Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

The Evolutionary Consequences of Disrupted Male Mating Signals: An Agent-Based Modelling Exploration of Endocrine Disrupting Chemicals in the Guppy

  • Alistair McNair Senior ,

    alistair.senior1985@gmail.com

    Affiliations Zoology Department, The University of Otago, Dunedin, Otago, New Zealand, The Charles Perkins Centre, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia, School of Biological Sciences, The University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia

  • Shinichi Nakagawa,

    Affiliation Zoology Department, The University of Otago, Dunedin, Otago, New Zealand

  • Volker Grimm

    Affiliations Department of Ecological Modelling, Helmholtz Centre for Environmental Research, Leipzig, Saxony, Germany, Institute for Biochemistry and Biology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Brandenburg, Germany

Abstract

Females may select a mate based on signalling traits that are believed to accurately correlate with heritable aspects of male quality. Anthropogenic actions, in particular chemicals released into the environment, are now disrupting the accuracy of mating signals to convey information about male quality. The long-term prediction for disrupted mating signals is most commonly loss of female preference. Yet, this prediction has rarely been tested using quantitative models. We use agent-based models to explore the effects of rapid disruption of mating signals. In our model, a gene determines survival. Males signal their level of genetic quality via a signal trait, which females use to select a mate. We allowed this system of sexual selection to become established, before introducing a disruption between the male signal trait and quality, which was similar in nature to that induced by exogenous chemicals. Finally, we assessed the capacity of the system to recover from this disruption. We found that within a relatively short time frame, disruption of mating signals led to a lasting loss of female preference. Decreases in mean viability at the population-level were also observed, because sexual-selection acting against newly arising deleterious mutations was relaxed. The ability of the population to recover from disrupted mating signals was strongly influenced by the mechanisms that promoted or maintained genetic diversity in traits under sexual selection. Our simple model demonstrates that environmental perturbations to the accuracy of male mating signals can result in a long-term loss of female preference for those signals within a few generations. What is more, the loss of this preference can have knock-on consequences for mean population fitness.

Introduction

Inter-sexual selection, resulting from heritable preferences for mates bearing specific traits, strongly influences the evolutionary trajectory of many species [1]. Often, females select a mate from the many available using signal traits such as large size or elaborate ornaments, which correlate with breeding value or ‘quality’. In doing so, females may obtain direct and/or indirect benefits [1], [2]. For example, under a good-genes scenario, signal traits may correlate with genetic factors that increase survival [3]. Hence, females choosing to mate with a male that bears such a trait benefit indirectly by selecting a father that can contribute genes that enhance offspring viability. Consequently, females showing preference for this signalling trait will, on average, have higher-quality offspring than females that show no preference, and will simultaneously pass on their preference for that trait.

Systems of mate-choice that confer indirect genetic benefits evolve and are maintained because the relationship between signal and breeding value is accurate; perhaps because the signal is too costly to ‘fake’ or because all males invest in signalling traits relative to their quality [4]. However, the ability of females to perceive, and of males to express, signals accurately also relies on the environment [5][7]. Anthropogenic actions are now changing environments at a rate that far surpasses any previously encountered in the evolutionary history of most taxa [8]. In some instances, environmental change, such as the presence of exogenous chemicals or urban noise, may disrupt the relationship between signals of male ‘mating value’ and the ‘true’ breeding value of a male [9][11]. Human-induced environmental changes have therefore been predicted to have consequences for systems of inter-sexual selection [10], [12][15]. At the population level the most common prediction is loss of female preference, because the benefits of expressing mating preferences are negated when signals are unreliable [10], [16], [17]. However, the consequences of disrupted mating signals may be as serious as reductions in population viability depending on the nature of the disruption and the system in question [10], [12], [15][18].

For example, disrupted mating signals have been observed in one of the most renowned examples of inter-sexual selection, the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Female guppies display preference for males that bear orange spots [19][21]. These markings correlate with traits that are essential to survival, such as foraging ability and immunocompetence, and hence are thought to indicate male genetic quality and viability [21][25]. The area of orange spots on male guppies can be reduced by pollutants known as endocrine-disrupting chemicals (EDCs) [26], [27]. EDCs are exogenous chemicals, which disrupt hormonal regulation in exposed individuals [28], [29], and have been suggested to affect intersexual selection in a range of taxa, as well as guppies [10], [15].

Understanding the short- and long-term effects of disrupted male sexual signalling will be achieved through multi-generational experimental studies. However, these studies require significant investment, which may not be justifiable without clear predictions. Thus far, predictions of the effects of environmental perturbations (such as EDCs) on systems of inter-sexual selection have rarely gone further than verbal arguments. Agent-based models (ABMs) can be used to simulate sexual selection [30], allowing preliminary long-term predictions to be obtained in a relatively short time [31]. Here, we present the results of an ABM designed to explore the effects of disrupted male mating signals in a simple system of inter-sexual selection, with stochasticity and overlapping generations.

Within our model, a quantitative heritable trait confers a survival, or viability, advantage (e.g. genes that improve foraging ability or immunocompetence in guppies), and the values of this trait vary among individuals. Males signal their mean allelic value for this trait (e.g. orange spots in guppies). For simplicity, we assume that males honestly and directly signal their genetic quality. However, the expression of signal may be disrupted by an environmental parameter (e.g. EDCs). Females express a heritable preference for male signals (e.g. preference for orange spots in guppies). We parameterise the model to maximise the correlation between evolved signal and female preference in the absence of disrupted mating signals, thus representing inter-sexual selection. We allow this system to evolve before introducing disruption of the relationship between male signal and survival for a period. Finally, we allow the environment to return to its original state. In this way, the model will provide mechanistic insights in to the potential breakdown and recovery of sexual selection.

