Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Personality Traits as Risk Factors for Treatment-Resistant Depression

  • Michio Takahashi,

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Teikyo University Chiba Medical Center, Ichihara, Japan, Division of Clinical Neuroscience, Chiba University Center for Forensic Mental Health, Chiba, Japan

  • Yukihiko Shirayama ,

    shirayama@rapid.ocn.ne.jp

    Affiliations Department of Psychiatry, Teikyo University Chiba Medical Center, Ichihara, Japan, Division of Clinical Neuroscience, Chiba University Center for Forensic Mental Health, Chiba, Japan

  • Katsumasa Muneoka,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Teikyo University Chiba Medical Center, Ichihara, Japan

  • Masatoshi Suzuki,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Teikyo University Chiba Medical Center, Ichihara, Japan

  • Koichi Sato,

    Affiliation Department of Psychiatry, Teikyo University Chiba Medical Center, Ichihara, Japan

  • Kenji Hashimoto

    Affiliation Division of Clinical Neuroscience, Chiba University Center for Forensic Mental Health, Chiba, Japan

Abstract

Background

The clinical outcome of antidepressant treatment in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) is thought to be associated with personality traits. A number of studies suggest that depressed patients show high harm avoidance, low self-directedness and cooperativeness, as measured on the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI). However, the psychology of these patients is not well documented.

Methods

Psychological evaluation using Cloninger’s TCI, was performed on treatment-resistant MDD patients (n = 35), remission MDD patients (n = 31), and age- and gender-matched healthy controls (n = 174).

Results

Treatment-resistant patients demonstrated high scores for harm avoidance, and low scores for reward dependence, self-directedness, and cooperativeness using the TCI, compared with healthy controls and remission patients. Interestingly, patients in remission continued to show significantly high scores for harm avoidance, but not other traits in the TCI compared with controls. Moreover, there was a significant negative correlation between reward dependence and harm avoidance in the treatment-resistant depression cohort, which was absent in the control and remitted depression groups.

Conclusions

This study suggests that low reward dependence and to a lesser extent, low cooperativeness in the TCI may be risk factors for treatment-resistant depression.

Introduction

Antidepressants are commonly used in the treatment of major depressive disorder (MDD). Between 60 and 70 percent of depressed patients respond to treatment with the first prescribed antidepressant at maximal doses for at least 2 months, and 80 to 90 percent of these patients respond to the first or second choice prescribed antidepressant. The 5 to 15 percent of patients who do not respond to treatment are deemed to have treatment-resistant depression [1]. It is noteworthy that response is defined as a reduction to less than 50 percent in depressive symptoms, but not necessarily recovery. Remission is defined as a full recovery, classified as a score of less than 7 on the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D) [1]. In order to study the psychopathological aspects of treatment-resistant depression, it is necessary to extract the core features of treatment-resistant depression.

Cloninger and his colleagues developed a dimensional psychosocial model of personality. This Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) defines four dimensions of temperament: novelty seeking, harm avoidance, reward dependence, and persistence, and three dimensions of character: self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence [2]. Personality is considered to affect the outcome of mood disorder. It is well established that depressed patients showed high scores of harm avoidance and that the severity of depression correlates positively with harm avoidance scores on the TCI [3][13]. Furthermore, scores in the harm avoidance section are altered by depression and antidepressant treatment [4], [7], [8], [14]. In a meta-analysis of MDD study data, harm avoidance scores showed a clear negative change from baseline to endpoint [15]. Similarly, it is known that depressed patients showed low scores of self-directedness and that the severity of depression correlates negatively with self-directedness scores on the TCI [5][13], [16].

