Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Tea consumption and measures of attention and psychomotor speed in the very old: the Newcastle 85+ longitudinal study

A Correction to this article was published on 08 February 2021

This article has been updated

Abstract

Background

A number of studies have indicated a beneficial effect of tea consumption on the reduction of risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in older aged populations. However, there is a paucity of data on these associations in the very old, defined as individuals aged 85 years and over. We investigated the relationship between tea consumption in the very old and measures of global cognitive function, memory, attention and psychomotor speed.

Method

Longitudinal (5-years), population-based cohort study of individuals aged 85+ years in the North East of England, United Kingdom. Participants were community-dwelling and institutionalized men and women recruited through general medical practices (n = 676). Baseline tea consumption and longitudinal measures of global and domain specific (memory, speed and attention) cognitive function were assessed. Linear mixed models, controlling for demographic (e.g. age, sex and education) and health variables were used to determine whether tea consumption was protective against cognitive decline.

Results

Tea consumption was not associated with cognitive function at baseline on any measure (unadjusted and adjusted analyses). In the linear mixed effects models adjusted for age, sex, education and disease co-morbidity, higher tea consumption was associated with significantly better attention (focused and sustained attention), and psychomotor speed (complex tasks only) over five-years follow-up. However, there was no association between tea consumption and global cognitive function, memory or performance on simple speed tasks over time.

Conclusions

In this cohort study of non-demented very old adults we found that higher (vs. lower) tea consumption was associated with better performance over time on measures of focused and sustained attention and some psychomotor speed tasks. No associations with global cognition, memory or easy speed tasks (simple Reaction Time or Word Recognition) were detected. The results have implications for the development of possible diet-based interventions focused on improving cognitive function in the very old age group. These findings need to be confirmed in a sufficiently powered and well-designed RCT with non-demented very old adults.

Peer Review reports

Background

The world age demographic is changing rapidly [1]. The World Health Organization (WHO) reports that between 2000 and 2050, the proportion of individuals aged 60 years and over will double from approximately 11 to 22% [2]. The greatest increase will be in persons aged 80 years and older, such that by 2050 the world will have almost 400 million people in this age group [2]. Despite dramatic success in improving human survival, longer life expectancy does not necessarily mean a healthier life [3]. The change in population age structure has been associated with large increases in the prevalence and incidence of age related conditions including cognitive impairment and dementia [4]. In 2013, it was estimated that there were over 46 million people with dementia worldwide and this number is predicted to rise to 131.5 million by 2050 [5]. Rates for severe cognitive impairment (no dementia) are also high and increasing [5]. The public health significance of these trends is dramatic and will impose considerable pressure on individuals, their families and society. The identification of effective strategies to maintain cognitive health with ageing is a research priority to ensure that the ageing transition is successful worldwide.

Several studies have demonstrated that modifiable risk factors such as diet can play a role in maintaining cognitive function and preventing decline in older aged populations [6,7,8]. Tea, the hot water infusion from the leaves of the plant Camellia sinensis, is the most consumed beverage in the world after water. Black tea is the most commonly consumed tea and accounts for 76–78% of world tea consumption [9]. The UK is ranked 3rd in the world for tea consumption per capita after Turkey and Ireland [10]. Tea has been associated with several positive psychosomatic states such as clarity of mind, alertness, enhanced attention and relaxation [11]. To date, no research has been undertaken on the association between tea consumption and cognitive function in the very old population; defined as those aged 85 years and over. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine the association between tea consumption and cognitive function including measures of global and domain specific (i.e. memory, speed and attention) cognitive performance using data from the Newcastle 85+ longitudinal study. Knowing if tea is protective against cognitive decline in this age group will have implications for the development of intervention strategies focused on improving health and cognitive function.

Methods

Participant cohort

The Newcastle 85+ study is a community based longitudinal study on multi-dimensional health and ageing aspects in persons aged 85 years and over in 2006 (born 1921). Participants with end-stage illness; or perceived to pose a risk to the research nurse visiting alone; or were diagnosed with dementia; or without tea consumption or SMMSE/CDR data were excluded from the study. Based on health assessments and General Practice (GP) clinical records, the cohort was broadly representative of the general UK population. Full recruitment, screening, baseline and follow-up repeat health assessment processes and measurements of core variables are described elsewhere [12, 13]. Baseline and follow-up interviews at 18 (1.5 years; wave 2), 36 (3 years; wave 3), and 60 months (5 years; wave 4) were completed by research nurses at the participants’ usual places of residence. Figure 1 indicates the participants’ flow chart for the study.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of the Newcastle 85+ study participants by SMMSE and CDR scores and dietary intake availability over the study period