Our primary aim was to test predictions that female preference will be lost in the face of rapid environmental change that disrupts male-quality signals. We estimate the number of generations over which any preference loss occurs and whether the evolutionary consequences of disrupted mating signals endure once the environment is returned to its original state. Our model was inspired by the guppy system outlined above and therefore takes life-history parameter values (e.g. fecundity and longevity) representing guppies. However, our overall approach is not specific to endocrine-disruption of mating signals in this species. Our model is designed as an initial exploration of the effects of disrupted sexual selection in a simplified system; in particular, our representation of genes, their expression, and selection acting upon them is simplistic. This approach allows us to produce preliminary generalisable results, whilst keeping the contribution of model components those results tractable.

Methods

The Model

The model description follows the ODD (Overview, Design concepts, Details) protocol for describing agent-based models [32], [33]. The model was implemented in NetLogo, a free software platform for implementing agent-based models [34]. NetLogo code for this model can be found in File S2: Model Code.

Purpose.

This model examines the effects of rapid environmental perturbations of male mating signals on systems of inter-sexual selection.

Entities, States, Variables and Scales.

The model includes three types of individuals, males, females and juveniles, and the environment. Individuals have the following state variables: Age (in time steps), and the traits Viability, Signal and Preference (all numeric variables). Viability and Preference are controlled genetically, and are diploid and heritable following Mendelian rules of inheritance. Thus, each trait is determined by two alleles, with one allele inherited from each the mother and father. All alleles are explicitly represented in the model. Traits are expressed as the arithmetic mean of the two alleles carried, an assumption made in previously published models of sexual selection [35]. Individuals senesce as they Age. The trait, Viability, is a numeric variable that can confer a survival advantage, with larger values of the variable conferring better survival. The magnitude of this advantage is scaled by the global parameter ViabilityEffect. Males directly display their level of Viability through the variable Signal. However, the relationship between Viability and Signal may be altered by the global parameter Disruption.

The second heritable trait, Preference, is only expressed by females and determines a female’s ‘choosiness’ for a mate; females with higher levels of Preference will only mate with males with high Signal. Females have three further state variables: 1) Pregnant, i.e. “true” or “false”; 2) Gestation, i.e. the number of time steps for which they have been pregnant and 3) PaternalAlleles, four variables that represent the alleles of the male with which a female has mated.

To prevent population numbers from growing exponentially, density dependence acts on juveniles via the global variables CarryingCapacity and JuvenileNumber. The global variables GestationLength, Fecundity, MaturityAge and Senescence are life history parameters that determine for how long females are pregnant before giving birth, how many offspring a female gives birth to, the age at which individuals are able to reproduce, and longevity, respectively. We parameterised such aspects of the model to represent guppy life history.

All the parameters outlined here can be found in Table 1, and their mode of action is given below. A time step in the model corresponds to one day; simulations are usually run for several thousand-time steps. Space is not represented.

thumbnail
Table 1. Descriptions of model parameters, the values (italicised values are specific to guppy life history) and range over which they have been used in simulations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103100.t001

Process Overview and Scheduling.

At every time step, the following four processes (name of submodel given in brackets) are executed in the given order: 1) at certain time points (Fig. 1), an environmental perturbation of signal expression is introduced or removed (environmental disruption); 2) individuals either die or increase their Age (ageing and death); 3) females with Pregnant = ‘true’, give birth or increase Gestation (reproduction); 4) females with Pregnant = ‘false’, select a male and mate (mating). Individuals are processed in a randomized sequence and state variables are updated immediately after an individual performs an action.

thumbnail
Figure 1. Overview of the Models Sampling Regime.

A schematic of the periods and time-points in the model and whether environmental disruption (Disruption) of male-mating signals is present of absent at those time points. Mating signals are disrupted (i.e. Disruption>0) for the period DisruptionDuration. The actual length of DisruptionDuration and value of Disruption is varied between model runs (Table 1). At each time-point (A, B, C.1 and C.2) genetic data about individuals in the population are recorded.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103100.g001

Design Concepts.

Individual behaviour is imposed via probabilistic rules. Females are able to sense the parameter Signal displayed by the males. Interaction occurs indirectly via density-dependent mortality of juveniles and directly via mating. Stochasticity is included in the model’s initialization to create initial genetic variation in all traits, and in all sub-models where probabilities of certain events are implemented via Bernoulli trials to represent demographic and genetic variability. Additionally, a random Poisson distribution determines fecundity at a given event.

To observe the evolutionary response of the simulated population, we observe the mean allelic values of all living individuals for: 1) Preference and 2) Viability. In both instances the paternally inherited allele was recorded, and preliminary model runs demonstrated that the same results are observed if the maternally inherited allele is used. These outputs were recorded at four time-points during a given model run (Fig. 1): 1) the time step immediately prior to the introduction of mating-signal disruption (time-point A); 2) the time step immediately prior to end of the disruption (time-point B); 3) 10,000 time steps after disruption has ceased (time-point C.1); and 4) 30,000 time steps after disruption has ceased (time-point C.2), immediately prior to the termination of a model run.

Initialization.

