18].

Due to these findings many studies regarding the correlation between negative symptoms and cognitive dysfunctions in schizophrenia have hypothesized and verified an inverse relationship between the extent of negative symptoms and so-called frontal lobe deficits. Inverse correlations were most often found with paradigms assessing logical thinking and categorization, e.g. the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test as well as indicators of verbal fluency [19]-[23].

Nevertheless inverse correlations with a variety of other cognitive dysfunctions have also been found. Pronounced negative symptoms have been linked to stronger impairments in recognition memory, psychomotor speed, attention or visuospatial abilities [22],[24]-[27].

Several morphometric studies support the link between negative symptoms in schizophrenia and frontal lobe impairment. Reduced frontal lobe volumes have been found in correlation with anhedonia and the apathy level of schizophrenic patients, which are core symptoms of schizophrenic negative symptomatology [28],[29]. The extent of negative symptoms was also significantly correlated with smaller orbitofrontal, left insular, dorsolateral prefrontal regions, bilaterally in the medial frontal, anterior cingulate, inferior frontal and superior temporal regions gray matter volume [30]-[32] as well as reductions in prefrontal gray and white matter [21],[33],[34]. Asami and coworkers found a positive correlation between frontolimbic and left superior temporal gyrus gray matter volume reduction and negative symptoms [35]. Reduced gray matter volume in the middle prefrontal gyrus (Brodmann area 9/46 of frontal association cortex) as a correlate of negative symptoms have also been shown by other authors [36]-[38]. Following Talati and Hirsch the medial prefrontal gyrus, appendant to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, seems to be associated with executive mechanisms and decision- making abilities as a higher processing unit and might thereby modulate negative symptoms [39]. A study using diffusion tensor imaging, a method to assess the integrity of neural fiber tracks, suggests an association between impaired white matter integrity in the inferior frontal region of schizophrenic patients and the severity of negative symptoms [40]. However, in analogy to the neuropsychological findings, these results do not seem to be unambiguous, since other studies reported correlations between negative symptomatology and anomalies in other brain areas, especially the temporal regions, with the superior temporal gyrus [34],[41]-[43] and the hippocampus being most often reported [44].

So far distinct conclusions concerning the relationship between cognitive dysfunctions and negative symptoms in schizophrenia especially with respect to the cerebral correlate cannot be drawn. Based on present findings anomalies connected to the frontal and temporal lobes are primarily discussed.

A disadvantage of many neuropsychological studies examining the association between negative symptoms and neuropsychological dysfunctions is the restriction to a single cognitive faculty or a group of functions related to only one brain region, especially the frontal lobes. Furthermore only few studies have combined a neuropsychological assessment with a morphometric approach to explore the relationship with negative symptoms.

To address this question this study aims to investigate the cognitive and cerebral correlates of negative symptomatology in schizophrenia.

Schizophrenic patients and healthy controls were examined using the SANS, volumetric MRI-measures of different frontal and temporal brain structures and a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery covering frontal lobe (executive) as well as mediotemporal (memory) functions to assess the relationship between negative symptoms, neuropsychological deficits and structural brain anomalies in patients with schizophrenia.

Methods

Participants

Approval from the Albert-Ludwigs university ethics committee Freiburg was obtained before onset of the study. 20 patients with DSM-IV-defined schizophrenia, 8 female, 12 male, hospitalised for acute exacerbation and adjusted to medication, were identified among the in-patients of the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy of the University Hospital of Freiburg. The clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia was made by experienced senior consultant psychiatrists based on a structured interview according to DSM-IV criteria. After giving informed written consent to participate on a voluntary basis, patients were included in this study. All patients received a full neurological and medical history and examination as well as routine blood tests and EEG examinations to exclude any other medical or neurological disorder. Patients were excluded if they had any other DSM-IV axis I diagnosis or met criteria for substance abuse within the previous 6 months. To keep the sample homogenous by avoiding an overlap to psychogeriatric problems the study group was restricted to patients under the age of 45 years and normal IQ range. Clinical state of patients was assessed using the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) [45] and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) [11]. The overall severity of psychiatric disturbance for the present state as well as for the past two years was measured using the Global Assessment Scale [46]. The Phillips scale [47] was used to assess the premorbid adjustment of the patients. The history of illness was documented in regard to the age of onset of clinical symptoms, the duration of schizophrenia, the total amount of time hospitalized for schizophrenia and the number of psychotic episodes (Table 1). Confounding variables like family history, any history of meningitis or encephalitis or other brain injuries as well as the present medication were documented. Chlorpromazine-equivalents (CPE) were calculated for the current antipsychotic medication. Due to the vague informations provided by the patients and carers lifetime exposure to antipsychotic medication could not be measured. 32 control subjects, 12 female, 20 male, were recruited via announcements and group matched according to age, sex and school education (Table 1). Exclusion criteria were identical for the patient and control group. In addition, volunteers with a positive personal or family history of any psychiatric disorder were excluded from participation.

Table 1 Demographic data and clinical characteristics of the schizophrenic group with a high SANS score, the schizophrenic group with a low SANS score, and the control group as well as significance of results

Neuropsychological assessment

A comprehensive neuropsychological test battery was administered by a trained and experienced psychologist. The test battery covered the following neuropsychological functions: verbal memory/learning, visual memory/learning, concentration, working memory, verbal fluency, verbal intelligence, interference and abstraction/cognitive flexibility.