Dietary assessment

Tea intake was assessed at baseline by a 24 h Multiple Pass Recall (24 h-MPR) on two non-consecutive occasions administered by trained research nurses. Total daily tea intake was calculated by averaging intakes of both 24 h-MPR. Portion sizes were estimated using the “Photographic Atlas of Food Portion Sizes” [14]. Black tea with or without milk was by far the most widely consumed type of tea (n = 738) therefore green (n = 3), herbal (n = 14) and lemon tea (n = 3) (instant powder) were excluded. The tea consumption variable was created by ranking tea intake into tertiles where one cup of tea was assumed to be 200 ml. Two groups were created: high tea consumption (tertile 3. Range 4.56 to 11.85 cups per day; n = 463) vs. moderate/low tea consumption (tertiles 1 and 2. Range 0.25 to 4.55 cups per day; n = 213).

Dementia

Diagnosis of dementia was taken from the GP record review.

Neuropsychological battery

Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) was used to assess global cognitive function at baseline, 36 and 60 months [15, 16]. Scores range from 0 to 30, with higher scores indicating better global cognitive performance. Domain specific cognitive performance, including speed, attention and episodic memory were assessed using the Cognitive Drug Research (CDR) computerised system [17,18,19,20]. The CDR battery of assessments included scores for: Word Recognition and Recall (WR), Simple Reaction Time (SRT), Digit Vigilance Task (DVT), and Choice Reaction Time (CRT). Five composite measures were derived from the combination of the above scores: Power of Attention (PoA; a measure of focused and time dependent attention), Continuity of Attention (CoA; a combination of and measurement of accuracy of scores from CRT and DVT), response time variability (RTV; a measure of fluxes in attention) and the WR accuracy sensitivity index (SI – an assessment of memory). Lower scores for speed and attention (except CoA) indicate better performance whilst higher scores for SI indicate better performance. The assessment battery took approximately 15 min to complete.

Covariates

Baseline covariates included in the analysis were: age, sex, years of education and disease co-morbidity (calculated as the sum of the presence or absence of a history of cardiac disease, cerebrovascular disease or diabetes; with scores coded as 0, 1, or 2–3).

Analysis

Differences in baseline demographic, lifestyle, health and cognitive variables between the high and low tea consumption groups were compared using the χ2 statistic for categorical variables or independent t-test for normally distributed continuous variables (or Wilcoxon Rank sum (Mann-Whitney) test for non-normally distributed variables). We fitted linear mixed effects models to the cognitive measures collected over 3 indicated time points including 18 (1.5 years), 36 (3 years), and 60 months (5 years). Due to the number of time points available for analysis, we estimated models with constant rate of change over time. (i.e. no quadratic term for time was fitted). The intercept was adjusted for baseline age (centred at age 85), sex (0 = male, 1 = female), years of full-time education (centred at 7 years, the average number of years of education in the sample), disease count. Rate of change was adjusted for baseline age, sex, and education. Results are presented as coefficients with 95% confidence intervals (95% CI). An unstructured correlation matrix was considered, which permitted the estimation of residual covariances. Models were estimated using maximum likelihood estimation and estimates were robust under a missing at random assumption. Statistical significance was set a p < 0.05. All analyses were completed using Stata Version 14.

Results

Demographics

Of the 845 participants who completed the health assessment and GP record review, 738 (87.3%) had information on tea consumption. Of the 738 participants, 676 had no dementia at baseline and form the analytical sample. Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of the sample by tea status (high vs. low consumption). As shown, there were no significant differences in baseline age, years of education or health status (all conditions and total co-morbidity score) between the low and high tea consumption groups. There was however a gender difference, with a greater number of females in the high (63.9%) compared to the low (54.9%) tea consumption group (p = 0.026).

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of the study sample (n = 676) by tea consumption (high vs. low)

Baseline cognitive function

There were no significance differences in baseline global or domain specific test scores between the low and high tea consumption groups in the whole sample (shown in Table 1) or in gender stratified analysis (results not shown).