The model is initialized with 75 males and 75 females. Each of these individuals is assigned an age from a random uniform distribution with a lower limit of zero and an upper limit of one thousand time steps (similar to a guppies maximum longevity). For mature females (i.e. Age>MaturityAge), Pregnancy is set ‘true’ with a probability of 0.5. Pregnant females are then assigned a Gestation from a random uniform distribution with a lower limit of one and an upper limit of GestationLength. For pregnant females, PaternalAlleles are assigned as randomly chosen floating point values between 0 and 1. Likewise, the alleles of all individuals are assigned random values between 0 and 1. These mechanisms initialise the model with genetic variability from which population levels of Viability and Preference evolve. Trait (phenotypic) values of Viability and Preference are then calculated as the arithmetic mean of the two alleles carried for that gene. Finally, males calculate their levels of Signal as via Eq. 1,(1)where all parameters are as defined in Table 1. Thus, in the absence of an environmental disruption Signal directly conveys Viability.

Sub-models.

Environmental Disruption: Environmental effects disrupt the expression of Signal via Eq 1.The stronger Disruption, the smaller the disparity between the Signal of males with differing Viability. At model initialisation, environmental effects are assumed to be absent and hence, Disruption is set to zero. The model is then given a burn-in period of 20,000 time steps (Fig. 1), simulating an established system of sexual selection that evolves from the genetic variation with which the model was initialised. Subsequently, a disrupting effect is introduced (time-point A, Fig. 1), and Disruption is set to an experimental level (Table 1) for a given period of time, DisruptionDuration (Fig. 1, Table 1). After this period, the disruption is removed from the environment and Disruption is set back to zero (time-point B, Fig. 1). Simulations then continue for 30,000 time steps. All the results presented here come from models that assume EDCs only affect the development of sexual signals in males, however such chemicals can also lead to sex-reversal [36], and likely have knock consequences for other aspects of fitness [37]. We tested the sensitivity of our conclusions to the presence of sex reversal with a short expansion of the model described above. That model simultaneously includes feminisation of males alongside reduction in sexual signal with EDC presence. The model incorporating feminisation produces qualitatively identical results to those described here in the main text. Larger differences between models incorporating feminisation and those not, may be expected under spatially explicit models as well as under differing assumptions about sperm limitation (Figs. S4, S5 and S6 and additional discussion in File S1: Supporting Information).

Aging and Death: During the aging and death submodel, each individual dies with a given probability. The probability of survival is calculated differently for juveniles and adults. For juveniles (i.e. Age<MaturityAge) survival is density-dependent, with the probability of surviving, being given by Eq. 2(2)where parameters are as given in Table 1. Thus, as the number of juveniles in the population approaches carrying capacity, juvenile probability of survival approaches 0. For adults, the probability of surviving is dependant on their Age as well as their level of Viability and the advantage conferred by Viability, given by Eq 3(3)where parameters are as given in Table 1. Thus as individuals age, they are more likely to die, but a higher level of Viability may confer a survival advantage depending on ViabilityEffect.

Reproduction: This sub-model applies to females with Pregnant = ‘true’. If Gestation equals GestationLength, a female gives birth to offspring, the number of which is determined by Fecundity (Table 1). Each offspring has an equal probability of being male and female. Each offspring is assigned, one allele from the mother and one allele from the father (stored in the females state variables; PaternalAlleles) for Viability and Preference. The alleles are randomly chosen from the two parental alleles with equal probability. Before an allele is assigned to an offspring, it may undergo mutation with a given probability; PreferenceMutation for Preference alleles and ViabilityMutation for Viability alleles (Table 1). If mutation occurs, the allele is assigned a random value between zero and one. Although guppies are diploid [38], we note that a number of groups of fish are largely polyploidy [39]; moving from a diploid to a polyploid system of inheritance in this model would have the same effect on the models outcome as increasing the number of alleles that contribute to the trait. Finally, the trait (phenotype) values Viability and Preference are calculated for all individuals (mean of allelic values). Offspring Age is set to zero. Male offspring set Signal according to Eq. 1, female offspring set Pregnant = ‘false’, Gestation = 0 and PaternalAlleles = 0. Here, we make the assumption that, whilst offspring a within a gravid female, they are sheltered from the effects of disruption on mating signals. Some data suggest that masculinising EDCs can affect offspring sexual development whilst in the mother, however such a response only seems to occur when the mother is directly fed large amounts EDC [40]. For lower background doses of EDCs, our assumption that the offspring is sheltered during pregnancy is probably sensible. Additionally, given that the majority of individuals will be exposed to disruption throughout life, the point at which that disruption has its effect will only alter the models output in a limited manner.

Mating: The mating sub-model only applies to females with Pregnant = ‘false’. Females randomly select a mate from all of the mature males with a level of Signal that is equal to or higher than her level of Preference. If no such male is available, the female does not mate on that iteration, thus imposing an opportunity cost to female selectivity. Where multiple males fulfil that criterion, each of those males has an equal probability of being selected as the mate. If a female does mate, she sets Pregnant = ‘true’, Gestation = 1, and the four alleles of the mated male are stored in the female’s state variables, PaternalAlleles.

Model Parameter Values

Parameter values regarding life history (Table 1) were taken from the guppy literature [41]. To keep the degrees of freedom in parameter choice manageable, we identified regions in the parameter space representative of the kind of system we were interested in investigating, i.e. a mating system where females display preference for male mating signals.