Verbal memory/learning were assessed using the subtest Logical Memory, trial 1 and 2, of the revised Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-R, German edition) [48] and the wordlist of the Nürnberg’s Aging Inventory (Nürnberger Altersinventar, NAI) [49]. The subtest Visual Reproduction, trial 1 and 2, of the WMS-R (German edition) was administered to measure visual memory/learning. Working memory and concentration were assessed using the subtest “digit span” of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (German edition) [50]. Interference, response inhibition, abstraction and cognitive flexibility were evaluated using the Stroop-Test (German version) [51] and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST, pc-edition) [52]. The subtest “Information” of the HAWIE-R [50] was administered to measure verbal intelligence. Verbal fluency was assessed using the verbal fluency test of the Performance Test System [53].

The tests were administered after patients had reached subacute status to avoid confounding psychotic symptoms. The neuropsychological assessment lasted between 1.5 and 2 h.

Imaging and measurements

Within two weeks to the neuropsychological assessment MRI images were obtained at the Department of Radiology at the University of Freiburg on a 2 T whole-body system (Medspec S200, Bruker BioSpin MRI, Ettlingen - Germany) using a standard quadrature head coil. T1 and T2 weighted coronal images were acquired to screen for brain pathology. For volumetric assessment a T1-weighed 3-D-data set was acquired using a modified driven equilibrium Fourier transfer sequence (MDEFT) [54] with the following parameters: TR =17 ms, TE =5.5 ms, flip angle =30, matrix 256×192×92, FOV =24×23×18 cm3. The images were transferred to a UNIX Sun workstation via a network (Sun Microsystems, Mountain View, California, USA). Volumetric measurements of the hippocampus, the amygdala, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) were performed for both hemispheres by a blinded rater after establishing a good reliability using the interactive software program MRreg [55]. The volumetric assessment followed well established and published protocols by our group [56]. Results of structural imaging as well as functional imaging aspects of parts of the study sample have been published in previous papers [57]-[61].

Statistical analysis

Demographics

To compare both groups concerning age, education and sex Student’s t-Tests and a Chi Square Test were employed, respectively.

Group comparisons – neuropsychology

The neuropsychological test scores were converted to Z-scores (standard equivalents) and single test values covering the same neuropsychological faculty were averaged to define the following factors: verbal learning/memory, visuo-spatial learning/memory, working memory/concentration and frontal lobe functioning. Schizophrenic patients and healthy controls were compared concerning the four different neuropsychological factors using multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVA) with CPE as covariate to control for medication effects. Following this post hoc multivariate analyses of covariance were calculated for each single neuropsychological test score, again considering CPE as covariate. In a second step a median split on the SANS score was performed to distinguish schizophrenic patients with high and with low SANS scores. The Median split rendered a clear cut between patients concerning negative symptoms, since the SANS global score in the low score group ranged from 0 to 16 and in the high score group from 24 to 74. The two groups differ significantly in the BPRS Score (Student’s t-Test) so that the median split is also relevant in relation to the BPRS. The statistical procedure explained above was repeated comparing schizophrenic patients with a high SANS score (HSS), schizophrenic patients with a low SANS score (LSS) and healthy controls, using multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVA). Post hoc analysis was done using Bonferroni correction.

Group comparisons – volumetric data

The volumes of the hippocampus, the amygdala, the anterior cingulate cortex, the DLPFC and the OC were corrected for differences in total brain volumes following earlier publications by our group [61]-[64]. The different volumes were also analysed in the two steps described above using Repeated Measurements ANOVA with hemisphere as intra-subject factor and CPE as covariate.

Correlation analyses

Correlations between psychopathology, neuropsychological results and volumetric data were computed for schizophrenic patients only using Spearman rank-correlation.

All data were analyzed using SPSS for Windows (Release 11.5). A p-value of p ≤ .05 was chosen as the criterion of significance for all statistical calculations.

Results

Patient and control group

The overall group of schizophrenic patients as well as the subgroup of patients with high and low negative symptom scores were matched in terms of age, sex and duration of education. There were no differences between the two schizophrenic subgroups in terms of duration of schizophrenia, age of onset of clinical symptoms, total amount of time hospitalized for schizophrenia and number of psychotic episodes (see Table 1). In relation to the BPRS schizophrenic patients with a high SANS score differ significantly from patients with a low SANS score. The BPRS scores for the patients with a high SANS score were 45,5 and a BPNRS scores for the patients with low SANS score were 27,6 points (F =7,3, p <0,01).

Group comparisons - neuropsychological performance

Schizophrenic patients vs. healthy controls

UNIANOVA showed a significantly worse performance of the schizophrenic patients compared to the healthy controls concerning verbal and visuospatial learning/memory (F =9,52, p ≤0,01; F =11,49, p ≤0,01). No differences in performance were found for working memory/concentration and frontal lobe functioning (F =0,28, n.s.; F =2,11, n.s.). Considering the single neuropsychological parameters UNIANOVAs showed a significantly worse performance of the schizophrenic patients compared to the healthy controls concerning WMS-R, Logical Memory immediate recall and delayed recall, NAI Wordlist immediate recall trials 2 and 3 as well as delayed recall, WMS-R, Visual Reproduction immediate and delayed recall and Stroop – Interference. No medication effects were found. Mean raw scores of the neuropsychological parameters for the schizophrenic and the control group as well as significance of results are shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Mean raw scores of the neuropsychological parameters for the schizophrenic and the control group as well as F- and significance of results

