Skip to main content

Which behaviour change techniques are effective to promote physical activity and reduce sedentary behaviour in adults: a factorial randomized trial of an e- and m-health intervention

Abstract

Background

E- and m-health interventions are promising to change health behaviour. Many of these interventions use a large variety of behaviour change techniques (BCTs), but it’s not known which BCTs or which combination of BCTs contribute to their efficacy. Therefore, this experimental study investigated the efficacy of three BCTs (i.e. action planning, coping planning and self-monitoring) and their combinations on physical activity (PA) and sedentary behaviour (SB) against a background set of other BCTs.

Methods

In a 2 (action planning: present vs absent) × 2 (coping planning: present vs absent) × 2 (self-monitoring: present vs absent) factorial trial, 473 adults from the general population used the self-regulation based e- and m-health intervention ‘MyPlan2.0’ for five weeks. All combinations of BCTs were considered, resulting in eight groups. Participants selected their preferred target behaviour, either PA (n = 335, age = 35.8, 28.1% men) or SB (n = 138, age = 37.8, 37.7% men), and were then randomly allocated to the experimental groups. Levels of PA (MVPA in minutes/week) or SB (total sedentary time in hours/day) were assessed at baseline and post-intervention using self-reported questionnaires. Linear mixed-effect models were fitted to assess the impact of the different combinations of the BCTs on PA and SB.

Results

First, overall efficacy of each BCT was examined. The delivery of self-monitoring increased PA (t = 2.735, p = 0.007) and reduced SB (t = − 2.573, p = 0.012) compared with no delivery of self-monitoring. Also, the delivery of coping planning increased PA (t = 2.302, p = 0.022) compared with no delivery of coping planning. Second, we investigated to what extent adding BCTs increased efficacy. Using the combination of the three BCTs was most effective to increase PA (x2 = 8849, p = 0.003) whereas the combination of action planning and self-monitoring was most effective to decrease SB (x2 = 3.918, p = 0.048). To increase PA, action planning was always more effective in combination with coping planning (x2 = 5.590, p = 0.014; x2 = 17.722, p < 0.001; x2 = 4.552, p = 0.033) compared with using action planning without coping planning. Of note, the use of action planning alone reduced PA compared with using coping planning alone (x2 = 4.389, p = 0.031) and self-monitoring alone (x2 = 8.858, p = 003), respectively.

Conclusions

This study provides indications that different (combinations of) BCTs may be effective to promote PA and reduce SB. More experimental research to investigate the effectiveness of BCTs is needed, which can contribute to improved design and more effective e- and m-health interventions in the future.

Trial registration

This study was preregistered as a clinical trial (ID number: NCT03274271). Release date: 20 October 2017.

Background

The prevalence of non-communicable diseases such as type 2 diabetes, cancer, osteoarthritis, depression and cardiovascular diseases in adults is high and rising [1]. These diseases are often attributed to unhealthy lifestyles [1]. Increasing physical activity (PA) and reducing sedentary behaviour (SB) are key to reduce the burden of these diseases [2,3,4,5]. The recommendation of the World Health Organisation for PA is to engage in at least 150 min PA at moderate to vigorous intensity each week [6, 7]. However, worldwide 31.1% of adults is insufficiently physically active [8]. For SB, only a few countries have formulated recommendations. For example, in Belgium, adults are recommended to restrict their sitting time to a maximum of 8 h per day and to interrupt sitting every 20 to 30 min [6]. Notwithstanding, Belgian adults sit on average 8.3 h a day [9]. This is in line with other studies revealing that adults spend on average 8.5 h sitting a day [10]. As such, there is a clear need to develop effective interventions that promote PA and reduce SB on a large scale.

The use of technologies, such as electronic (e-) or mobile (m-) health interventions, is one strategy to promote health behaviour. E- and m- health is defined as “the use of information and communications technology, especially the internet, to improve or enable health and health care” [11]. These interventions are promising: They can use a personal and interactive approach while reaching a large group of participants at a relatively low cost [12, 13].

Commercial and theory-based e- and m-health interventions are available. Most of them target only the intention towards health behaviour (e.g. by targeting knowledge) [14]. However, intentions do not always translate into the actual behaviour [15], a phenomenon known as the “intention-behaviour gap” [16]. Several theoretical models have proposed ways to bridge this gap. These models, amongst which self-regulation models [16, 17], guide individuals to change their behaviour, beginning with the development of an intention over the actual adoption of the behaviour to finally behaviour maintenance [18]. In doing so, self-regulation models, as for example the Health Action Process Approach (HAPA) model [19], address not only pre-intentional but also post-intentional processes of behaviour change.

Several behaviour change techniques (BCTs) may target post-intentional processes. In this paper, the focus is on ‘action planning’, ‘coping planning’ and ‘self-monitoring’ [19, 20]. These techniques are key within the HAPA-model and are well established to bridge the intention-behaviour gap [15, 19, 21, 22]. The HAPA model is frequently used for the promotion of PA [15, 21, 23] in both clinical [24, 25] and non-clinical populations. The model is generic and flexible, and can easily be applied to several behaviours [26] such as dietary behaviour [19, 26], sunscreen use [27], and sedentary behaviour [28,29,30]. Therefore we chose to focus on the BCTs ‘action planning’, ‘coping planning’ and ‘self-monitoring’ for both PA and SB.

BCTs are most often used as part of complex interventions, consisting of many other techniques. The efficacy of single BCTs or particular combinations of BCTs remains unclear. There are some indications in the literature that some BCTs are better than others. In their meta-analysis, Michie et al. [31] reported that ‘self-monitoring’ combined with at least one other technique was more effective in changing healthy eating and physical activity than interventions that did not include ‘self-monitoring’. Nonetheless, firm conclusions are not yet possible, because of the non-experimental nature of the design, precluding cause-effect inferences. What is needed is experimental research investigating the efficacy of single BCTs, or the efficacy of combinations of BCTs. Such studies are largely lacking [31, 32]. Furthermore, insight into the efficacy of single BCTs can provide guidance in developing future interventions. Only including effective BCTs may create more efficient interventions [33]. This is important for e- and m-health interventions as they often suffer from high attrition rates (60–80%) [34]. Interventions with many BTCs may make interventions time-consuming, less engaging and more confusing [35, 36]. Including only effective techniques may be promising to lower the burden on participants and to reduce attrition rates.

Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate the specific role of three BCTs that target post-intentional processes [19] in changing PA and SB. In a first step, we investigated the overall efficacy of each of the three BCTs (i.e. action planning, coping planning, self-monitoring) in an e- and m-health intervention. In a second step, we used an additive approach to identify the effective BCTs or their combinations. We investigated to what extent adding BCTs increased efficacy. We applied an experimental approach and conducted a factorial trial using the e- and m-health intervention to increase PA and reduce SB among adults.