Allelic values for Viability were bound at 0 and 1, thus allowing no (0) or some ‘theoretical maximum’ (1) survival advantage. To parameterize mutation rates and the ViabilityEffect parameter we explored the model without disruption to identify values that favoured the co-evolution of Preference and Signal, thus imitating sexual selection. We searched for combinations of parameter values that maximised the correlation between the resulting final population averages of Preference and Signal. Correlations were estimated based on the results of 200 model runs per parameter set using the cor.test function in R 3.0.1 [42]. Stronger correlations indicate settings conducive to the co-evolution of female preference for the males’ signals; i.e. strong inter-sexual selection. Of the values we tested, we found the strongest correlation between preference and signal was favoured when ViabilityMutation was high (0.05), PreferenceMutation was low (0.001) and ViabilityEffect was low (0.3) (Pearson’s product moment correlation coefficient (r) = 0.86). A complete set of said results and discussion thereof can be found in the supplementary material (Table S1 in File S1: Supporting Information). As observed, the difference between mutation rates of alleles of different genes, which maximizes the correlation between Preference and Signal, is extreme. Hence, models of disrupted signals were also run with a less extreme difference in mutation rates (ViabilityMutation = 0.05 and PreferenceMutation = 0.01), although the correlation between Signal and Preference was weaker (r = 0.72). In the following, we refer to the two genetic environments as those with low (0.001) and high (0.01) PreferenceMutation.

The probabilities of mutation in our model are high [43]. However, any model of sexual selection must maintain variance in the traits under sexual selection via some mechanism [44]. In reality, a suite of interacting factors may maintain variation in traits under sexual selection and exploring those effects is an active field of research [45][49]. Here, for simplicity, we assume a high level of mutation to implicitly represent all these processes, as has been done in previous models of sexual selection (e.g. Kokko et al. [44]).

To understand the effect of environmental disruption, we varied Disruption and DisruptionDuration (Table 1). The parameter space for these two variables was initially sampled sparsely. Once areas of parameter space that effectively captured a range of effects of Disruption and DisruptionDuration on the evolution of Preference had been identified, these areas were subject to more rigorous testing (i.e. sensitivity analysis). The values tested are given in Table 1 and each parameter set was tested with 100 model runs, giving a total of eight thousand model runs.

Results

Population Extinction

On a number of model runs, population extinction occurred following the disruption of mating signals. Extinctions occurred when females forwent mating because males displayed ‘unsatisfactory’ Signal. Under low PreferenceMutation, extinctions occurred on 19.6% of runs (n = 1568/8000) and visual exploration of the data suggested extinctions were predicted by three variables. Firstly, extinctions occurred relatively infrequently at weak Disruption, and became more frequent as Disruption became stronger (Fig. 2A). Secondly, DisruptionDuration had a threshold effect on extinction, with extinctions being observed frequently at values over one thousand (Fig. 2B). This threshold was determined by the generation time in the model (i.e. once males with high levels of Signal had died out), and hence would likely be species specific. Finally, Preference at time-point A predicted extinctions, with extinctions occurring more frequently at higher levels of Preference (Fig. 2C). Hence, extinctions were more common when females were choosy.

thumbnail
Figure 2. Factors Affecting Extinction After Disruption of Mating Signals.

The proportion of model runs that resulted in population extinction with (A) varying Disruption, (B) varying DisruptionDuration, and (C) the level of Preference at time-point A (at the paternally inherited allele immediately prior to disruption of mating signals). In panels A, B and C the PreferenceMutation is 0.001, favouring a strong correlation between Signal and Preference. The presented data are from full model runs where Disruption and DisruptionDuration were co-varied (see section “Parameter Settings” for an overview of parameter values). Panel (D) represents the total proportion of experimental model runs resulting in population extinction with differing levels of Preference.Mutation (0.001 and 0.01). Disruption and DisruptionDuration were co-varied and other parameters were fixed at levels given in Table 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103100.g002

Under high PreferenceMutation, extinctions were predicted by the same three parameters as those with low PreferenceMutation (Fig. S1 in File S1: Supporting Information), but occurred on only 8.05% of model runs (n = 644/8000, Fig. 2D). These contrasting results highlight the importance of variation in Preference in preventing population extinction. Under such a scenario Preference was maintained by a higher mutation rate (PreferenceMutation = 0.01), which was a key factor in facilitating population persistence in the presence of Disruption. Higher mutation rates generated alleles for low Preference, which were necessary for females to adapt to the lower levels of Signal being expressed by males in the presence of Disruption.

The Effects of Environmental Disruption on Preference and Viability

We then explored the effects of co-varying Disruption and DisruptionDuration with low PreferenceMutation, as these settings maximised the correlation between Signal and Preference. At time-point A (prior to the mating signal disruption), the average level of Preference (at the paternally inherited allele) was just over 0.575, with some stochastic variation (Fig. 3). At time-point B (at the end of the disruption period), Disruption had reduced Preference. The extent to which Preference was reduced by Disruption and DisruptionDuration was proportional to the magnitude of these variables in an interactive manner (Fig. 3). When Disruption and DisruptionDuration were set at low values, Preference was only mildly affected. When Disruption and DisruptionDuration were both set at high values, Preference was reduced by a large amount (Fig. 3). However, if either DisruptionDuration or Disruption was low (500–1000 or ≤0.2, respectively), the other variable could be relatively high value, and only mild reductions of Preference occurred (Fig. 3). In the 10.000 and 20.000 iterations after disruption of mating signals (time-points C.1 and C.2), Preference typically remained low, except at very low levels of environmental disruption; e.g. at Disruption = 0.1 through 0.3 and DisruptionDuration = 500 through 2000 (Fig. 3).

thumbnail
Figure 3. The Effects of Disruption on Mating Preference.