Longitudinal cognitive performance

Table 2 shows the pattern of associations between tea consumption and cognitive performance on each measure over time when controlling for covariates (for the full model results see Supplementary Appendix 1). Compared to the low tea consumption group people in the high tea consumption group displayed significantly better cognitive function over five-years follow-up on measures of attention (PoA and CoA) and speed (i.e. complex tasks only including choice and digit vigilance RT). The low tea group also had lower performance over time on the response time variability score (i.e. a measure of fluctuations in attention), but this failed to reach statistical significance (p = 0.069). Tea consumption was not associated with measures of global cognitive function (i.e. MMSE score), memory or speed on simple tasks (i.e. simple RT or word recognition RT) over 5 years follow-up.

Table 2 Baseline cognitive test (median) scores by tea group

Discussion

In this study, tea consumption was associated with some (e.g. attention and complex speed tasks), but not all (i.e. global, memory and simple speed tasks) cognitive domains over time. The results support previous work of a link between tea consumption and cognition [21,22,23,24], extending the findings for the first time to the very-old population and highlight a role of black tea in cognitive performance. The results have important implications for informing the development of possible prevention and risk reduction approaches, of which nutritional interventions are key modifiable factors for age associated cognitive decline or dementia.

Previous studies linking tea consumption to cognitive performance have primarily focused on green tea. The findings have mostly been positive with higher consumption associated with protective effects against global cognitive decline in normal and probable-AD cases and a lower prevalence of cognitive impairment (i.e. MMSE score or comprehensive Global Geriatric Assessment scores [21, 22]. The association has been linked to, inter alia, amount of polyphenol intake and associated lowering of plasma homocysteine, a risk factor for probable dementia. In contrast, a study of community-living Chinese adults in Singapore aged ≥55 years found that regular tea consumption, particularly black and oolong tea (4–7 cups (215 mL/day), was associated with lower risk of cognitive impairment and decline on the MMSE [23]. This was supported by a RCT study of the effects of consumption of black tea and other beverages (400 mL per subject) on cognitive and psychomotor performance which indicated that in the experimental arm where participants drank 400 mL of black tea/day there was a strong association with rapid increases in alertness and information processing capacity [24].

The putative effects of tea consumption on psychological states such as mood and cognitive function are attributed to their phytochemical constituents. Examples of such compounds include, inter alia, the methylxanthine, caffeine which as a central nervous system stimulant has been shown to increase alertness and concentration, notably through its antagonistic effects on adenosine A1 and A2A receptors [25]; the amino acid L-theanine which has been shown to have positive effects on mood such as increased calmness and cognitive performance, perhaps through modulation od GABA receptors [26] and various polyphenols with varied positive effects on brain function [11, 27,28,29,30]. Studies by Scholey et al. [31] and Okello et al. [32] demonstrated that supplementation with 300 mg of the tea constituent Epigallocatechin Gallate (EGCG) or consumption of either black or green tea increase the Electroencephalogram (EEG) spectral powers of Theta, Alpha and Beta brain wave activities, a potential indication of their putative anxiolytic, attention and concentration effects.

Lifestyle, socio-demographic and health cofounders have a bearing on cognitive function. Although significant differences were observed between the low and high tea consumers in terms of smoking and alcohol use, these differences had no significant effects on the baseline cognitive tests (Tables 1 & 2). Other notable health aspects purported to be associated with cognitive functions are deficiencies in vitamins such as vitamins D and B12. Previous studies based on our 85+ cohort showed no effect of Vitamin D on global cognition over a 3-year observation period although small season-specific incidents of impairment on attention specific tasks were observed [33]. A larger cohort and longer term (12- years) study concluded that there was ‘no association between vitamin D status and long-term risk of dementia or cognitive impairment’ [34]. Another study based on our 85+ cohort showed that neither folate nor plasma vitamin B12 were predictive for cognitive decline over the 5-year study period [35].

Conclusion

In the very old (85+ years) and non-demented we found that higher (vs. lower) tea consumption was not associated with baseline cognitive performance on any measure. However, higher (vs. lower) tea consumption was associated with better performance over time (5-years) on measures of attention (focused and sustained but not fluctuations – only a trend with these measures) and some speed measures (choice and digit vigilance RT). No association with global cognition, memory or easy speed tasks (simple RT or word recognition RT). These results add to the growing body of research showing that teas derived from the plant Camellia sinensis, irrespective of their manufacturing processing and demographic consumption habits have a beneficial effect on cognitive function dependent on frequency of consumption.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of the study are large sample size, specific focus on the very old population, and an extensive collection of multidimensional health variables, including longitudinal cognitive follow-up. Limitations include dietary assessments which are based on at baseline on two non-consecutive occasions (e.g. we could not detect if tea consumption declined with time). However tea consumption patterns were stable throughout the week in this population.