Favorable outcomes after antidepressant treatment are associated with personality score changes. Depressive patients with low harm avoidance scores on the TCI tend to have good outcomes [17], [18]. Typical TCI scores indicating a favorable outcome, show decreases in harm avoidance and self-transcendence and increases in self-directedness and cooperativeness, from baseline to post-treatment values [19]. Furthermore, the harm avoidance rate in treatment-resistant patients is significantly higher than that of the treatment-response group [18], [20]. However, no significant personality changes were observed in patients with poor outcome after antidepressant treatment [19]. Both non-responders and responders showed increased harm avoidance scores, and decreased self-directedness and cooperativeness scores on the TCI [7]. However, it remains unclear which components of personality influence treatment-resistance in MDD patients.

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the psychological features of treatment-resistant patients with MDD. Here, we investigated the possibility of personality biases in treatment-resistant patients with MDD, using Cloninger’s seven-factor model, TCI [2].

Methods

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the ethics committee of Teikyo University Chiba Medical Center (study number 09–30) and performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after the procedures had been fully explained.

Participants

A total of 174 healthy subjects, 35 antidepressant treatment-resistant depressive patients, and 31 remitted depressive patients were enrolled in this study (Table 1). All patients were recruited from the outpatient clinics of Teikyo University Chiba Medical Center (Ichihara, Chiba, Japan), and met the DSM-IV criteria for MDD (first episode) [21]. Two senior-level psychiatrists assessed patients’ psychopathology. Patients were all physically healthy and free of alcohol or drug abuse. Inclusion criteria required symptoms of moderate depression, after treatment with at least two antidepressants, for 8 weeks. Patient scores were 14 or more on the 17-item HAM-D scale, where the definition of remission (recovery) was 7 or less [1]. Healthy control subjects with no past history of psychiatric disorders or drug dependence were recruited. Clinical information regarding the subjects is provided in Table 1. The duration of depression in treatment-resistant patients was significantly longer than in remitted depression (Table 1).

Personality Scores and Psychological Tests

Personality was assessed using the TCI-125 (a shortened version of the TCI) [2], [10], [22], [23]. The Japanese version of TCI has been validated and tested for reliability in Japan [24], [25]. Items were rated on a four-point scale (1; totally disagree, 2; disagree, 3; agree and 4; totally agree). This test covers four dimensions of temperament, namely: harm avoidance, novelty seeking, reward dependence, and persistence, and three dimensions of character: self-directedness, cooperativeness, and self-transcendence. To obtain normative data, this test was performed on the 174 healthy controls.

Statistical Analysis

Data from the seven TCI dimensions were first analyzed using multiple analysis of variance (MANOVA), to determine the simultaneous existence of significant differences. Statistical differences among the three groups were determined by a one-way factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA), followed by a multiple comparison test (Scheffe’s test). Chi-square test was used for categorical variables. Statistical evaluation between the two groups was performed using a two-tailed Student’s t-test. Coefficients among scores of TCI were estimated by Pearson coefficient. Differences were considered to be significant when p values were less than 0.01.

Results

MANOVA indicated a significant group effect (F = 9.101, P<0.0001). Subsequent one-way ANOVA demonstrated that treatment-resistant patients showed significantly altered scores on harm avoidance, reward dependence, self-directedness and cooperativeness, but not novelty seeking, persistence, or self-transcendence compared with remitted depression patients and healthy controls (Figure 1). Relative to healthy controls, patients in remission only showed significantly increases in scores for harm avoidance (Figure 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. The data of TCI.

Analysis of variance (ANOVA) shows a significant difference between three groups for (B) Harm Avoidance (F (2,237) = 50.58; P<0.001), (C) Reword Dependence (F (2,237) = 13.19; P<0.001), (E) Self-Directedness (F (2,237) = 25.98; P<0.001), and (F) Cooperativeness (F 2,237) = 5.42; P = 0.005). There are not significant differences between three groups in (A) Novelty seeking, F (2,237) = 4.44, p = 0.013, (D) Persistence, (F (2,237) = 1.81, P = 0.167, and (G) Self-transcendence, F (2,237) = 2.51, P = 0.084. *P<0.01, **P<0.001 compared to control (ANOVA followed by Scheffe’s test).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0063756.g001

The subscales of each dimension of the TCI are shown in Table 2. Treatment-resistant patients showed significantly high scores for anticipatory worry, fear of uncertainty and fatigability in the harm avoidance, and low scores for attachment in reward dependence, responsibility, purposefulness, resourcefulness and congruent second nature in the self-directedness, and empathy, helpfulness and pure-heartedness in the cooperativeness category, compared to remitted depression patients and healthy controls.