Methods

Study design

A 2x2x2 factorial randomized controlled trial was carried out to evaluate the efficacy of three BCTs (action planning, coping planning and self-monitoring) and their combinations [32]. The protocol was preregistered [37]. Participants were randomly allocated to one of eight experimental groups, with each experimental group receiving a unique combination of either none, one, two or all three BCTs (see Table 1). The CONSORT 2010 checklist and TIDieR checklist were taken into account [38, 39]; the completed checklists are included as Additional files 1 and 2.

Table 1 Combinations of the three BCTs for each experimental group

Participants and procedure

Adults (18–70 year) were recruited either in person by researchers at the city library of Ghent (which is also a meeting place and strongly focusses on innovation and research in collaboration with Ghent University), or through social media (i.e. Facebook) between February 2018 and December 2018. Only Dutch-speaking adults who had internet access at home and/or work and owned a smartphone (iOS or Android) were eligible. In addition, individuals had to complete the ‘Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire’ (PAR-Q) [40] as a screening instrument to detect individuals at risk for adverse effects when being more physically active (e.g. individuals with heart problems). The PAR-Q is a self-report instrument consisting of seven items with ‘yes’ or ‘no’ answers. Only those who answered ‘no’ to all items were eligible for the study. Eligible participants were then provided with more information about the study either in person (for those recruited at the library) or via email (for those recruited via social media).

The intervention lasted for 5 weeks. It consisted of 5 weekly website sessions and a mobile application, which could be used at any time during these 5 weeks. The procedure of the study is shown in Fig. 1. Participants were measured at 2 time points; pre-test (T0) and post-test (T1). The pre-test measurements were conducted via an online survey tool (Limesurvey, 2017), and included: 1) an agreement requiring them to provide informed consent, 2) sociodemographic information, 3) information about the difference between PA and SB, followed by the question about whether they wanted to improve PA or SB, and 4) questions assessing their current PA or SB level respectively (depending on their response to the previous question). Subsequently, participants were instructed on how to download the “MyPlan” (i.e. the English translation of “Mijn Actieplan”) app on the iOS AppStore or the Android PlayStore. Thereafter, participants were randomly assigned by a researcher (HS) to one of the eight experimental groups using block randomization (on the website www.randomization.com) [37]. The study was single-blinded: participants did not know which experimental group they were allocated to, but the researcher did [37]. After allocation, participants received an email with a link to the intervention and further instructions.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Procedure of the study

One week after each intervention session, an automatic email to encourage people to start with the following weekly session was sent. Participants who did not complete a session, were sent a reminder. If they had not completed a session after two weeks, they were phoned by the researcher (HS). When there was no reaction after 3 weeks, the participant was considered as a drop-out. Approximately one week after completing all 5 sessions, an email for post-test measurements (T1) was sent to the participants. Post-test measurements included the same questions to assess their PA or SB level (depending on what they had chosen at the pre-test measurement).

As stated in the protocol [37], a power analysis indicated a sample size of 260 participants, assuming an effect size of 0.19 [41], an alpha of 0.05 and a power of 0.80. The required sample size was calculated using GPower 3.1.9.2 [42]. Taken attrition into account, we aimed to recruit approximately 480 participants [37]. The factorial trial was approved by the Committee of Medical Ethics of the Ghent University Hospital (ID number: NCT03274271).

Intervention MyPlan 2.0

MyPlan 2.0 is an e-and m-health intervention consisting of a website and a mobile application to increase PA and reduce SB. MyPlan 2.0 is based on a previous e-health intervention MyPlan 1.0 which was found to be effective for improving health behaviour [43, 44], but had high attrition rates (78.2%) [45]. Therefore, in MyPlan 2.0 a number of changes were implemented based upon users’ needs and feedback [45, 46]. MyPlan 2.0 is based on the Health Action Process Approach model [19], and includes BCTs such as goal setting, providing information on consequences of behaviour, providing feedback on performance, social support, action planning, coping planning, self-monitoring and reviewing behaviour goals. The BCTs are defined according to Michie et al. [20], Schwarzer et al. [47], Sniehotta et al. [48] and can be found in Table 2. In this study, only the effects of action planning, coping planning and self-monitoring were experimentally investigated (Table 1) because they are well known to address post-intentional processes by bridging the intention-behaviour gap [19]. Before a participant started with the intervention, they already chose at pre-test measurement which of the two behaviours (PA or SB) they wanted to improve (i.e. goal setting). This was done to create a feeling of ‘goal-ownership’ [20, 49]. Depending on the chosen behaviour, the participant was directed to MyPlan 2.0 either targeting PA or SB. The two versions of the programme have an identical structure and offer the same BCTs, only the content was adapted to either PA or SB. Additional files 3 and 4 provide screenshots of the website and the mobile app.

Table 2 The definitions of the behaviour change techniques used in MyPlan 2.0

The website

In brief, the website consisted of 5 weekly sessions (Fig. 1). In the first session, users had to register with their email-address and create a profile. Every participant received a basic intervention to which the delivery of the BCTs action planning, coping planning and/or self-monitoring was experimentally manipulated (Table 1). The basic intervention consisted of 1) The possibility to fill in a quiz with information about the benefits of the selected target behaviour (increasing PA or decreasing SB) (i.e. providing information on consequences of behaviour), 2) Tailored feedback on the current state of their chosen behaviour (i.e. providing feedback on performance), 3) The possibility to read about how they could obtain social support from their partner, friends, family or colleagues.

With the BCT “action planning” participants were prompted to make a plan by specifying how they wanted to reach their goal, what they wanted do to, where and when they wanted to do it. For example a set of possible answers to the questions for a participant who chose PA was: 1) “How do you want to be more physically active this week?” and his/her answer could be: “I will be physically active in my leisure time”, 2) “What do you want to do to be more physically active in your leisure time?” Answer: “I will go swimming”, 3)“Where do you want to do it?” Answer: “At the swimming pool 2 blocks from my house”, 4) “When do you want to do it?” Answer: “On Wednesday and Friday evening”.

With the BCT “coping planning” participants had to identify difficult situations or barriers they anticipated to experience while changing their behaviour over the course of the intervention. Participants were then given a list of possible solutions for each barrier, from which they could choose a relevant solution for their specific situation. For example a participant who chose PA might have been asked: “What could be a barrier for you this week to be more physically active?” and the answer could be: “It’s very busy at work this week, so I will have no time”. In response a list of possible solutions adjusted to the barrier were offered (e.g. “I will block a moment in my agenda”).

With the BCT “self-monitoring” participants were prompted to monitor their behaviour. They were offered several possibilities (e.g. the app, a diary, a calendar, an activity tracker). For example a person who chose PA may have been asked: “How do you want to keep track of how physically active you are this week?” and an answer could be: “Via the app”.