The effects of co-varying Disruption and DisruptionDuration on the mean level of Preference at the paternally inherited allele of all living individuals at time-point A (immediately prior to signal disruption), time-point B (immediately after signal disruption has ended), time-point C.1 (ten thousand iterations after time-point B), and time-point C.2 (thirty thousand iterations after time-point B). If a model run resulted in extinction post disruption, data from time-points post extinction on that model run have been removed. Therefore, sample sizes vary for different time points. Time-point A n = 8000, time-point B n = 6679, time-point C.1 n = 6432, Time-point C.2 n = 6432. PreferenceMutation = 0.001 and all other parameters, except those on the x and y axes, were fixed at values given in Table 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103100.g003

At time-point A, Viability (at the paternally inherited allele) was high with minor variation (∼0.85–0.9; Fig. 4). At time-point B, Viability remained high but was slightly less variable than at time-point A (Fig. 4). At time-points C.1 and C.2, declines in Viability were associated with high levels of Disruption and DisruptionDuration (Fig. 4).

thumbnail
Figure 4. The Effects of Disruption on Viability.

The effects of co-varying Disruption and DisruptionDuration on the mean level of Viability at the paternally inherited allele of all living individuals at time-point A (immediately prior to signal disruption), time-point B (immediately after signal disruption has ended), time-point C.1 (ten thousand iterations after time-point B), and time-point C.2 (thirty thousand iterations after time-point B). If a model run resulted in extinction post disruption, data from time-points post extinction on that model run have been removed. Therefore, sample sizes vary for different time-points. Time-point A n = 8000, time-point B n = 6679, time-point C.1 n = 6432, Time-point C.2 n = 6432. PreferenceMutation = 0.001 and all other parameters, except those on the x and y axes, were fixed at values given in Table 1.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103100.g004

Disruption of Mating Signals Under High and Low PreferenceMutation

Finally, we compared the effects of varying Disruption at high (0.01) and low (0.001) levels of PreferenceMutation with DisruptionDuration fixed at 5000. Complete sets of results from model runs with high PreferenceMutation can be found in the supporting material (Figs. S1, S2 and S3 in File S1: Supporting Information).

Slightly lower mean levels of Preference evolved at time-point A with high PreferenceMutation than with low PreferenceMutation (Figs. 5A and 5B). With both high and low PreferenceMutation, Preference was reduced at time-point B relative to time-point A, with greater reductions in Preference seen with higher levels of Disruption. As discussed above, Preference remained largely reduced at time-points C.1 and C.2 with low PreferenceMutation (Fig. 5A). However, with high PreferenceMutation promoting genetic variation in Preference, levels of Preference began to recover at time-points C.1 and C.2 (Fig. 5B).

thumbnail
Figure 5. The Effects of Disruption Under Differing Levels of Allele Mutation.

The effects of Disruption at different time-points (A, B, C.1 and C.2) under varying levels of Disruption (0 through 0.7 by 0.1 = grey scale, light to black) on A) Preference under low mutation for preference alleles (MutationPreference = 0.001), B) Preference under high mutation for preference alleles (MutationPreference = 0.01), C) Viability under low mutation for preference alleles (MutationPreference = 0.001) and D) Viability under high mutation for preference alleles (MutationPreference = 0.01). All estimates are based on the mean allelic values carried by all living individuals at their paternally inherited alleles. Dashed lines are means +1SD at Disruption = 0 (light grey) and means - 1SD at Disruption = 0.7 (black). All parameters were fixed at the values given in Table 1 and DisruptionDuration = 5000. If a model run resulted in extinction post disruption, data from time-points post extinction on that model run have been removed.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103100.g005

The levels of Viability at time-point A were similar with high and low PreferenceMutation (Figs. 5C and 5D). In the periods after DisruptionDuration, Viability was reduced under both high and low PreferenceMutation, although to a greater extent in the latter and with increasing Disruption (Figs. 5C and 5D). Recovery of Viability was observed by time-point C.2 under high PreferenceMutation, but not under low PreferenceMutation (Figs. 5C and 5D).

Discussion

The main aim of this study was to test to what degree and how fast female preference was lost in the presence of disruptions to the accuracy of male mating signals to convey information about male genetic quality. We also intended to establish insight into the capacity for the population to recover after disruption. We found that when the environment had a strong disruptive effect and female preference evolved to be highly stringent, the population could be driven to extinction (discussed in detail below). However, when the population was not driven to extinction, our results were in line with previous predictions [10]; we saw that environmental effects that disrupted the reliability of traits which signalled male quality reduced the population-average level of female preference for that signalling trait. We also saw reductions in mean viability of individuals within the population.

The level and duration of loss of preference was correlated with the strength of the disruption present. For most levels of disruption, after around two thousand iterations (about 24 simulated generations), persistent reductions in female preference for male mating signals were noticeable. Anthropogenic disruptions of mating signals have been recognised for over a decade [50], [51] and are likely to have been active for even longer. For example, it is around 20 years since the publication of initial studies describing the presence of EDC effects in fish [52], [53]. Hence, in areas where chronic EDC-contamination disrupts male mating signals, short-lived species may already display decreased mating preferences.

While we expected that ultimately the disrupting chemical would lead to a loss of female preferences, we were surprised by the effects on individual and population viability. Additionally, predictions about the effects of EDCs on individual viability were difficult to make, as it was unclear whether viability selection alone would maintain this trait. Initially in our model, sexual selection acted against mutations that reduced mean viability, because ‘choosy’ females would not mate with males carrying these mutations. However, after disruption of mating signals (time-points C.1 and C.2) female preference was reduced to such an extent that sexual selection against deleterious mutations relaxed and, ultimately, mean viability declined. Where preference re-evolved after disruption (i.e. with high PreferenceMutation), sexual selection promoted a recovery in population viability. Our observations agree with hypotheses that sexual selection via good-genes mechanisms can increase mean population-level viability, as mate-choice increases the frequency of highly viable individuals [54], [55]. What is more, this hypothesis is supported by recent experimental evidence [56], [57]. We do note, however, that this effect may not be universal; Arbuthnott and Rundle [58] failed to find similar effects in Drosophila melanogaster. Nevertheless, we suggest that for species in which sexual selection plays a role in purging deleterious alleles, disruption of mating signals may counteract this important effect.