Availability of data and materials

By application to the Data Manager, Newcastle 85+ study Data Guardians Group. http://research.ncl.ac.uk/85plus/

Change history

Abbreviations

CDR:

Cognitive Drug Research

CoA:

Continuity of Attention

CRT:

Choice Reaction Time

DVT:

Digit Vigilance Task

EEG:

Electroencephalogram

EGCG:

Epigallocatechin Gallate

GP:

General Practice

MMSE:

Mini-Mental State Examination

MPR:

Multiple Pass Recall

PoA:

Power of Attention

PVD:

Peripheral Vascular Disease

RCT:

Randomised Controlled Trial

RT:

Reaction Time

RTV:

Response Time Variability

SI:

Sensitivity Index

SRT:

Simple Reaction Time

WHO:

World Health Organization

WR:

Word Recognition

References

  1. Christensen K, Doblhammer G, Rau R, Vaupel JW. Ageing populations: the challenges ahead. Lancet. 2009;374(9696):1196–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. World Health Organization (WHO). Ageing and life course. 2014; http://www.who.int/ageing/about/facts/en/.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Jagger C, Matthews FE, Wohland P, et al. A comparison of health expectancies over two decades in England: results of the cognitive function and ageing study i and ii. Lancet (London, England). 2016;387(10020):779–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimer's Dementia. 2013;9(1):63–75.e62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Alzheimer's Disease International. The World Alzheimer Report. The global impact of dementia: An analysis of prevalence, incidence, cost and trends. 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Barberger-Gateau P. Nutrition and brain aging: how can we move ahead? Eur J Clin Nutr. 2014;68(11):1245–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Volkert D, Chourdakis M, Faxen-Irving G, Frühwald T, Landi F, Suominen MH, Vandewoude M, Wirth R, Schneider SM. ESPEN guidelines on nutrition in dementia. Clin Nutr. 2015;34(6):1052–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Durga J, van Boxtel MP, Schouten EG, Kok FJ, Jolles J, Katan MB, Verhoef P. Effect of 3-year folic acid supplementation on cognitive function in older adults in the FACIT trial: arandomised, double blind, controlled trial. Lancet. 2007;369(9557):208–16.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Graham HN. Green tea composition, consumption, and polyphenol chemistry. Prev Med. 1992;21(3):334–50.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. QUARTZ. (https://qz.com/168690/where-the-worlds-biggest-tea-drinkers-are/20 January 2014. Retrieved 21st April 2017).

  11. Bryan J. Psychological effects of dietary components of tea: caffeine and L-theanine. Nutr Rev. 2008;66:82–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Collerton J, Davies K, Jagger C, et al. Health and disease in 85 year olds: baseline findings from the Newcastle 85+ cohort study. BMJ. 2009;399:b4904.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Davies K, Collerton JC, Jagger C, et al. Engaging the oldest old in research: lessons from the Newcastle 85+ study. BMC Geriatr. 2010;10:64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Mendonça N, Hill T, Granic A, Davies K, Collerton J, Mathers J, Adamson A. Micronutrient intake and food sources in the very old: analysis of the Newcastle 85 study. Br J Nutr. 2016;116(4):751–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Molloy DW, Standish TI. A guide to the standardized mini-mental state examination. Int Psychogeriatr / IPA. 1997;9(Suppl 1):87–94 discussion 143-150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Simpson PM, Surmon DJ, Wesnes KA, Wilcock GK. The cognitive drug research computerized assessment system for demented patients: a validation study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1991;6(2):95–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Wesnes KA. Assessing change in cognitive function in dementia: the relative utilities of the Alzheimer's disease assessment scale-cognitive subscale and the cognitive drug research system. Neurodegener Dis. 2008;5(3–4):261–3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ballard CG, Aarsland D, McKeith I, et al. Fluctuations in attention: PD dementia vs DLB with parkinsonism. Neurology. 2002;59(11):1714–20.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Wesnes KA, McKeith I, Edgar C, Emre M, Lane R. Benefits of rivastigmine on attention in dementia associated with Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2005;65(10):1654–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Hasegawa T, Okello E, Yamada T. Protective effect of Japanese green tea against cognitive impairment in the elderly, a two-year follow-up observation. Alzheimer’s Dementia. 2005;(1):S100. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jalz.2005.06.346.