At the end of the first session, the completed plan was summarised in a printable form. After one week, users received an email to start the following session. There were four follow-up sessions (session 2–5) in which all participants, as part of the basic intervention, were required to reflect on their progress of behaviour change of the past week by evaluating their PA or SB goal (i.e. reviewing behaviour goals). Furthermore, participants could see their plan from the previous week and were prompted to adapt or maintain their action plan, coping plan and/or self-monitoring method depending on the experimental group they were assigned to.

The mobile application

The application was intended to provide support to the participants on a daily basis. It consisted of different modules through which the users could freely navigate. Again, every participant received a basic intervention with 1) a module consisting out of a quiz regarding the benefits of more PA or less SB, 2) a module where they could collect medals for completing website sessions and quizzes. In the other modules, the experimental manipulated BCTs were included as a function of the experimental group they were assigned to.

The BCT “action planning” was implemented in the app by providing the users with the option to review and change their plan throughout the week. Furthermore, a message was sent to remind participants about their plan of the day. If a person planned to go swimming on Wednesday and Friday, a message was sent those days as a reminder.

The BCT “coping planning” was implemented by providing a list of barriers participants could encounter. When selecting a barrier, a list of specific solutions for this barrier appeared.

The BCT “self-monitoring” supported users to monitor their behaviour. At the end of each day, participants received a message where they could rate on a scale from 0 to 5 if they succeeded in their plan of the day. They could also assess an overview of their responses of the week which were displayed in a graph.

A more detailed description of the intervention can be found in the study protocol paper of “MyPlan 2.0” [37]. No changes regarding bug fixes, downtimes, or content changes to the MyPlan 2.0 intervention occurred after trial commencement.

Measurements

At pre-test (T0), the following sociodemographic variables were assessed: age, gender, weight, height, educational level (4 categories), occupation (7 categories) and marital status (4 categories). Weight and height were self-reported.

Primary outcomes (PA or SB) were assessed via self-report at pre-test measurement (T0) and post-test measurement (T1). If participants chose to increase their PA, the Dutch long version of the ‘International Physical Activity Questionnaire’ (IPAQ) [50] was used to measure total PA and moderate-to-vigorous-intensity PA (MVPA) in four domains (i.e. work, home, leisure time, transport), as stated in the protocol [37]. However in this study, only MVPA in minutes per week (across all domains) is the outcome for PA. MVPA is considered the most important indicator of PA and PA guidelines for adults only address activities of at least moderate to vigorous intensity [7]. The IPAQ has good reliability (ICC range from 0.46 to 0.96) and a fair-to-moderate criterion validity (ρ = 0.30–0.37). As the IPAQ overestimates PA [51], the data were truncated following the method described by Dubuy et al. [52]. If participants chose to reduce their SB, the Dutch 7-days sedentary behaviour self-report questionnaire (SIT-Q-7d) [53] was used to measure sedentary time in five domains (meals, transportation, occupation, non-occupational screen time and other sedentary time). Also, here, only sedentary time in hours per day (i.e. the sum of sedentary time spent across all domains) is the outcome for SB, as existing SB guidelines focus on the total time spent sedentary. The SIT-Q-7d shows moderate-to-good test-retest reliability (ICC = 0.77) and moderate-to-good criterion validity (ρ = 0.49) for total sitting time on an average day [54]. The data were truncated at a maximum of 16 h of sedentary time a day [55].

Statistical analysis

We initially planned to perform an analysis with the data for PA and SB combined by using a standardized PA/SB score (z-score) in order to achieve more power. In that analysis, the selected behaviour (i.e. PA or SB) would have been introduced as a moderator [37]. In later meetings with statistical experts, we decided to perform the analyses separately for PA and SB. This was done for two reasons. First, it was considered less meaningful to interpret the results of the standardized scores. For example, the difference in MVPA between pre- and posttest in minutes is more straightforward to interpret (387.42 min MVPA at pretest, 827.63 min MVPA at posttest) than the z-score (\( \mathrm{z}=\frac{{\mathrm{x}}_{\mathrm{Pre}}-{\mathrm{m}}_{\mathrm{MVPA}}}{{\mathrm{SD}}_{\mathrm{MVPA}}} \) = − 0.18 at pretest, z= \( \frac{{\mathrm{x}}_{\mathrm{Post}}-{\mathrm{m}}_{\mathrm{MVPA}}}{{\mathrm{SD}}_{\mathrm{MVPA}}} \) = 0.90 at posttest). Second, a certain increase of PA (e.g. 30 min/week) does not have the same impact on health outcomes as the same decrease in SB (e.g. 30 min/week), regardless of whether standardization is used or not [56].

Descriptive statistics are provided for the total sample and for each of the 8 experimental groups. We checked whether there were significant differences in the sociodemographic variables between participants who chose PA or SB. We also checked whether there were significant differences in the sociodemographic variables and the baseline levels of PA or SB between those who completed the study and those who dropped out (before week 5 or at post-test) using t-tests (continuous variables) or chi-square tests (nominal variables).

Linear mixed effect models with random intercepts for the participant [57] were fitted to the observed data using the lmer function from the ‘lme4-package’ [58] in R (version 3.2.5) [59] to investigate the effect of three BCTs (i.e. action planning, coping planning and self-monitoring) and their combinations on either PA or SB [57]. The p-values were obtained using the ‘lmerTest-package’. Linear mixed effect models are available case methods, that use all available information to estimate means and covariances and can therefore accommodate missing data points in repeated measures [60] provided that data are assumed to be missing at random. We initially planned to impute missing outcome data, in later meetings with statistical experts however, we chose not to do so for the following reasons. From a statistical point of view, mixed models without ad hoc imputation provide equal or more power than mixed models with ad hoc imputation [61]. Furthermore, using imputation is not advised when the proportions of data that are missing are too large (e.g. more than 40%) [62]. From a theoretical perspective, behaviour change techniques will only be effective if participants are motivated with the intervention and consequently, actually receive these techniques. In real life settings, non-motivated participants will also drop-out of the intervention. Imputing this data could lead to an underestimation of the efficacy of the behaviour change techniques.

There were separate analyses for MVPA and SB. In a first step, for each of the three BCTs (i.e. action planning, coping planning, self-monitoring) the interaction between time (T0 = 0, T1 = 1) and BCT (absent = 0, present = 1) was analysed. This interaction effect examines the efficacy of each BCT, independent from the other BCTs, and provides evidence for the overall efficacy of each of the three techniques [63]. In a second step, a model for MVPA (and SB, respectively) was fitted with all possible interactions: time (T0 = 0, T1 = 1) x action planning (absent = 0, present = 1) x coping planning (absent = 0, present = 1) x self-monitoring (absent = 0, present = 1). Based on this model, pairwise comparisons between different combinations of the BCTs were analysed. By doing so, we examined to what extent adding a BCT increased efficacy (e.g. the effect of action planning alone can result in a different outcome than for example action planning combined with coping planning). The pairwise comparisons were calculated using the linear hypothesis function of the ‘car-package’ in R [64]. These ‘follow-up’ or ‘post-hoc’ tests compare the different combinations of the techniques (represented by the 8 experimental groups). The codes for the linear hypotheses, can be found in Additional file 5. Effect sizes (with corresponding 95% confidence intervals) are expressed as the difference in change from pre-test to post-test between combinations.