We found that recovery of preference and viability after disruption was strongly associated with the rates of mutation of preference alleles. These results highlight the importance of understanding how additive genetic variation is generated and maintained in predicting how systems of sexual selection respond to rapid environmental changes. The importance of genetic variation for evolutionary responses to anthropogenic change has certainly not previously gone unnoticed in the literature [59], [60].

When females became highly selective and disruption of mating signals was strong, we observed population extinctions. Under such conditions, the signals displayed by males were shifted below the stringent level of preference that evolved. Therefore, females did not mate as males displayed ‘substandard’ signals; sometimes termed the ‘wallflower effect’ [61]. Jennions and Petrie [62] noted that where time constraints play a role, females may go unmated as a result of displaying mating preferences. However, such a scenario seems unlikely to apply to species like guppies that breed year-round [41]. What is more, models have also shown that when wallflower effects are present, extreme mate preferences should not evolve [63].

The extinction risk that we observe could, therefore, be considered unrealistic because the extreme mating preferences underlying extinction are unlikely to evolve. However, in some species, females may well have evolved high levels of discrimination in the absence of a wallflower effect before disruption of mating signals makes mating failure a possibility. This form of ‘evolutionary trap’ [64] may not be relevant to all species. However, for species in which females have become extremely choosy, rapid loss of mating signals could pose a risk to population viability.

So far, no consensus on the nature of the relationship between extinction risk and sexual selection has been reached [16], [57], [65]. Candolin and Heuschele [16] found that most evidence suggests sexual selection has either no, or mildly maladaptive effects, on a population’s initial ability to adapt to a changing environment. They conclude that sexual selection most likely negatively influences population persistence in the face of rapid environmental change [16]. The form of disruption that we fitted to male mating signals in our model would constitute a rapid change: within 6 to 12 generations, environmental disruption had reduced the level of male signals within the population to such an extent as the cost of female choosiness (a wallflower effect) increased extinction risk.

Our model contained a simplified mechanism of sexual selection. We felt that it would be best to begin with a simple model, as it allowed us to build a framework in which the results of the model remained tractable. Our approach may make our findings the result of ‘imposed’ or ‘rigid’ behaviours. For example, we use a strict model of mate choice, which underpinned the observed extinction. We have identified a number of aspects from which complexity may be added to the model to more realistically capture biological evolution. Table 2 outlines these areas as well as predictions of how these changes to the model may affect the evolutionary consequences of disrupted male mating signals.

thumbnail
Table 2. Future directions under which our model may be expanded to better capture the complexity of biological systems or to make further predictions about the effects of EDCs on systems of sexual selection.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103100.t002

Nevertheless, our model showed that rapid environmental disruptions to the relationship between mating signals of male quality and underlying male fitness could alter the dynamics of female mate-choice and even mean population fitness. What is more, these effects can remain after the removal of the responsible environmental change. Short-term behavioural studies may be used to better parameterize models, allowing more precise long-term predictions to be made. We believe that ABMs represent an ideal system in which to further explore and generate predictions about the effects of disrupted male mating signals on systems of sexual selection. However, ultimately a suite of multi-generational experimental manipulations will be required to test the results presented here.

Supporting Information

File S1.

Supporting Information. Details of model testing, including Table S1, as well as details of our exploration of a model containing environmental sex reversal as a result of endocrine disrupting chemicals. Supplementary results (Figs. S1 through S6) are also given.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103100.s001

(DOCX)

File S2.

Model Code. Model code for Netlogo and programming notes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103100.s002

(RTF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Emily Weiser, Catherine Grueber an anonymous reviewer and the editorial team at PLOS ONE for their comments on this manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: AMS SN VG. Performed the experiments: AMS VG. Analyzed the data: AMS VG. Contributed to the writing of the manuscript: AMS SN VG.