Schizophrenic patients with a high SANS score vs. schizophrenic patients with a low SANS score vs. healthy controls

The MANCOVAS showed significant group effects for verbal and visuospatial learning/memory (F =8,18, p ≤0,01; F =5,67, p ≤0,01). Post-hoc analysis indicated a worse performance of the schizophrenic HSS patients compared to the schizophrenic patients with a LSS and the controls concerning verbal learning/memory (mean difference =0,73, p ≤0,05; mean difference =1,08, p ≤0,01). Concerning visual learning/memory schizophrenic patients with a HSS score achieved lower scores than controls (mean difference =0,78, p ≤0,01). No differences in performance were found for working memory/concentration and frontal lobe functioning (F =0,15, n.s.; F =1,55, n.s.). No medication effects were found.

We found significant group effects for WMS-R, Logical Memory immediate and delayed recall, NAI Wordlist immediate recall trial 2–3 and delayed recall, HAWIE-R, subtest “information” WMS-R, Visual Reproduction immediate and delayed recall, Stroop – Interference, verbal fluency, WCST, no. of trials to complete category 1 and no. of perseverative errors.

Post-hoc analyses indicated a worse performance of the schizophrenic patients with a HSS compared to the controls concerning WMS-R, Logical Memory immediate recall and delayed recall, NAI Wordlist immediate recall, trial 2 and trial 3 and WMS-R, NAI Wordlist delayed recall, HAWIE-R, subtest “information”, WMS-R, Visual Reproduction delayed recall, Stroop – Interference and WCST, no. of trials to complete category 1. Schizophrenic patients with a HSS also achieved lower test scores than schizophrenic patients with a LSS concerning WMS-R, Logical Memory delayed recall. Schizophrenic patients with a LSS showed a worse performance than controls in regard to WMS-R, Visual Reproduction immediate recall, Stroop – Interference and WCST, perseverative errors. The mean raw scores of the neuropsychological tests for the two schizophrenic groups and the control group as well as significance of results are shown in Table 3.

Table 3 Mean raw scores of the neuropsychological parameters for the schizophrenic group with a high SANS score (S1), the schizophrenic group with a low SANS score (S2), and the control group (C) as well as significance of results

Group comparisons – volumetric findings

Schizophrenic patients vs. healthy controls

In 10 control subjects the quality of structural 3-D-data sets was insufficient for further analysis. There were no significant differences between the schizophrenic and the control group with respect to total brain volume (tbv; F =0,89; p = n.s.) as well as the volumes of the hippocampus (F =0,40; p = n.s.), the amygdala (F =0,13; p = n.s.), the anterior cingulate cortex (F =0,00; p = n.s.), the DLPFC (F =0,79; p = n.s.) or the OC (F =0,69; p = n.s.). Neither medication effects (hippocampus: F =0,91; amygdala: F =0,36; anterior cingulate cortex: F =0,39; DLPFC: F =0,15; OC: F =0,33; tbv: F =0,36; p = n.s.) nor any interaction with medication (hippocampus: F =3,11; amygdala: F =2,57; anterior cingulate cortex: F =0,00; DLPFC: F =1,64; OC: F =0,37; p = n.s.) or group (hippocampus: F =0,42; amygdala: F =2,01; anterior cingulate cortex: F =0,07; DLPFC: F =0,01; OC: F =0,05; p = n.s.) were observed. For the hippocampus a significant effect for hemisphere was found (F =15,47; p ≤0,01), indicating a greater right volume of the hippocampus. The same was true for the amygdala (F =5,51; p ≤0,05) and the anterior cingulate cortex (F =4,13; p ≤0,05). An effect for hemisphere was also found for the DLPFC with a greater left than right DLPFC (F =6,81; p ≤0,05). For the OC hemisphere did not show any effects (F =2,16; p = n.s.). The volumetric data for the two schizophrenic groups and the control group are shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean corrected volumes of the amygdalae, hippocampi, anterior cingulate cortices, DLPFC and OC for the schizophrenic group with a high SANS score, the schizophrenic group with a low SANS score, and the control group [error bar indicates Standard error].

Schizophrenic patients with a high SANS score vs. schizophrenic patients with a low SANS score vs. healthy controls

The UNIANOVA did not show any group effects for total brain volume (F =0,43; p = n.s.). Repeated Measurements ANOVA revealed no group effects (F =0,18; p = n.s.) for the hippocampus. There was a significant effect for hemisphere (F =5,89; p ≤0,05), indicating a greater right volume of the hippocampus, and a significant interaction with group (F =3,37; p ≤0,05). For the anterior cingulate cortex there were no statistically significant effects for group (F =0,56; p = n.s.) or the interaction between group and hemisphere (F =0,12; p = n.s.), but a significant effect for hemisphere corresponding to the lateralization of the hippocampus (F =6,81; p ≤0,05). No group (F =0,54; p = n.s.) or hemisphere (F =2,52; p = n.s.) effects or an interaction between group and hemisphere (F =0,68; p = n.s.) were found for the amygdala. The same was true for the DLPFC (group: F =1,28; p = n.s.; hemisphere: F =3,13; p = n.s.; interaction with group: F =0,71; p = n.s.) and the OC (group: F =2,45; p = n.s.; hemisphere: F =1,55; p = n.s.; interaction with group: F =0,32; p = n.s.). Post hoc analyses did not show any significant differences between any of the three groups in regard to total brain volume, the volume of the hippocampus, the amygdala, the anterior cingulate cortex, the DLPFC and the OC.