In order to assess the effects of all possible combinations we had to perform multiple statistical tests. The multiple testing problem can be avoided by adjusting the cut-off score α using the Bonferroni correction to \( \frac{\upalpha}{n} \) with n being the number of tests performed [65, 66]. However, we chose not to make such adjustments for multiple comparisons because this 1) will lead to fewer errors of interpretation and 2) will not lead to missing possibly important findings [65, 66]. As such, p-values below .05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Flow of the study

The flow of participants throughout the study can be found in Fig. 2. Participants were recruited at the library of Ghent (n = 710) and through social media (n = 50). In total, 473 individuals agreed to participate, gave their informed consent and completed pre-test measurement. Of these participants, 335 chose to increase their PA and 138 chose to decrease their SB. There were no significant differences in sociodemographic variables between participants who chose PA or SB. Of those who chose PA, 166 dropped out (49,5%). Of those who chose SB, 61 dropped out (44,2%). In our study, non-usage attrition (i.e. not completing the 5 website sessions) automatically equalled drop-out attrition (i.e. not completing post-test measurement T1) since it was impossible for participants to skip certain parts of the intervention because of the linear design of the study (e.g. it was not possible to start for example with session 4 on the website if session 3 was not completed) [34, 37].

Fig. 2
figure 2

Flow of the participants throughout the study. DI = discontinued, T0 = pre-test measurement, T1 = post-test measurement

The results related to PA and SB are reported separately. The first section describes the effects on PA, the second section describes the effects on SB.

Physical activity

Participant characteristics

Characteristics of participants who chose PA are provided in Table 3. No significant difference in sociodemographic variables or MVPA at baseline was detected between those who completed the study and those who dropped out.

Table 3 Characteristics of participants of the PA intervention

Effects on physical activity as a function of behaviour change technique

In the first step, we investigated the overall efficacy of each of the three techniques. The relevant descriptive information of the interaction effects between time and each of the three BCTs are provided for MVPA. The means and 95% confidence intervals for each interaction effect are displayed in Table 4. Analyses indicated that the interaction effect between time and action planning on MVPA was not significant (t (221.88) = − 0.131, p = 0.895) (Table 4). The increase in MVPA from pre-test to post-test was significantly higher when receiving coping planning versus when not receiving coping planning (t (218.424) = 2.302, p = 0.022), and when receiving self-monitoring versus not receiving self-monitoring (t (216.63) = 2.735, p = 0.007), respectively (Table 4).

Table 4 The interaction effects between time and each of the three BCTs on MVPA. Means and 95% confidence intervals are displayed

In a second step, an additive approach to identify the effective BCTs or their combinations was used. We investigated to what extent adding a BCT, increased efficacy by using pairwise comparisons between each of the eight experimental groups. Figure 3 shows the plot of the average outcomes of MVPA at pre- and post-test for each combination of none, one, two or all techniques (represented by the 8 experimental groups). Table 3 provides the means and 95% confidence intervals for each experimental group at each of the two time points. Figure 4 provides the comparison between the different combinations of the techniques based on the average difference in MVPA from pre- to post-test.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Average MVPA at pre- and post-test for each of the eight experimental groups. Comb = combination, A = action planning, C = coping planning, S = self-monitoring

Fig. 4
figure 4

Comparison between the different combinations of the techniques (represented by each of the 8 experimental groups) based on the average difference in MVPA at pre- and post-test. Black arrow = no significant difference between the experimental groups in MVPA from pre- to post-test, p > 0.05. Green arrow = significant difference between the experimental groups in MVPA from pre- to post-test, p < 0.05

The pairwise comparisons are described following the structure of Fig. 4, starting at the top of the figure and then from the left to the right side. 1) The increase in MVPA was significantly larger for the combination A + C + S (=group 1) (x2 = 8.879, p = 0.0028) than for the group who received none of the three BCTs (=group 8). 2) No significant differences in MPVA from pre- to post-test were identified between the group who received none of the three BCTs (=group 8) and the groups who received A alone (=group 5), C alone (=group 6), or S alone (=group 7). 3) The increase in MVPA was significantly larger for the combination A + C (=group 2) (x2 = 5.95, p = 0.014) and the combination A + C + S (=group 1) (x2 =17.72, p < 0.001) than for the group who received A alone (=group 5). Furthermore, the increase in MVPA was significantly larger for the combination A + C + S (=group 1) (x2 = 4.552, p = 0.033) than for the combination A + S (=group 3). 4) The increase in MVPA was significantly larger for the combination A + C + S (=group 1) (x2 = 4.435, p = 0.035) than for the group who received C alone (=group 6). Moreover, the increase in MVPA was significantly larger for the combination A + C + S (=group 1) (x2 = 4.094, p = 0.043) than for the combination C + S (=group 4). 5) No significant differences in MVPA from pre- to post-test were identified between the group who received S alone (=group 7) and the groups receiving the combination A + S (=group 3), the combination C + S (=group 4) or the combination A + C + S (=group 1). 6) Additional analyses (not shown in Fig. 4) showed a significant decrease in MVPA for action planning alone (=group 5) compared with coping planning alone (=group 6) (x2 = 4.389p = 0.031) and self-monitoring alone (=group 7) (x2 = 8.858, p = 003), respectively. The outcomes and effect sizes for all pairwise comparisons are provided in Additional file 5.

Sedentary behaviour

Participants characteristics

Characteristics of the participants who chose SB are provided in Table 5. No significant difference in sociodemographic variables or SB at baseline was detected between those who completed the study and those who dropped out.

Table 5 Characteristics of participants of the SB intervention

Effects on sedentary behaviour as a function of behaviour change technique

In the first step, we investigated the overall efficacy of each of the three techniques. The relevant descriptive information of the interaction effects between time and each of the three BCTs are provided for SB. The means and 95% confidence intervals for each BCT are displayed in Table 6. Analyses indicated that the interaction effect between time and action planning (t (103.94) = − 0.952, p = 0.343) and the interaction effect between time and coping planning (t (104.05) = − 0.084, p = 0.933) on SB were not significant (Table 6). The decrease in SB from pre-test to post-test was significantly higher when receiving self-monitoring versus when not receiving self-monitoring (t (100.66) = − 2.573, p = 0.012) (Table 6).