Interestingly, harm avoidance, self-directedness and self-transcendence correlate significantly with HAM-D scores in all MDD patients (harm avoidance, r = 0.434, p<0.0001; self-directedness, r = −0.485, p<0.0001; self-transcendence, r = −0.343, p<0.001). In contrast, there were no correlations between TCI scores and severity of depression in patients with treatment-resistant depression (data not shown).

There was a significant negative correlation between reward dependence and harm avoidance in treatment-resistant depression patients. This correlation was not present in healthy controls and remitted depression patients (Table 3). Furthermore, there was a significant negative relationship between cooperativeness and the dimensions of novelty seeking and harm avoidance, in the treatment-resistant group, but not in the healthy control and remitted depression groups (Table 3). Conversely, there was a significant negative correlation between novelty seeking and harm avoidance scores in the healthy control and remitted depression groups, but not in the treatment-resistant depression group (Table 3).

Discussion

In this study, we found a number of psychological features that appeared to be associated with treatment-resistant MDD. Firstly, we found treatment-resistant patients showed higher scores for harm avoidance and lower scores for self-directedness on the TCI, consistent with previous reports on depressed patients [5], [6], [9][11], [13]. We also found that harm avoidance and self-directedness correlate significantly with HAM-D scores in both remitted and treatment-resistant patients with depression, replicating previous studies [6], [7], [9], [11], [12]. Of the seven published studies on TCI scores in depression (Table 4), all found significant alterations in the score for harm avoidance and all but one in the score for self-directedness, indicating that this is a common pattern in depression. Since treatment-resistant patients suffer from depressive symptoms, it is not surprising that these non-responders showed the same pattern of high harm avoidance and low self-directedness as depressed patients.

Secondly, treatment-resistant patients demonstrated low scores for reward dependence on the TCI. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report showing this feature in treatment-resistant MDD patients, and there are no equivalent reports in depressed patients groups (Table 4). This is suggestive of low reward dependence being a characteristic feature of treatment-resistant MDD. Looking more closely at reward dependence, the subscale altered between remitted patients and healthy controls is attachment. Thus, it is likely that the dimension of attachment in reward dependence could be specific to treatment-resistant patients. In a recent study of the antidepressant treatment responders, scores for reward dependence had a small overall positive change from baseline to endpoint [15]. This is supportive of other studies where depressive patients showing high reward dependence on the TCI, also showed a good outcome after antidepressant treatment [17], [22]. Interestingly, scores for reward dependence in non-depressive siblings of depressed patients, were significantly higher than for siblings with a history of depression, suggesting that high reward dependence may protect against the development of depression [9]. However, it is unknown whether the enduring characteristics of non-responders are primary or secondary to the disease. A long history of treatment-resistant depression may induce character changes within patients and these changes may persist after the recovery from disease. Future studies will be needed to elucidate these points.

Thirdly, treatment-resistant patients showed low scores for cooperativeness. Significant scores for low cooperativeness in depressed patients were reported in three of the seven studies examining this issue (Table 4) [5], [9], [13]. This lower concordance suggests that low cooperativeness is a less common characteristic of depressed patients compared with high harm avoidance and low self-directedness. Within the subscale of cooperativeness, the most significantly altered dimensions in comparison with healthy controls, were empathy, helpfulness and pure-heartedness. These results should prove useful in tailoring psychotherapy for MDD treatment-resistant patients. The cooperativeness score correlated negatively with the severity of depression among depressive patients [5]. Other studies showed large increases in cooperativeness and self-directedness scores among treatment responders in MDD, with relative stability of these features among non-responders during treatment [7], [19], [23]. A recent study demonstrated that cooperativeness was strongly associated with perceived social support [26]. As mentioned before, it remains unknown whether low cooperativeness is related primarily to treatment-resistant depression or is a secondary effect due to the long duration of illness. Previous studies using TCI showed that low scores for cooperativeness and self-directedness strongly predicted personality disorders in patients with mood disorders [16], [23]. Furthermore, low cooperativeness could be a predictor for hostility and paranoia [27]. Low reward dependence is strongly associated with cluster A symptoms, such as paranoid, schizoid and schizotypal personality disorders [16]. It is well known that personality disorders have negative effects on the course and outcome of MDD [28][30]. Therefore, treatment-resistant patients with MDD may suffer from some underlying personality disorder traits, although patients with overt personality disorders were excluded from this study at recruitment.