Correlation analysis

Correlations between SANS sub scores and SANS global score, respectively, and neuropsychological performance are primarily found for indicators of verbal learning/memory. There were no significant correlations between the volumes of the hippocampus, the amygdala, the DLPFC and the OC on the one hand and scores of negative symptoms on the other hand. A positive correlation between total brain volume and the SANS sub score attention was found. Results of correlation analysis can be seen in Table 4.

Table 4 Significant correlations between neuropsychological test results and SANS subscores/SANS global score

Discussion and conclusions

In this study, schizophrenic patients displayed pronounced impairments in verbal and visuospatial memory as well as deficits concerning response inhibition. Dividing the schizophrenic group in patients with low and high SANS scores demonstrated especially strong memory, but also executive impairments in the HSS patients, whereas the LSS patients were relatively unimpaired. Correlations between SANS sub scores and global score, respectively, were mainly found with indicators of verbal memory. Anomalies in frontal or temporal brain regions or associations with neuropsychological variables were not found. These findings underline the association between negative symptomatology in schizophrenia and pronounced neuropsychological deficits and emphasise the relevance of verbal memory impairments.

The impact of negative symptoms on neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia

The neuropsychological findings in our schizophrenic group are in general agreement with findings of other studies showing specific deficits concerning learning and memory in schizophrenic patients [65].

Although the prevalence of executive dysfunctions in schizophrenia is estimated to be around 90% [66] we could not confirm impairments in frontal lobe functioning with the exception of difficulties in response inhibition as assessed by the Stroop task. Results of a study by Hutton [67] indicates, that the type of executive impairment might depend on the phase of illness. At the beginning of the disease process patients display deficits of planning and strategical thinking, whereas in chronic patients this shifts towards impairments concerning categorization and flexibility as assessed by the WCST. Since on average our schizophrenic patients have had only 2 schizophrenic episodes, this might explain the negative WCST results.

Studies have already shown the impact of negative symptomatology on certain neuropsychological functions in schizophrenic patients. Our results confirm the association between negative symptomatology in schizophrenia and pronounced neuropsychological deficits already found by others [22],[26],[27],[29]. Patients with a high SANS global score showed significantly more pronounced deficits especially concerning mnestic functioning than patients with a low SANS global score.

These results underline the close connection between negative symptomatology and verbal memory on the background of macroscopically unchanged structures in frontal and temporal cerebral areas. This is in contrast to other studies, which primarily reported a correlation between negative symptoms and executive functioning, sometimes associated with the corresponding structural changes in frontal lobe regions [19],[20],[22].

Following the general line of thought a correlation between negative symptoms and executive – or so called frontal lobe - impairments would have been expected, since negative symptoms are assumed to be a consequence of frontal lobe impairment [68]. On the other hand Hill and coworkers observed an amplifying effect of negative symptomatology in combination with disorganized behaviour on learning and memory deficits in schizophrenic patients, thereby confirming the results of this study [69].

The schizophrenic group in this study showed a relatively high level of functioning in the neuropsychological categories studied here. The patients showed almost no executive impairments and no deficits concerning concentration and working memory. Patients with high and low negative symptoms could mainly be separated by their memory performance, even HSS patients could adhere their executive functioning. Memory, especially concerning the verbal modality, should be regarded as left temporal lobe functions in the majority of people and might be more vulnerable in regard to the cerebral changes associated with a schizophrenic disorder than other neuropsychological functions.

Interestingly the schizophrenic patients in this study did neither not show any change in the examined cerebral structures nor any relationship between neuropsychological measures and the examined anatomical faculties. Current findings report contradictory results. Some studies have found smaller volumes of brain structures in schizophrenic patients, especially of the hippocampus, the amygdala and the anterior cingulate cortex [36],[70]-[72]. Whereas other findings could neither find any volumetric differences in the hippocampus-amygdala-complex nor in the different frontal lobe regions [73],[74]. Possible explanations for these inconsistencies might be differences in the kind of volume assessment, e.g. morphometric measurements using SPM or manual tracing of a structure according to validated protocols. However, such contradictions could also relate to the fact that the different study samples vary in terms of underlying pathophysiology [75]. Especially in schizophrenia it is extremely difficult to assess a sufficiently homogenous population to be able to generalize findings to the results of other studies. The coexistence of normal volumes of the examined frontal and temporal faculties on the one hand and memory impairments as well as deficits in response inhibition on the other hand allow different interpretations. A functional recovery may have taken place before the structural regeneration. It could also be the case, that the macroscopically normal volume of a certain brain area might mask more subtle histological alterations or functional anomalies not related to structural change.