Table 6 The interaction effects between time and each of the three BCTs on SB. Means and 95% confidence intervals are displayed

In a second step, an additive approach to identify the effective BCTs or their combinations was used. We investigated to what extent adding a BCT, increased efficacy by using pairwise comparisons between each of the eight experimental groups. Figure 5 shows the plot of the average outcomes of SB at pre- and post-test for each combination of none, one, two or all techniques (represented by the 8 experimental groups). Table 5 provides the means and 95% confidence intervals for each experimental group at each of the two time points. Figure 6 provides the comparison between the different combinations of the techniques based on the average difference in SB from pre- and post-test.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Average SB at pre- and post-test for each of the eight experimental groups. Comb = combination, A = action planning, C = coping planning, S = self-monitoring

Fig. 6
figure 6

Comparison between the different combinations of the techniques (represented by each of the 8 experimental groups) based on the average difference in SB at pre- and post-test. Black arrow = no significant difference between the experimental groups in SB from pre-test to post-test measurement, p > 0.05. Green arrow = significant difference between the experimental groups in SB from pre-test to post-test measurement, p < 0.05

The pairwise comparisons are described following the structure of Fig. 6, starting at the top of the figure and then from the left to the right side. 1) The decrease in SB was significantly larger for the combination A + S (=group 3) (x2 = 3.917, p = 0.048) than for the group who received no techniques (=group 8). 2) No significant differences in SB from pre- to post-test were identified between the group who received no techniques and the groups who received A alone (=group 5), C alone (=group 6) or S alone (=group 7). 3) The decrease in SB was also significantly larger for the combination A + S (=group 3) (x2 = 5.761, p = 0.016) than for the group who received A alone (=group 5). 4) No significant differences in SB from pre- to post-test were identified for C and its combinations. 5) No significant differences in SB from pre- to post-test were identified for S and its combinations. The outcomes and effect sizes for all pairwise comparisons are provided in Additional file 5.

Discussion

Behaviour change interventions are complex interventions, including a range of techniques [67]. Meta-analyses have shown that these interventions work, and also suggest that certain techniques may be more effective than others (e.g. self- monitoring) [31, 68]. However, the conclusion that certain techniques work better than others is preliminary, and await experimental corroboration [32]. Using a 2x2x2 factorial study [63], the presence of three BCTs that target post-intentional processes (i.e. action planning, coping planning, self-monitoring) was experimentally manipulated, and their effects upon PA and SB were investigated. These experimental manipulations were implemented on top of a basic intervention, which all participants received. In a first step, we examined the overall efficacy of each of three BCTs. In a second step, we used an additive approach to identify the effective BCTs or their combinations. We investigated to what extent adding BCTs increased efficacy.

The pattern of results indicates that an intervention including self-monitoring is overall more effective than an intervention without self-monitoring for both MVPA and SB. This is in line with research showing that self-monitoring is a critical technique for behaviour change [31]. It may be sufficient to change behaviour, as for example in reducing SB [68, 69]. For other behaviours, other techniques may also be needed. For example, we may expect that increasing MVPA requires planning. In this study, we found that an intervention including coping planning was overall more effective in changing MVPA. For action planning, no overall effect was found. Previous research has also found no or only moderate effects for action planning, in particular when accounting for the effects of past behaviour [70, 71]. A possible explanation is that individuals may not need to explicitly form plans, but that the intention to be physically active is sufficient to initiate previously developed behavioural schema. The efficacy of action planning could then wane as the intervention proceeds. In line with this possibility, action planning has been found to be especially efficacious for intenders (individuals that intend to, but not yet execute the behaviour) as opposed to actors (individuals that already execute the behaviour) [72, 73]. The inclusion of coping planning in an intervention did have a significant effect. This is remarkable because in a previous study, the use of coping plans has been shown to frustrate users, and many users provided coping plans of low quality [46]. At least two explanations may account for the overall effect of coping planning. First, in our intervention participants were approached as their own expert and received more guidance than in previous versions of MyPlan. Coping plans were constructed by filling out questions and several examples were provided. It thus may well be that coping planning turned out to be effective, because of the proper guidance in coping planning. Second, coping planning may have prompted action planning. Indeed, in contrast to pharmacological interventions, individuals may spontaneously make use of particular BCTs [74, 75], even when the BCTs are not delivered in the intervention.

Of further importance to this study was a detailed analysis of which BCTs or their combination were more effective. Such approach is much needed and warranted to provide guidance in developing efficient interventions [33]. Before providing the results in more detail, a cautionary note should be kept in mind. Despite our efforts to have a large number of participants in each experimental group, statistical power may be low to detect significant differences. This is particularly the case for sedentary behaviour: the number of participants in each group was in any group not larger than 21. Hence, in interpreting the results below one should not confuse ‘no evidence for an effect’ with ‘evidence for no effect’.

For MVPA, results showed that the combination of the three BCTs (action planning, coping planning and self-monitoring) were required to result in an effective intervention (i.e. larger difference from pre-test to post-test in MVPA compared to the group without these three BCTs). Increasing MVPA may thus require more than only self-monitoring. Interestingly, action planning is more effective when combined with coping planning. This is in line with previous research [76, 77]. Coping planning, however, was not more effective when combined with action planning. As mentioned above, it might be that individuals spontaneously start to plan actions when invited to develop a coping plan. A remarkable finding was that action planning was less effective than the other two techniques. Action planning even decreased PA (on average 216.73 min/week less PA after vs. before the intervention), whereas self-monitoring and coping planning each increased PA (respectively on average 123.68 and 71.27 min/week more PA after vs. before the intervention). We do not yet have a full explanation for this finding. A possible one may be found in Maher & Conroy [78], who revealed that action planning is a useful BCT for increasing PA in individuals with weak habits, but may decrease PA in individuals with strong habits. Most people in our sample were already quite physically active at the start of the study, and might thus have had strong habits with respect to PA. Explicit efforts to manipulate these habits may have backfired. These results, together with those of previous studies suggest that action planning, at least when implemented alone, should be preferably used in people with weak PA habits [78] and rather, as already mentioned above, in intenders as opposed to actors [72, 73].

For SB, the combination of action planning and self-monitoring was sufficient for an effective intervention. It may not be surprising that coping planning is of less importance for SB, as standing up does not take much time and is not difficult to achieve. Barriers that do present itself, may be difficult to overcome (e.g. cannot stand up during meeting). Previous studies have already shown that self-monitoring is important to decrease SB [68]. In the present study we found no evidence that self-monitoring alone is efficacious. However, it should be noted that the sample size in the present study was small and that this may have obscured effects. With respect to action planning, De Cocker et al. [79] found that their ‘Start to Stand’ intervention for office workers was only efficacious for those participants completing action plans. Here we showed that including both self-monitoring and action planning might be most beneficial.

Strengths and limitations

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that used an experimental 2x2x2 design to isolate the effects of three post-intentional BCTs (i.e. action planning, coping planning and self-monitoring) and their combinations to promote PA and reduce SB. Until now, other studies investigated their efficacy mostly within multiple BCT interventions or in a non-experimental way. Moreover, the BCTs were tested in a PA intervention as well as a SB intervention, which gave insight into the working mechanisms of both behaviours.