  22. Kuriyama S, Hozawa A, Ohmori K, et al. Green tea and cognitive function- a cross sectional study from the Tsurugaya project. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83:355–61.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Ng TP, Feng L, Niti M, Kua EH, Yap KB. Tea consumption and cognitive inmpairement and decline in older Chineese adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;88:224–31.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Hindmarch I, Quinlan PT, Moore KL, Parkin C. The effects of black tea and other beverages on aspects of cognition and psychomotor performance. Psychopharmacology. 1998;139:230–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Clark I, Landolt HP. Coffee, caffeine, and sleep: a systematic review of epidemiological studies and randomized controlled trials. Sleep Med Rev. 2017;31:70–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Nathan PJ, Lu K, Gray M, Oliver C. The neuropharmacology of L-theanine(N-ethyl-L-glutamine): a possible neuroprotective and cognitive enhancing agent. J Herb Pharmacother. 2006;6(2):21–30.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Einöther SJ, Martens VE. Acute effects of tea consumption on attention and mood. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;98(suppl):1700S–8S.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Nobre AC, Rao A, Owen GN. L-theanine, a natural constituent in tea, and its effect on mental state. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 2008;17(Suppl 1):167–8.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Camfield DA, Stough C, Farrimond J, Scholey AB. Acute effects of tea constituents L-theanine, caffeine, and epigallocatechin gallate on cognitive function and mood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr Rev. 2014;72(8):507–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Dietz C, Dekker M. Effect of green tea phytochemicals on mood and cognition. Curr Pharm Des. 2017;23(19):2876–905.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Scholey A, Downey LA, Ciorciari J, Pipingas A, Nolidin K, Finn M, Wines M, Catchlove S, Terrens A, Barlow E, Gordon L, Stough C. Acute neurocognitive effects of epigallocatechin gallate (EGCG). Appetite. 2012;58(2):767–70.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Okello EJ, Abadi AM, Abadi SA. Effects of green and black tea consumption on brain wave activities in healthy volunteers as measured by a simplified electroencephalogram (EEG): a feasibility study. Nutri Neurosci. 2016;19(5):196–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Granic A, Hill TR, Kirkwood TB, et al. Serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D and cognitive decline in the very old: the Newcastle 85+ study. Eur J Neurol. 2015;22(1):106–e7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Olsson E, Byberg L, Karlström B, et al. Vitamin D is not associated with incident dementia or cognitive impairment: an 18-y follow-up study in community-living old men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;105(4):936–43.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. Mendonça N, Granic A, Mathers JC, et al. One-carbon metabolism biomarkers and cognitive decline in the very old: The Newcastle 85+ study. J Am Med Dir Assoc. 2017;18(9):806.e19–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the operational support of National Health Service North of Tyne and of the local general practitioners and their staff - research nurses, laboratory technicians, data management and clerical as well as many colleagues for their expert advice. Thanks are due especially to the study participants and their families without whom the study would not have taken place.

This paper is dedicated to the memory of our recently deceased colleague - Professor Keith Wesnes, a wonderful colleague and an accomplished scientist whose CDR system is of great benefit to scientific research around the world.

Funding

The Newcastle 85+ Study was funded by The Medical Research Council, Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council. Dunhill Medical Trust. Parts of the work were also funded by the British Heart Foundation; Unilever Corporate Research, Newcastle University; and Newcastle Primary Care Trust.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

EJO formulated the hypothesis based on the 85+ tea intake data, reviewed the statistical analyses results and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. NM, G M-T, MS and BS conducted the data analysis. All authors participated in the data interpretation, preparation and approval of the final manuscript. KW was the original designer and proprietor of the CDR Battery of cognitive tests provided on a non-commercial basis during the study. All authors were affiliated to Newcastle University during the preparation of the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Edward Jonathan Okello.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The Newcastle and North Tyneside 1 Research Ethics Committee approved the study (Ref:06/Q0905/2). Informed written consent was obtained from all participants or their consultee (usually a relative or carer) where capacity to consent was assessed as lacking.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Okello, E.J., Mendonça, N., Stephan, B. et al. Tea consumption and measures of attention and psychomotor speed in the very old: the Newcastle 85+ longitudinal study. BMC Nutr 6, 57 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40795-020-00361-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s40795-020-00361-8

Keywords