This leaves the question of the organic basis for the observed deficits and especially the correlation between negative symptomatology and verbal memory impairments unanswered. As macro-structural aberrations do not seem to be the organic basis for the correlation between negative symptoms and verbal memory impairment, biochemical or physiological mechanisms as the underlying common cerebral correlate have to be considered. Glutamate seems to be a good candidate to explain our correlation results. The glutamate hypothesis of schizophrenia was developed after it had been recognized that Phencyclidine (PCP) leads to an exacerbation of symptoms in schizophrenic patients [76] and produces a schizophrenia-like state in normal individuals with positive and negative symptoms as well as cognitive disturbances [77],[78]. PCP serves as a noncompetitive antagonist at the glutamatergic NMDA receptor [79]. Glutamate acts via different receptor types, but so far the main focus of the relationship between glutamate and schizophrenia has been on the NMDA receptor. It plays a major role in long-term potentiation, a synaptical process involved in memory formation [80]. Besides its role concerning mnestic functions glutamate also affects schizophrenic negative symptomatology. Using D-serine, a full agonist at the glycine modulatory site of the NMDA receptor, Tsai et al. (1998) have found significant improvements in negative symptoms, psychosis, and executive function as measured by the cognitive subscale of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [81]. Similarly D-cycloserine, a partial agonist at the glycine modulatory site of the NMDA receptor, has led to a diminution of negative symptomatology either alone or added to conventional antipsychotics [82],[83]. Considering these results glutamate might be the latent variable underlying the relationship between mnestic deficits and negative symptomatology observed in this study.

Methodological issues

Some methodological issues have to be considered. Dividing a group by a median split could be criticised due to the artificial allocation of patients to a certain group, who would otherwise be described on a continuum. In this study the highest SANS score of the LSS group was 16, whereas the lowest SANS score of the HSS group was 24. This yielded a clear differentiation of schizophrenic patients with low and high negative symptoms.

The chosen neuropsychological tests had to exist in a German version thereby limiting the range of possible assessment procedures. Nevertheless only those tests were taken that are profoundly validated and broadly accepted as neuropsychological diagnostic tools. The neuropsychological tests were administered by a trained and experienced neuropsychologist, guaranteeing an adherent administration. The volumetric method used is sound and has been described in a number of other publications [59]-[61]. However, according to our measurement protocol we did not separate gray from white matter and thus we cannot commend on whether there might have been a volume reduction of one of these compartments in the different groups.

The patient assessment was done by an experienced team of clinicians on a ward specialized on the treatment of chronic schizophrenia and the psychopathology was validated and quantified using internationally accepted psychometric tools.

Summary

In summary in this study we present data of a carefully diagnosed sample of patients with schizophrenia and control subjects. Following broad and parallel clinical, psychometric and MRI-based volumetric assessment of temporal and frontal lobe areas we did not identify group overall volumetric differences in frontal or temporal brain volumes. High scores of negative symptoms were categorically and dimensionally related in particular to verbal memory deficits rather than to dysexecutive dysfunction. Therefore the main finding of this study is, that from a functional point of view and in spite of absence of clear macroscopic volumetric brain differences negative symptoms are more related to left temporal brain properties (verbal memory deficits) than to frontal lobe dysfunction (dysexecutive symptoms). Further research should specifically adress this important research question and should possibly test the putative role of temporal glutamatergic dysfunction and its relationship to negative symptoms.

Ethic committee

AZ: 23–7532.22-11/1 L TvE, Ethik-Kommission der Albert-Ludwigs-Universität, Engelbergerstr. 21, 79106 Freiburg.

Authors’ contributions

HMO designed the study, wrote the protocol and participated in data analysis and interpretation. GV managed patient recruitment and data collection and participated in data analysis, interpretation and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. LTvE participated in design, analysis and interpretation of the data. TH participated in design, analysis and interpretation and wrote the final draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ information

Hans M Olbrich and Ludger Tebartz van Elst senior authors contributed equally to the paper.