This study also has a number of limitations. First, PA and SB were measured by self-report questionnaires, which may lead to response and recall biases [80]. Future research may consider the use of devices to measure PA and SB [81, 82]. Second, as stated in the protocol paper [37] our initial power calculations were based on the analyses of PA and SB together. After consultation with statistical experts, PA and SB were analysed separately. Unfortunately, the SB intervention had less than half of the participants of the PA intervention. As such, the sample size in the SB intervention was small, probably resulting in a lack of power to detect some differences. Third, there was a high drop-out rate (49.5% in the PA intervention and 44.2% in the SB intervention), despite efforts to accommodate end-users [45, 46]. A high drop-out is often seen in e- and m-health research: Stopping the intervention is only one click away [34, 83]. Using only a subset of BCTs that have shown to be effective might decrease attrition. Therefore, it is important to experimentally identify which BCTs are effective as to reduce attrition in future interventions. Also, e- and m-health interventions are often “one-size-fits all” interventions. A more personalised intervention may increase user engagement and reduce attrition rates, and may occur at two levels. Regarding the first level: individuals might differ in terms of their motivational stages (pre-intender, intender, actor) [21], and these may change over time during the intervention. In the future, interventions should be developed in which the delivery of BCTs are dynamically tailored to these changing demands. For example, action planning may be effective in the beginning of the intervention, but not later on; once physical activity has become a habit, action planning might become ineffective, or even counterproductive [78, 84]. Regarding the second level: The delivery of the BCT itself (e.g. the support to make qualitative action and coping plans) is often generic, and not contextualized and personalised to the individual. As such, there is a need to provide a more contextualized and personalised support in this process [85]. Fourth, this study only experimentally manipulated action planning, coping planning and self-monitoring. Other techniques (e.g. providing information about the consequences of behaviour, feedback on performance, social support, reviewing behaviour goals) were also present, but not experimentally manipulated. As such, one should interpret the efficacy of our experimentally manipulated BCTs against the background of these BCTs.

Availability of data and materials

Data supporting the results reported in this article are stored at the University of Ghent, Belgium. The datasets used during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

A:

Action planning

BCT:

Behaviour change technique

C:

Coping planning

IPAQ:

International Physical Activity Questionnaire

MVPA:

Moderate to vigorous physical activity

PA:

Physical activity

PAR-Q:

Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire

SB:

Sedentary behaviour

S:

Self-monitoring

SIT-Q-7d:

Last 7-days Sedentary Behaviour Questionnaire

T0:

Pre-test

T1:

Post-test

References

  1. WHO. Global status report on noncommunicable diseases 2014: “Attaining the nine global noncommunicable diseases targets; a shared responsibility”. World Health Organisation, 2014.

  2. Haskell WL, Blair SN, Hill JO. Physical activity: health outcomes and importance for public health policy. Prev Med. 2009;49(4):280–2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Young DR, Hivert MF, Alhassan S, Camhi SM, Ferguson JF, Katzmarzyk PT, et al. Sedentary behavior and cardiovascular morbidity and mortality a science advisory from the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2016;134(13):E262–E79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Owen N, Healy GN, Matthews CE, Dunstan DW. Too much sitting: the population health science of sedentary behavior. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2010;38(3):105–13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Warburton DER, Nicol CW, Bredin SSD. Health benefits of physical activity: the evidence. Can Med Assoc J. 2006;174(6):801–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Vlaams Instituut Gezond Leven. Vlaamse gezondheidsaanbevelingen lichaamsbeweging en sedentair gedrag (lang stilzitten), i.s.m. de Vlaamse expertengroep. Vlaams Instituut Gezond leven, 2017. [https://www.gezondleven.be/themas/beweging-sedentair-gedrag].

  7. WHO. Global recommendations on physical activity for health. World Health Organization, 2010.

  8. Hallal PC, Andersen LB, Bull FC, Guthold R, Haskell W, Ekelund U, et al. Global physical activity levels: surveillance progress, pitfalls, and prospects. Lancet. 2012;380(9838):247–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Spittaels H, Van Cauwenberghe E, Verbestel V, De Meester F, Van Dyck D, Verloigne M, et al. Objectively measured sedentary time and physical activity time across the lifespan: a cross-sectional study in four age groups. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:149.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Van Dyck D, Cerin E, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Hinckson E, Reis RS, Davey R, et al. International study of objectively measured physical activity and sedentary time with body mass index and obesity: IPEN adult study. Int J Obes. 2015;39(2):199–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Eng T. The eHealth landscape: a terrain map of emerging information and communication technologies in health and health care. In: Foundation TRWJ, editor. Princeton, NJ2001.

  12. Broekhuizen K, Kroeze W, van Poppel MN, Oenema A, Brug J. A systematic review of randomized controlled trials on the effectiveness of computer-tailored physical activity and dietary behavior promotion programs: an update. Ann Behav Med. 2012;44(2):259–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Glasgow RE, McKay HG, Piette JD, Reynolds KD. The RE-AIM framework for evaluating interventions: what can it tell us about approaches to chronic illness management? Patient Educ Couns. 2001;44(2):119–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Spittaels H, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Vandelanotte C. Evaluation of a website-delivered computer-tailored intervention for increasing physical activity in the general population. Prev Med. 2007;44(3):209–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Sniehotta FF, Scholz U, Schwarzer R. Bridging the intention-behaviour gap: planning, self-efficacy, and action control in the adoption and maintenance of physical exercise. Psychol Health. 2005;20(2):143–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Schwarzer R. Modeling health behavior change: how to predict and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors. Appl Psychol. 2008;57(1):1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Sniehotta FF, Presseau J, Araujo-Soares V. Time to retire the theory of planned behaviour. Health Psychol Rev. 2014;8(1):1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Karoly P. Mechanisms of self-regulation: a systems view. Annu Rev Psychol. 1993;44(1):23–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Schwarzer R, Luszczynska A. How to overcome health-compromising behaviors - the health action process approach. Eur Psychol. 2008;13(2):141–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Michie S, Ashford S, Sniehotta FF, Dombrowski SU, Bishop A, French DP. A refined taxonomy of behaviour change techniques to help people change their physical activity and healthy eating behaviours: the CALO-RE taxonomy. Psychol Health. 2011;26(11):1479–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Schwarzer R, Lippke S, Luszczynska A. Mechanisms of health behavior change in persons with chronic illness or disability: the health action process approach (HAPA). Rehabilitation Psychology. 2011;56(3):161–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Sutton S. How does the health action process approach (HAPA) bridge the intention-behavior gap? An examination of the model's causal structure. Applied Psychology: An International Review. 2008;57(1):66–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Lippke S, Ziegelmann JP, Schwarzer R. Stage-specific adoption and maintenance of physical activity: testing a three-stage model. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2005;6(5):585–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. MacPhail M, Mullan B, Sharpe L, MacCann C, Todd J. Using the health action process approach to predict and improve health outcomes in individuals with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Diabetes, metabolic syndrome and obesity : targets and therapy. 2014;7:469–79.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Poppe L, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Verloigne M, Shadid S, Van Cauwenberg J, Compernolle S, et al. Efficacy of a self-regulation-based electronic and Mobile health intervention targeting an active lifestyle in adults having type 2 diabetes and in adults aged 50 years or older: two randomized controlled trials. J Med Internet Res. 2019;21(8):e13363.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Schwarzer R, Schuz B, Ziegelmann JP, Lippke S, Luszczynska A, Scholz U. Adoption and maintenance of four health behaviors: theory-guided longitudinal studies on dental flossing, seat belt use, dietary behavior, and physical activity. Ann Behav Med. 2007;33(2):156–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Craciun C, Schüz N, Lippke S, Schwarzer R. Facilitating sunscreen use in women by a theory-based online intervention: a randomized controlled trial. J Health Psychol. 2012;17(2):207–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Sui W, Prapavessis H. Standing up for student health: an application of the health action process approach for reducing student sedentary behavior-randomised control pilot trial. Applied psychology Health and well-being. 2018;10(1):87–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Rollo S, Prapavessis H. A combined health action process approach and mHealth intervention to increase non-sedentary Behaviours in office-working adults-a randomised controlled trial. Applied psychology Health and well-being. 2020.