Fourthly, our results showed significant negative correlations between reward dependence and harm avoidance and between cooperativeness and novelty seeking in the treatment-resistant depression group, which were absent in healthy controls and remitted depression patients. This newly highlighted relationship in treatment-resistant depression patients indicates that low scores for reward dependence and cooperativeness could at least in part be due to harm avoidance and novelty seeking, respectively.

Finally, patients in remission still showed high scores for harm avoidance, compared with normal controls, although the difference was small. In this case, the altered subscale between remitted patients and controls was fatigability. This finding is supported by a previous study demonstrating that even though a significant reduction occurred, higher harm avoidance among unipolar depression patients persisted after treatment, compared with healthy controls [6]. Future studies will be needed to elucidate whether harm avoidance plays a role in the relapse of depression.

In conclusion, treatment-resistant patients with MDD demonstrated high scores for harm avoidance, and low scores for reward dependence, self-directedness, and cooperativeness, using the TCI. It is well known that depressed patients show high harm avoidance and low self-directedness, and sometimes low cooperativeness on the TCI. Patients with treatment-resistant MDD show persistent symptoms of depression. Our findings suggest that low reward dependence and to a lesser extent, cooperativeness on the TCI may constitute possible risk factors of treatment-resistant depression.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: MT YS. Performed the experiments: MT YS. Analyzed the data: MT YS. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: MT YS KM MS KS KH. Wrote the paper: MT YS KH.