References

  1. Crow TJ: The two-syndrome concept: origins and current status. Schizophr Bull. 1985, 11: 471-486.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Fenton WS, McGlashan TH: Natural history of schizophrenia subtypes. I. Longitudinal study of paranoid, hebephrenic, and undifferentiated schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991, 48: 969-977.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Fenton WS, McGlashan TH: Natural history of schizophrenia subtypes. II. Positive and negative symptoms and long-term course. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991, 48: 978-986.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Hoffmann H, Kupper Z: Relationships between social competence, psychopathology and work performance and their predictive value for vocational rehabilitation of schizophrenic outpatients. Schizophr Res. 1997, 23: 69-79.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Moller HJ, Bottlender R, Wegner U, Wittmann J, Strauss A: Long-term course of schizophrenic, affective and schizoaffective psychosis: focus on negative symptoms and their impact on global indicators of outcome. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 2000, 407: 54-57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Hoffmann H, Kupper Z: [Predictive factors of successful vocational re-integration in patients with chronic schizophrenia]. Psychiatr Prax. 2003, 30: 312-317.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. McGurk SR, Mueser KT, Harvey PD, LaPuglia R, Marder J: Cognitive and symptom predictors of work outcomes for clients with schizophrenia in supported employment. Psychiatr Serv. 2003, 54: 1129-1135.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Wieselgren IM, Lindstrom E, Lindstrom LH: Symptoms at index admission as predictor for 1–5 year outcome in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1996, 94: 311-319.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Green MF: What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia?. Am J Psychiatry. 1996, 153: 321-330.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Umbricht DS, Wirshing WC, Wirshing DA, McMeniman M, Schooler NR, Marder SR, Kane JM: Clinical predictors of response to clozapine treatment in ambulatory patients with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychiatry. 2002, 63: 420-424.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Andreasen NC: The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS): conceptual and theoretical foundations. Br J Psychiatry Suppl. 1989, 7: 49-58.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Andreasen NC: Negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Definition and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1982, 39: 784-788.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Christensen BK, Patrick RE, Stuss DT, Gillingham S, Zipursky RB: Verbal Episodic Memory Impairment in Schizophrenia: A Comparison with Frontal Lobe Lesion Patients. Clin Neuropsychol. 2013, 27 (4): 647-666.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Goto M, Abe O, Miyati T, Yoshikawa T, Hayashi N, Takao H, Inano S, Kabasawa H, Mori H, Kunimatsu A, Aoki S, Ino K, Ida K, Yano K, Ohtomo K: Entorhinal cortex volume measured with 3 T MRI is positively correlated with the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised logical/verbal memory score for healthy subjects. Neuroradiology. 2011, 53: 617-622.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Wang CS, Yang YK, Chen M, Chiu NT, Yeh TL, Lee IH: Negative symptoms and regional cerebral blood flow in patients with schizophrenia: a single photon emission computed tomography study. Kaohsiung J Med Sci. 2003, 19: 464-469.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Vaiva G, Thomas P, Llorca PM, Dupont S, Cottencin O, Devos P, Mazas O, Rascle C, Steinling M, Goudemand M: SPECT imaging, clinical features, and cognition before and after low doses of amisulpride in schizophrenic patients with the deficit syndrome. Psychiatry Res. 2002, 115: 37-48.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Andreasen NC, Rezai K, Alliger R, Swayze VW, Flaum M, Kirchner P, Cohen B, O’Leary DS: Hypofrontality in neuroleptic-naive patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Assessment with xenon 133 single-photon emission computed tomography and the Tower of London. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992, 49: 943-958.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Liddle PF, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS: Cerebral blood flow and mental processes in schizophrenia. J R Soc Med. 1992, 85: 224-227.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Botero S, Munoz CC, Ocampo MV, Escobar M, Rangel A, Quintero C, Marin C, Jaramillo LE, Sanchez R, Rodrigues-Losada R, Beltran D, Ospina J, Palacio C, Arango JCAquirre-Acevedo DC, Paez AL, Valencia AV, Garcia J: Verbal working memory in individuals with schizophrenia and their first degree relatives: relationship with negative and disorganized symptoms. Actas Esp Psiquiatr. 2013, 41: 106-114.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ziauddeen H, Dibben C, Kipps C, Hodges JR, McKenna PJ: Negative schizophrenic symptoms and the frontal lobe syndrome: one and the same?. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2011, 261: 59-67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Sanfilipo M, Lafargue T, Rusinek H, Arena L, Loneragan C, Lautin A, Feiner D, Rotrosen J, Wolkin A: Volumetric measure of the frontal and temporal lobe regions in schizophrenia: relationship to negative symptoms. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000, 57: 471-480.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Schuepbach D, Keshavan MS, Kmiec JA, Sweeney JA: Negative symptom resolution and improvements in specific cognitive deficits after acute treatment in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2002, 53: 249-261.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Liddle PF: Schizophrenic syndromes, cognitive performance and neurological dysfunction. Psychol Med. 1987, 17: 49-57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Bagney A, Rodriguez-Jimenez R, Martinez-Gras I, Sanchez-Morla EM, Santos JL, Jimenez-Arriero MA, Lobo A, McGorry PD, Palomo T: Negative Symptoms and Executive Function in Schizophrenia: Does Their Relationship Change with Illness Duration?. Psychopathology. 2012, 46 (4): 241-248.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Pelletier M, Achim AM, Montoya A, Lal S, Lepage M: Cognitive and clinical moderators of recognition memory in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2005, 74: 233-252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Heydebrand G, Weiser M, Rabinowitz J, Hoff AL, DeLisi LE, Csernansky JG: Correlates of cognitive deficits in first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004, 68: 1-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Berman I, Viegner B, Merson A, Allan E, Pappas D, Green AI: Differential relationships between positive and negative symptoms and neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 1997, 25: 1-10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Lee JS, Park HJ, Chun JW, Seok JH, Park IH, Park B, Kim JJ: Neuroanatomical correlates of trait anhedonia in patients with schizophrenia: a voxel-based morphometric study. Neurosci Lett. 2011, 489: 110-114.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Roth RM, Flashman LA, Saykin AJ, McAllister TW, Vidaver R: Apathy in schizophrenia: reduced frontal lobe volume and neuropsychological deficits. Am J Psychiatry. 2004, 161: 157-159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Benoit A, Bodnar M, Malla AK, Joober R, Lepage M: The structural neural substrates of persistent negative symptoms in first-episode of non-affective psychosis: a voxel-based morphometry study. Front Psychiatry. 2012, 3: 42-