References

  1. 1. Andersson M (1994) Sexual Selection; Krebs JR, Clutton-Brock TH, editors. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  2. 2. Kokko H, Brooks R, Jennions M, Morley J (2003) The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proc R Soc Lond B 270: 653–664.
  3. 3. Jennions MD, Moller AP, Petrie M (2001) Sexually selected traits and adult survival: A meta-analysis. Quarterly Review of Biology 76: 3–36.
  4. 4. Maynard-Smith J, Harper D (2003) Animal Signals; Harvey PH, May RM, editors. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  5. 5. Candolin U (2009) Population responses to anthropogenic disturbance: lessons from three-spined sticklebacks Gasterosteus aculeatus in eutrophic habitats. Journal of Fish Biology 75: 2108–2121.
  6. 6. David P, Bjorksten T, Fowler K, Pomiankowski A (2000) Condition-dependent signalling of genetic variation in stalk-eyed flies. Nature 406: 186–188.
  7. 7. Vergara P, Martinez-Padilla J, Mougeot F, Leckie F, Redpath SM (2012) Environmental heterogeneity influences the reliability of secondary sexual traits as condition indicators. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 25: 20–28.
  8. 8. Sih A, Ferrari MCO, Harris DJ (2011) Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change. Evolutionary Applications 4: 367–387.
  9. 9. Jašarević E, Sieli PT, Twellman EE, Welsh TH, Schachtman TR, et al. (2011) Disruption of adult expression of sexually selected traits by developmental exposure to bisphenol A. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108: 11715–11720.
  10. 10. Shenoy K, Crowley PH (2011) Endocrine disruption of male mating signals: ecological and evolutionary implications. Functional Ecology 25: 433–448.
  11. 11. Slabbekoorn H, Ripmeester EAP (2008) Birdsong and anthropogenic noise: implications and applications for conservation. Molecular Ecology 17: 72–83.
  12. 12. Candolin U, Wong BBM (2012) Sexual selection in changing environments: consequences for individuals and populations In: Candolin U, Wong BBM, editors. Behavioural responses to a changing world. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 201–215.
  13. 13. Tuomainen U, Candolin U (2011) Behavioural responses to human-induced environmental change. Biological Reviews 86: 640–657.
  14. 14. Tuomainen U, Sylvin E, Candolin U (2011) Adaptive phenotypic differentiation of courtship in response to recent anthropogenic disturbance. Evolutionary Ecology Research 13: 697–710.
  15. 15. Lane JE, Forrest MNK, Willis CKR (2011) Anthropogenic influences on natural animal mating systems. Animal Behaviour 81: 909–917.
  16. 16. Candolin U, Heuschele J (2008) Is sexual selection beneficial during adaptation to environmental change? Trends in Ecology & Evolution 23: 446–452.
  17. 17. Candolin U, Salesto T, Evers M (2007) Changed environmental conditions weaken sexual selection in sticklebacks. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20: 233–239.
  18. 18. Muir WM, Howard RD (1999) Possible ecological risks of transgenic organism release when transgenes affect mating success: Sexual selection and the Trojan gene hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 96: 13853–13856.
  19. 19. Brooks R, Endler J (2001) Female guppies agree to differ: phenotypic and genetic variation in mate-choice behaviour and the consequences for sexual selection. Evolution 55: 1644–1655.
  20. 20. Endler JA (1980) Natural selection on color patterns in Poecilia reticulata. Evolution 34: 76–91.
  21. 21. Houde AE (1997) Sex, Color, and Mate Choice In Guppies; Krebs JR, Clutton-Brock T, editors. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
  22. 22. Houde AE, Torio AJ (1992) Effect of parasitic infection on male color pattern and female choice in guppies. Behavioral Ecology 3: 346–351.
  23. 23. Karino K, Shimada Y, Kudo H, Sato A (2010) Relative importance of the area and intensity of the orange spots of male guppies Poecilia reticulata as mating traits preferred by females. Journal of Fish Biology 77: 299–307.
  24. 24. Kodric-Brown A (1989) Dietary carotenoids and male mating success in the guppy - an environmental component to female choice. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 25: 393–401.
  25. 25. Kolluru GR, Grether GF, Dunlop E, South SH (2009) Food availability and parasite infection influence mating tactics in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Behavioral Ecology 20: 131–137.
  26. 26. Baatrup E, Junge M (2001) Antiandrogenic pesticides disrupt sexual characteristics in the adult male guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Environmental Health Perspectives 109: 1063–1070.
  27. 27. Shenoy K (2012) Environmentally realistic exposure to the herbicide atrazine alters some sexually selected traits in male guppies. PLoS ONE 7: e30611.
  28. 28. Colborn T, Saal FSV, Soto AM (1993) Developmental effects of endocrine-disrupting chemicals in widlife and humans. Environmental Health Perspectives 101: 378–384.
  29. 29. Hotchkiss AK, Rider CV, Blystone CR, Wilson VS, Hartig PC, et al. (2008) Fifteen years after “Wingspread” - Environmental endocrine disrupters and human and wildlife health: Where we are today and where we need to go. Toxicological Sciences 105: 235–259.
  30. 30. Kuijper B, Pen I, Weissing FJ (2012) A Guide to Sexual Selection Theory. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 43: 287–311.
  31. 31. Grimm V, Railsback SF (2005) Individual-Based Modeling and Ecology; Levin SA, editor. New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
  32. 32. Grimm V, Berger U, Bastiansen F, Eliassen S, Ginot V, et al. (2006) A standard protocol for describing individual-based and agent-based models. Ecological Modelling 198: 115–126.
  33. 33. Grimm V, Berger U, DeAngelis DL, Polhill JG, Giske J, et al. (2010) The ODD protocol: A review and first update. Ecological Modelling 221: 2760–2768.
  34. 34. Wilensky U (1999) NetLogo. Available: http://ccl.northwestern.edu/netlogo/. Center for Connected Learning and Computer-Based Modeling, Northwestern University Evanston, IL.