References

  1. 1. Thase ME, Rush AJ (1995) Treatment-resistant depression. Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ editors. Psychopharmacology: the fourth generation of progress. New York: Raven Press. 1081–1097.
  2. 2. Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR (1993) A psychological model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry 50: 975–990.
  3. 3. Mulder RT, Joyce PR (1994) Relationships of the tridimensional personality questionnaire to mood and personality measures for depressed patients. Psychol Rep 75: 1315–1325.
  4. 4. Chien AJ, Dunner DL (1996) The tridimensional personality questionnaire in depression: state versus trait issues. J Psychiatr Res 30: 21–27.
  5. 5. Hansenne M, Reggers J, Pinto E, Kijiri K, Ajamier A, et al. (1999) Temperament and character inventory (TCI) and depression. J Psychiatr Res 33: 31–36.
  6. 6. Richter J, Eisemann M, Richter G (2000) Temperament and character during the course of unipolar depression among inpatients. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 250: 40–47.
  7. 7. Hirano S, Sato T, Narita T, Kusunoki K, Ozaki N, et al. (2002) Evaluating the state dependency of the temperament and character inventory dimensions in patients with major depression: a methodological contribution. J Affect Disord 69: 31–38.
  8. 8. Marijnissen G, Tuinier S, Sijben AES, Verhoeven WMA (2002) The temperament and character inventory in major depression. J Affect Disord 70: 219–223.
  9. 9. Farmer A, Mahmood A, Redman K, Harris T, Sadler S, et al. (2003) A sib-pair study of the temperament and character inventory scales in major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 60: 490–496.
  10. 10. Smith DJ, Duffy L, Stewart ME, Muir WJ, Blackwood DH (2005) High harm avoidance and low self-directedness in euthymic young adults with recurrent, early-onset depression. J Affect Disord 87: 83–89.
  11. 11. Celikel FC, Kose S, Cumurcu BE, Erkorkmaz U, Sayar K, et al. (2009) Cloninger’s temperament and character dimensions of personality in patients with major depressive disorder. Compr Psychiatry 50: 556–561.
  12. 12. Spittlehouse J, Pearson JF, Luty SE, Mulder RT, Carter JD, et al. (2010) Measures of temperament and character are differentially impacted on by depression severity. J Affect Disord 126: 140–146.
  13. 13. Sasayama D, Hori H, Teraishi T, Hattori K, Ota M, et al. (2011) Difference in Temperament and Character Inventory scores between depressed patients with bipolar II and unipolar major depressive disorders. J Affect Disord 132: 319–324.
  14. 14. Joffe RT, Bagby RM, Levitt AJ, Regan JJ, Parker JD (1993) The tridimensional personality questionnaire in major depression. Am J Psychiatry 150: 959–960.
  15. 15. Kampman O, Poutanen O (2011) Can onset and recovery in depression be predicted by temperament? A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord 135: 20–27.
  16. 16. Svrakic DM, Whitehead C, Przybeck TR, Cloninger CR (1993) Differential diagnosis of personality disorders by the seven-factor model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry 50: 991–999.
  17. 17. Joyce PR, Mulder TR, Cloninger CR (1994) Temperament predicts clomipramine and desipramine response in major depression. J Affect Disord 30: 35–46.
  18. 18. Kampman O, Poutanen O, Illi A, Setälä-Soikkeli E, Viikki M, et al. (2012) Temperament profiles, major depression, and response to treatment with SSRIs in psychiatric out patients. Eur Psychiatry 27: 245–249.
  19. 19. Corruble E, Duret C, Pelissolo A, Falissard B, Guelfi JD (2002) Early and delayed personality changes associated with depression recovery? a one- year follow-up study. Psychiatry Res 109: 17–25.
  20. 20. Nelsen MR, Dunner DL (1995) Clinical and differential diagnostic aspects of treatment-resistant depression. J Psychiatr Res 29: 43–50.
  21. 21. American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM-IV).Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
  22. 22. Tome MB, Cloninger CR, Wason JP, Isaac MT (1997) Serotonergic autoreceptor blockade in the reduction of antidepressant latency: personality variables and response to paroxetine and pindolol. J Affect Disord 44: 101–109.
  23. 23. Black KJ, Sheline YI (1997) Personality disorder scores improve with effective pharmacology of depression. J Affect Disord 43: 11–18.
  24. 24. Kijima M, Tanaka E, Suzuki N, Higuchi H, Kitamura T (2005) Reliability and validity of the Japanese version of the temperament and character inventory. Psychol Rep 86: 1050–1058.
  25. 25. Takeuchi M, Miyaoka H, Tomoda A, Suzuki M, Lu X, et al. (2011) Validity and reliability of Japanese version of the Temperament and Character Inventory: a study of university and college students. Compr Psychiatry 52: 109–117.
  26. 26. Cloninger CR, Zohar AH (2011) Personality and the perception of health and happiness. J Affect Disord 128: 24–32.
  27. 27. Conrad R, Wegener I, Imbierowicz K, Liedtke R, Geiser F (2009) Alexithymia, temperament and character as predictors of psychopathology in patients with major depression. Psychiatry Res 165: 137–144.
  28. 28. Mulder RT (2002) Personalty pathology and treatment outcome in major depression: A review. Am J Psychiatry 159: 359–371.
  29. 29. Gorwood P, Rouillon F, Even C, Falissard B, Corruble E, et al. (2010) Treatment response in major depression: effects of personality dysfunction and prior depression. Br J Psychiatry 196: 139–142.
  30. 30. Skodol AE, Grilo CM, Keyes KM, Geier T, Grant BF, et al. (2011) Relationship of personality disorders to the course of major depressive disorder in a nationally representative sample. Am J Psychiatry 168: 257–264.