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Ha TH, Youn T, Ha KS, Rho KS, Lee JM, Kim IY, Kim SI, Kwon JS: Gray matter abnormalities in paranoid schizophrenia and their clinical correlations. Psychiatry Res. 2004, 132: 251-260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Baare WF, Hulshoff Pol HE, Hijman R, Mali WP, Viergever MA, Kahn RS: Volumetric analysis of frontal lobe regions in schizophrenia: relation to cognitive function and symptomatology. Biol Psychiatry. 1999, 45: 1597-1605.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Bora E, Fornito A, Radua J, Walterfang M, Seal M, Wood SJ, Yücel M, Velakoulis D, Pantelis C: Neuroanatomical abnormalities in schizophrenia: a multimodal voxelwise meta-analysis and meta-regression analysis. Schizophr Res. 2011, 127: 46-57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Mathalon DH, Sullivan EV, Lim KO, Pfefferbaum A: Progressive brain volume changes and the clinical course of schizophrenia in men: a longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001, 58: 148-157.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Asami T, Whitford TJ, Bouix S, Dickey CC, Niznikiewicz M, Shenton ME, Vogelmaier MM, McCarley RW: Globally and locally reduced MRI gray matter volumes in neuroleptic-naive men with schizotypal personality disorder: association with negative symptoms. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013, 70: 361-372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Shepherd AM, Laurens KR, Matheson SL, Carr VJ, Green MJ: Systematic meta-review and quality assessment of the structural brain alterations in schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2012, 36: 1342-1356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Berge D, Carmona S, Rovira M, Bulbena A, Salgado P, Vilarroya O: Gray matter volume deficits and correlation with insight and negative symptoms in first-psychotic-episode subjects. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2011, 123: 431-439.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Ananth H, Popescu I, Critchley HD, Good CD, Frackowiak RS, Dolan RJ: Cortical and subcortical gray matter abnormalities in schizophrenia determined through structural magnetic resonance imaging with optimized volumetric voxel-based morphometry. Am J Psychiatry. 2002, 159: 1497-1505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Talati A, Hirsch J: Functional specialization within the medial frontal gyrus for perceptual go/no-go decisions based on “what”, “when”, and “where” related information: an fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005, 17: 981-993.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Wolkin A, Choi SJ, Szilagyi S, Sanfilipo M, Rotrosen JP, Lim KO: Inferior frontal white matter anisotropy and negative symptoms of schizophrenia: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Am J Psychiatry. 2003, 160: 572-574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Kim JJ, Crespo-Facorro B, Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Magnotta V, Nopoulos P: Morphology of the lateral superior temporal gyrus in neuroleptic nai;ve patients with schizophrenia: relationship to symptoms. Schizophr Res. 2003, 60: 173-181.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Anderson JE, Wible CG, McCarley RW, Jakab M, Kasai K, Shenton ME: An MRI study of temporal lobe abnormalities and negative symptoms in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2002, 58: 123-134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Turetsky B, Cowell PE, Gur RC, Grossman RI, Shtasel DL, Gur RE: Frontal and temporal lobe brain volumes in schizophrenia. Relationship to symptoms and clinical subtype. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995, 52: 1061-1070.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Rajarethinam R, DeQuardo JR, Miedler J, Arndt S, Kirbat R, Brunberg JA, Tandon R: Hippocampus and amygdala in schizophrenia: assessment of the relationship of neuroanatomy to psychopathology. Psychiatry Res. 2001, 108: 79-87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Overall JE, Gorham DR: The brief psychiatric rating scale. Psychological reports. 1962, 89 (3): 259-268.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Endicott J, Spitzer RL, Fleiss JL, Cohen J: The global assessment scale. A procedure for measuring overall severity of psychiatric disturbance. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1976, 33: 766-771.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. PHILLIPS L: Case history data and prognosis in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1953, 117: 515-525.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Wechsler D: Wechsler Memory Scale. 1973, Psychological Corporation, New York

    Google Scholar 

  49. Oswald WD: Das Nürnberger-Alters-Inventar. 1997, Hogrefe, Göttingen

    Google Scholar 

  50. HAWIE-R: Hamburg-Wechsler- Intelligenztest für Erwachsene. Revision 1991; Handbuch und Testanweisung. 1991, Hans, Huber, Bern

  51. Bäumler G: Farb-Wort-Interferenztest (FWIT) nach J.R. Stroop. 1985, Hogrefe, Göttingen

    Google Scholar 

  52. Monchi O, Petrides M, Petre V, Worsley K, Dagher A: Wisconsin Card Sorting revisited: distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2001, 21: 7733-7741.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Horn W: L-P-S Leistungsprüfsystem. 2.Auflage. 1983, Hogrefe, Göttingen