  35. 35. Rankin DJ, Dieckmann U, Kokko H (2011) Sexual conflict and the tragedy of the commons. The American Naturalist 177: 780–791.
  36. 36. Senior AM, Nakagawa S (2013) A comparative analysis of chemically induced sex reversal in teleosts: challenging conventional suppositions. Fish and Fisheries 14: 60–76.
  37. 37. Senior AM, Lim JN, Nakagawa S (2012) The fitness consequences of environmental sex reversal in fish: a quantitative review. Biological Reviews 87: 900–911.
  38. 38. Lodi E (1978) Chromosome Complement of the Guppy, Poecilia Reticulata Peters (Pisces, Osteichtyhyes). Caryologia 31: 475–477.
  39. 39. Zhan SH, Glick L, Tsigenopoulos CS, Otto SP, Mayrose I (2014) Comparative analysis reveals that polyploidy does not decelerate diversification in fish. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 27: 391–403.
  40. 40. Kavumpurath S, Pandian TJ (1993) Masculinization of Poecilia reticulata by dietary administration of synthetic or natural androgen to gravid females. Aquaculture 116: 83–89.
  41. 41. Magurran AE (2005) Evolutionary Ecology: The Trinidadian Guppy; Harvey PH, May RM, editors. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  42. 42. R-Development-Core-Team (2013) R: A language and environemnt for Statistical computing. Version 3.0.1 Available: http://www.r-project.org.
  43. 43. Sniegowski PD, Gerrish PJ, Johnson T, Shaver A (2000) The evolution of mutation rates: separating causes from consequences. Bioessays 22: 1057–1066.
  44. 44. Kokko H, Brooks R, McNamara J, Houston A (2002) The sexual selection continuum. Proc R Soc Lond B 269: 1331–1340.
  45. 45. Bonduriansky R, Day T (2013) Nongenetic inheritance and the evolution of costly female preference. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 26: 76–87.
  46. 46. Kotiaho J, LeBas N, Puurtinen M, Tomkins J (2007) On the resolution of the lek paradox. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 23: 1–3.
  47. 47. Kotiaho J, Simmons L, Tomkins J (2001) Towards a resolution of the lek paradox. Nature 410: 684–686.
  48. 48. Lehtonen TK, Wong BBM, Lindström K (2010) Fluctuating mate preferences in a marine fish. Biology Letters 6: 21–23.
  49. 49. Miller CW, Moore AJ (2007) A potential resolution to the lek paradox through indirect genetic effects. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274: 1279–1286.
  50. 50. Eeva T, Lehikoinen E, Ronka M (1998) Air pollution fades the plumage of the Great Tit. Functional Ecology 12: 607–612.
  51. 51. Seehausen O, vanAlphen JJM, Witte F (1997) Cichlid fish diversity threatened by eutrophication that curbs sexual selection. Science 277: 1808–1811.
  52. 52. Purdom CE, Hardiman PA, Bye VJ, Eno NC, Tyler CR, et al. (1994) Estrogenic effects of effluents from sewage treatment works. Chemistry and Ecology 8: 275–285.
  53. 53. Rempel MA, Schlenk D (2008) Effects of environmental estrogens and antiandrogens on endocrine function, gene regulation, and health in fish. International Review of Cell and Molecular Biology 267: 207–252.
  54. 54. Rice WR (1998) Requisite mutational load, pathway epistasis, and deterministic mutation accumulation in sexual versus asexual populations. Genetica 102–3: 71–81.
  55. 55. Whitlock MC, Agrawal AF (2009) Purging the genome with sexual selection: reducing mutation load through selection on males. Evolution 63: 569–582.
  56. 56. McGuigan K, Petfield D, Blows MW (2011) Reducing mutation load through sexual selection on males. Evolution 65: 2816–2829.
  57. 57. Jarzebowska M, Radwan J (2010) Sexual selection conteracts extinction of small populations of the bulb mites. Evolution 64.
  58. 58. Arbuthnott D, Rundle HD (2012) Sexual selection is ineffectual or inhibits purging of deleterious mutations in Drosophila melongaster. Evolution 66: 2127–2137.
  59. 59. Tinghitella RM, Zuk M (2009) Asymmetric mating preferences accommodated the rapid evolutionary loss of a sexual signal. Evolution 63: 2087–2098.
  60. 60. Barrett RDH, Hendry AP (2012) Evolutionary rescue under environmental change? In: Candolin U, Wong BBM, editors. Behavioural Responses to a Changing World. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 216–233.
  61. 61. Kokko H, Mappes J (2005) Sexual selection when fertilization is not guranteed. Evolution 59: 1876–1885.
  62. 62. Jennions M, Petrie M (1997) Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences. Biol Rev 72: 283–327.
  63. 63. De Jong MCM, Sabelis MW (1991) Limits to runaway selection: The wallflower paradox. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 4: 637–355.
  64. 64. Robertson B, Rehage J, Sih A (2013) Ecological novelty and the emergence of evolutionary traps. Trends in Ecology & Evolution.
  65. 65. Kokko H, Brooks R (2003) Sexy to die for? Sexual selection and the risk of extinction. Annales Zoologici Fennici 40: 207–219.
  66. 66. Hall D, Kirkpatrick M, West B (2000) Runaway sexual selection when female preferences are directly selected. Evolution 54: 1862–1869.
  67. 67. Eshel I, Volovik I, Sansone E (2000) On Fisher-Zahavi’s handicapped sexy son. Evolutionary Ecology Research 2: 509–523.
  68. 68. Andersson M, Iwasa Y (1996) Sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 11: 53–58.
  69. 69. Chandler CH, Ofria C, Dworkin I (2013) Runaway sexual selection leads to good genes. Evolution 67: 110–119.
  70. 70. Iwasa Y, Pomiankowski A, Nee S (1991) The Evolution of Costly Mate Preferences II. The ‘Handicap’ Principle. Evolution 45: 1431–1442.
  71. 71. Kirkpatrick M, Ryan M (1991) The evolution of mating preferences and the paradox of the lek. Nature 350: 33–38.
  72. 72. Pomiankowski A, Iwasa Y, Nee S (1991) The Evolution of Costly Mate Preferences I. Fisher and Biased Mutation. Evolution 45: 1422–1430.
  73. 73. Gordon SP, Lopez-Sepulcre A, Reznick DN (2011) Predation-associated differences in sex linkage of wild guppy coloration. Evolution 66: 918–918.
  74. 74. Candolin U (2003) The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biological Reviews 78: 575–595.
  75. 75. Charlesworth B (1990) Mutation-selection balance and the evolutionary adavantage of sex and recombination. Genetical Research 55: 199–221.