  30. Poppe L, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Verloigne M, Degroote L, Shadid S, Crombez G. A Self-Regulation-Based eHealth and mHealth Intervention for an Active Lifestyle in Adults With Type 2 Diabetes: Protocol for a Randomized Controlled Trial. Jmir Research Protocols. 2019;8(3).

  31. Michie S, Abraham C, Whittington C, McAteer J, Gupta S. Effective techniques in healthy eating and physical activity interventions: a meta-regression. Health Psychol. 2009;28(6):690–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Peters GJ, de Bruin M, Crutzen R. Everything should be as simple as possible, but no simpler: towards a protocol for accumulating evidence regarding the active content of health behaviour change interventions. Health Psychol Rev. 2015;9(1):1–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Collins LM, Murphy SA, Strecher V. The multiphase optimization strategy (MOST) and the sequential multiple assignment randomized trial (SMART): new methods for more potent eHealth interventions. Am J Prev Med. 2007;32(5):S112–S8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Eysenbach G. The law of attrition. J Med Internet Res. 2005;7(1):e11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Dombrowski SU, Sniehotta FF, Avenell A, Johnston M, MacLennan G, Araújo-Soares V. Identifying active ingredients in complex behavioural interventions for obese adults with obesity-related co-morbidities or additional risk factors for co-morbidities: a systematic review. Health Psychol Rev. 2012;6(1):7–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Michie S, Jochelson K, Markham WA, Bridle C. Low-income groups and behaviour change interventions: a review of intervention content, effectiveness and theoretical frameworks. J Epidemiol Commun H. 2009;63(8):610–22.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Schroe H, Van der Mispel C, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Verloigne M, Poppe L, Crombez G. A factorial randomised controlled trial to identify efficacious self-regulation techniques in an e- and m-health intervention to target an active lifestyle: study protocol. Trials. 2019;20.

  38. Schulz KF, Altman DG, Moher D. CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMJ. 2010;340:c332.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Hoffmann TC, Glasziou PP, Boutron I, Milne R, Perera R, Moher D, et al. Better reporting of interventions: template for intervention description and replication (TIDieR) checklist and guide. BMJ : British Medical Journal. 2014;348:g1687.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Thomas S, Reading J, Shephard RJ. Revision of the physical activity readiness questionnaire (PAR-Q). Canadian journal of sport sciences. 1992.

  41. Conn VS, Hafdahl AR, Mehr DR. Interventions to increase physical activity among healthy adults: meta-analysis of outcomes. Am J Public Health. 2011;101(4):751–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175–91.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Plaete J, Crombez G, Van der Mispel C, Verloigne M, Van Stappen V, De Bourdeaudhuij I. Effect of the Web-Based Intervention MyPlan 1.0 on Self-Reported Fruit and Vegetable Intake in Adults Who Visit General Practice: A Quasi-Experimental Trial. J Med Internet Res. 2016;18(2):e47.

  44. Degroote L, Plaete J, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Verloigne M, Van Stappen V, De Meester A, et al. The Effect of the eHealth Intervention 'MyPlan 1.0′ on Physical Activity in Adults Who Visit General Practice: A Quasi-Experimental Trial. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(2).

  45. Van der Mispel C, Poppe L, Crombez G, Verloigne M, De Bourdeaudhuij I. A self-regulation-based eHealth intervention to promote a healthy lifestyle: investigating user and website characteristics related to attrition. J Med Internet Res. 2017;19(7):e241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Poppe L, Crombez G, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Van der Mispel C, Shadid S, Verloigne M. Experiences and Opinions of Adults with Type 2 Diabetes Regarding a Self-Regulation-Based eHealth Intervention Targeting Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviour. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(5).

  47. Schwarzer R, Sniehotta F, Lippke S, Luszczynska A, Scholz U, Schüz B. On the assessment and analysis of variables in the health action process approach: conducting an investigation. Freie Universeitat Berlin. 2003.

  48. Sniehotta FF, Schwarzer R, Scholz U, Schuz B. Action planning and coping planning for long-term lifestyle change: theory and assessment. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2005;35(4):565–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Locke EA, Latham GP. Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task motivation. A 35-year odyssey. Am Psychol. 2002;57(9):705–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Vandelanotte C, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Philippaerts R, Sjöström M, Sallis J. Reliability and validity of a computerized and Dutch version of the international physical activity questionnaire (IPAQ). J Phys Act Health. 2005;2(1):63–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Bermúdez VJ, Rojas JJ, Córdova EB, Añez R, Toledo A, Aguirre MA, et al. International physical activity questionnaire overestimation is ameliorated by individual analysis of the scores. Am J Ther. 2013;20(4):448–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Dubuy V, De Cocker KA, De Bourdeaudhuij IM, Maes L, Metsu P, Van Acker R, et al. ‘10 000 steps Flanders’: evaluation of the state-wide dissemination of a physical activity intervention in Flanders. Health Educ Res. 2013;28(3):546–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Wijndaele K. I DEB, Godino JG, lynch BM, griffin SJ, Westgate K, et al. reliability and validity of a domain-specific last 7-d sedentary time questionnaire. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2014;46(6):1248–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Busschaert C, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Van Holle V, Chastin SF, Cardon G, De Cocker K. Reliability and validity of three questionnaires measuring context-specific sedentary behaviour and associated correlates in adolescents, adults and older adults. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12:117.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Lakerveld J, Mackenbach J, Horvath E, Rutters F, Compernolle S. Bárdos H, et al. The relation between sleep duration and sedentary behaviours in European adults obesity reviews. 2016;17:62–7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. van der Ploeg HP, Hillsdon M. Is sedentary behaviour just physical inactivity by another name? Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2017;14(1):142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Verbeke G, Molenberghs G. Linear mixed models for longitudinal data. New York: Springer - Verlag; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S, Christensen R, Singmann H, et al. Linear Mixed-Effects Models using 'Eigen' and S4. R package version 1. 2014:1–7.