    Google Scholar 

  54. Ugurbil K, Garwood M, Ellermann J, Hendrich K, Hinke R, Hu X, Kim SG, Menon R, Merkle H, Ogawa S: Imaging at high magnetic fields: initial experiences at 4 T. Magn Reson Q. 1993, 9: 259-277.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Lemieux L, Liu RS, Duncan JS: Hippocampal and cerebellar volumetry in serially acquired MRI volume scans. Magn Reson Imaging. 2000, 18: 1027-1033.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Cendes F, Andermann F, Gloor P, Evans A, Jones-Gotman M, Watson C, Melanson D, Oliver A, Peters T, Lopes-Cendes I: MRI volumetric measurement of amygdala and hippocampus in temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology. 1993, 43: 719-725.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Rusch N, Tebartz vE, Valerius G, Buchert M, Thiel T, Ebert D, Henning J, Olbrich HM: Neurochemical and structural correlates of executive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2008, 99: 155-163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Tebartz VE, Baumer D, Ebert D, Trimble MR: Chronic antidopaminergic medication might affect amygdala structure in patients with schizophrenia. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2004, 37: 217-220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Hesslinger B, Thiel T, Tebartz VE, Hennig J, Ebert D: Attention-deficit disorder in adults with or without hyperactivity: where is the difference? A study in humans using short echo (1)H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Neurosci Lett. 2001, 304: 117-119.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Tebartz vE, Woermann F, Lemieux L, Trimble MR: Increased amygdala volumes in female and depressed humans. A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci Lett. 2000, 281: 103-106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Tebartz vE, Hesslinger B, Thiel T, Geiger E, Haegele K, Lemieux L, Lieb K, Bohus M, Henning J, Ebert D: Frontolimbic brain abnormalities in patients with borderline personality disorder: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2003, 54: 163-171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Tebartz vE, Greenlee MW, Foley JM, Lucking CH: Contrast detection, discrimination and adaptation in patients with Parkinson’s disease and multiple system atrophy. Brain. 1997, 120 (Pt 12): 2219-2228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Tebartz vE, Baeumer D, Lemieux L, Woermann FG, Koepp M, Krishnamoorthy S, Thompson PJ, Ebert D, Trimble MR: Amygdala pathology in psychosis of epilepsy: A magnetic resonance imaging study in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain. 2002, 125: 140-149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Tebartz vE, Woermann FG, Lemieux L, Trimble MR: Amygdala enlargement in dysthymia--a volumetric study of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Biol Psychiatry. 1999, 46: 1614-1623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Aleman A, Hijman R, de Haan EH, Kahn RS: Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 1999, 156: 1358-1366.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Velligan DI, Bow-Thomas CC: Executive function in schizophrenia. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry. 1999, 4: 24-33.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Hutton SB, Puri BK, Duncan LJ, Robbins TW, Barnes TR, Joyce EM: Executive function in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 1998, 28: 463-473.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Mattson DT, Berk M, Lucas MD: A neuropsychological study of prefrontal lobe function in the positive and negative subtypes of schizophrenia. J Genet Psychol. 1997, 158: 487-494.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Hill SK, Ragland JD, Gur RC, Gur RE: Neuropsychological profiles delineate distinct profiles of schizophrenia, an interaction between memory and executive function, and uneven distribution of clinical subtypes. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2002, 24: 765-780.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Vita A, De Peri L, Deste G, Sacchetti E: Progressive loss of cortical gray matter in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis and meta-regression of longitudinal MRI studies. Transl Psychiatry. 2012, 2: e190-

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Fujiwara H, Hirao K, Namiki C, Yamada M, Shimizu M, Fukuyama H, Hayashi T, Murai T: Anterior cingulate pathology and social cognition in schizophrenia: a study of gray matter, white matter and sulcal morphometry. Neuroimage. 2007, 36: 1236-1245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Noga JT, Aylward E, Barta PE, Pearlson GD: Cingulate gyrus in schizophrenic patients and normal volunteers. Psychiatry Res. 1995, 61: 201-208.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Vita A, De Peri L, Silenzi C, Dieci M: Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of quantitative magnetic resonance imaging studies. Schizophr Res. 2006, 82: 75-88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Braff DL, Swerdlow NR: Neuroanatomy of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1997, 23: 509-512.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Tebartz vE, Ebert D, Hesslinger B: Depression--augmentation or switch after initial SSRI treatment. N Engl J Med. 2006, 354: 2611-2613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Itil T, Keskiner A, Kiremitci N, Holden JM: Effect of phencyclidine in chronic schizophrenics. Can Psychiatr Assoc J. 1967, 12: 209-212.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Murray JB: Phencyclidine (PCP): a dangerous drug, but useful in schizophrenia research. J Psychol. 2002, 136: 319-327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Luby E, Cohen BDRG, Gottlieb JS, Kelley R: Study of a new schizophrenomimetic drug; sernyl. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1959, 81: 363-369.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Moller P, Husby R: The initial prodrome in schizophrenia: searching for naturalistic core dimensions of experience and behavior. Schizophr Bull. 2000, 26: 217-232.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Bliss TV, Collingridge GL: A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993, 361: 31-39.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Tsai G, Yang P, Chung LC, Lange N, Coyle JT: D-serine added to antipsychotics for the treatment of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 1998, 44: 1081-1089.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. van Berkel PH, van Veen HA, Geerts ME, de Boer HA, Nuijens JH: Heterogeneity in utilization of N-glycosylation sites Asn624 and Asn138 in human lactoferrin: a study with glycosylation-site mutants. Biochem J. 1996, 319 (Pt 1): 117-122.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Goff DC, Tsai G, Levitt J, Amico E, Manoach D, Schoenfeld DA, Hayden DL, McCarley R, Coyle JT: A placebo-controlled trial of D-cycloserine added to conventional neuroleptics in patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999, 56: 21-27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all subjects who have participated in this study. This work was supported by a grant from the Ministry of Science, Research and the Arts of Baden-Württemberg AZ: 23–7532.22-11/1 to Dr. Tebartz van Elst.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ludger Tebartz van Elst.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests. We certify that there is no conflict of interest with any financial organization or non- financial competing interests regarding the material discussed in the manuscript.

No reimbursements, fees, funding or salary from an organization that could lose financially was received by any author in the past five years or any stocks or shares are hold. The authors don’t hold any patents relating to the content of this manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver ( https://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/ ) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hornig, T., Valerius, G., Feige, B. et al. Neuropsychological and cerebral morphometric aspects of negative symptoms in schizophrenia: negative symptomatology is associated with specific mnestic deficits in schizophrenic patients. BMC Psychiatry 14, 326 (2014). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-014-0326-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12888-014-0326-4

Keywords