  59. Team RDC. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Maas CJM, Snijders TAB. The multilevel approach to repeated measures for complete and incomplete data. Qual Quant. 2003;37(1):71–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Chakraborty H, Gu H. A Mixed Model Approach for Intent-to-Treat Analysis in Longitudinal Clinical Trials with Missing Values. RTI Press Methods Report Series. Research Triangle Park (NC): RTI Press © 2009 Research Triangle Institute. All rights reserved.; 2009.

  62. Jakobsen JC, Gluud C, Wetterslev J, Winkel P. When and how should multiple imputation be used for handling missing data in randomised clinical trials – a practical guide with flowcharts. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2017;17(1):162.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Collins LM, Dziak JJ, Kugler KC, Trail JB. Factorial experiments: efficient tools for evaluation of intervention components. Am J Prev Med. 2014;47(4):498–504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Fox J, Weisberg S. An {R} companion to applied regression. Thousand Oaks CA: SAGE publications; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Rothman KJ. No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology. 1990;1(1):43–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Perneger TV. What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. BMJ. 1998;316(7139):1236–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Craig P, Dieppe P, Macintyre S, Michie S, Nazareth I, Petticrew M. Developing and evaluating complex interventions: the new Medical Research Council guidance. BMJ. 2008;337:a1655.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Compernolle S, DeSmet A, Poppe L, Crombez G, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Cardon G, et al. Effectiveness of interventions using self-monitoring to reduce sedentary behavior in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phy. 2019;16(1):63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Gardner B, Smith L, Lorencatto F, Hamer M, Biddle SJ. How to reduce sitting time? A review of behaviour change strategies used in sedentary behaviour reduction interventions among adults. Health Psychol Rev. 2016;10(1):89–112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Barg CJ, Latimer AE, Pomery EA, Rivers SE, Rench TA, Prapavessis H, et al. Examining predictors of physical activity among inactive middle-aged women: an application of the health action process approach. Psychol Health. 2012;27(7):829–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Ernsting A, Gellert P, Schneider M, Lippke S. A mediator model to predict workplace influenza vaccination behaviour – an application of the health action process approach. Psychol Health. 2013;28(5):579–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Wiedemann AU, Lippke S, Reuter T, Ziegelmann JP, Schüz B. The more the better? The number of plans predicts health behaviour change. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being. 2011;3(1):87–106.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Reinwand DA, Crutzen R, Storm V, Wienert J, Kuhlmann T, de Vries H, et al. Generating and predicting high quality action plans to facilitate physical activity and fruit and vegetable consumption: results from an experimental arm of a randomised controlled trial. BMC Public Health. 2016;16.

  74. Cane J, O’Connor D, Michie S. Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implement Sci. 2012;7(1):37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Michie S, Johnston M, Abraham C, Lawton R, Parker D, Walker A, et al. Making psychological theory useful for implementing evidence based practice: a consensus approach. Qual Saf Health Care. 2005;14(1):26–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Sniehotta FF, Scholz U, Schwarzer R. Action plans and coping plans for physical exercise: a longitudinal intervention study in cardiac rehabilitation. Br J Health Psychol. 2006;11(Pt 1):23–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Kwasnicka D, Presseau J, White M, Sniehotta FF. Does planning how to cope with anticipated barriers facilitate health-related behaviour change? A systematic review. Health Psychol Rev. 2013;7(2):129–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Maher JP, Conroy DE. Habit strength moderates the effects of daily action planning prompts on physical activity but not sedentary behavior. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2015;37(1):97–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. De Cocker K, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Cardon G, Vandelanotte C. What are the working mechanisms of a web-based workplace sitting intervention targeting psychosocial factors and action planning? BMC Public Health. 2017;17(1):382.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Sallis JF, Saelens BE. Assessment of physical activity by self-report: status, limitations, and future directions. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2000;71(2 Suppl):S1–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Degroote L, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Verloigne M, Poppe L, Crombez G. The Accuracy of Smart Devices for Measuring Physical Activity in Daily Life: Validation Study. Jmir Mhealth and Uhealth. 2018;6(12).

  82. Degroote L, Hamerlinck G, Poels K, Maher C, Crombez G, De Bourdeaudhuij I, et al. Low-cost consumer-based trackers to measure physical activity and sleep duration among adults in free-living conditions: a validation study. JMIR mHealth and uHealth. 2020.

  83. Geraghty AWA, Torres LD, Leykin Y, Pérez-Stable EJ, Muñoz RF. Understanding attrition from international internet health interventions: a step towards global eHealth. Health Promot Int. 2012;28(3):442–52.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Ziegelmann JP, Lippke S, Schwarzer R. Adoption and maintenance of physical activity: planning interventions in young, middle-aged, and older adults. Psychol Health. 2006;21(2):145–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Michie S, Thomas J, Johnston M, Mac Aonghusa P, Shawe-Taylor J, Kelly MP, et al. The human behaviour-change project: harnessing the power of artificial intelligence and machine learning for evidence synthesis and interpretation. Implement Sci. 2017;12.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Prof. Dr. Armand De Clercq, PhD Louise Poppe and Celien Van der Mispel for their support in developing MyPlan 2.0.

Funding

The research of author HS is funded by the Special Research Fund of Ghent University (BOF). The research of author LP and WWL is funded by Research Foundation Flanders (FWO). The research of author ADP is funded by both BOF and FWO. The funding body is not involved in the study design, collection, management, analysis and interpretation of data or in writing of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors HS, LP, MV, GC and IDB were involved in designing the study. The author HS drafted the manuscript. DVD, ADP, LP, MV, IDB and GC critically revised the manuscript. The authors HS, ADP, WWL and TL analyzed and interpret the data. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Helene Schroé.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The research protocol of this study was approved by the Committee of Medical Ethics of the Ghent University Hospital (Belgian registration number: B670201731996) and registered as a clinical trial at https://register.clinicaltrials.gov (ID number: NCT03274271). Participants provided web-based informed consent regarding the study measures and log data of the website and app before enrolling in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1.

Completed CONSORT 2010 checklist.

Additional file 2.

Completed TIDieR checklist.

Additional file 3.

Screenshots of the website.

Additional file 4.

Screenshots of the mobile application.

Additional file 5.

The codes of the linear hypotheses and the outcomes with effect sizes for all pairwise comparisons for MVPA and SB.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schroé, H., Van Dyck, D., De Paepe, A. et al. Which behaviour change techniques are effective to promote physical activity and reduce sedentary behaviour in adults: a factorial randomized trial of an e- and m-health intervention. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 17, 127 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12966-020-01001-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12966-020-01001-x

Keywords