Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Identifying Stride-To-Stride Control Strategies in Human Treadmill Walking

  • Jonathan B. Dingwell ,

    jdingwell@austin.utexas.edu

    ‡ Joint Senior Authors on this work.

    Affiliation Department of Kinesiology & Health Education, University of Texas, Austin, Texas, United States of America

  • Joseph P. Cusumano

    ‡ Joint Senior Authors on this work.

    Affiliation Department of Engineering Science & Mechanics, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania, United States of America

Abstract

Variability is ubiquitous in human movement, arising from internal and external noise, inherent biological redundancy, and from the neurophysiological control actions that help regulate movement fluctuations. Increased walking variability can lead to increased energetic cost and/or increased fall risk. Conversely, biological noise may be beneficial, even necessary, to enhance motor performance. Indeed, encouraging more variability actually facilitates greater improvements in some forms of locomotor rehabilitation. Thus, it is critical to identify the fundamental principles humans use to regulate stride-to-stride fluctuations in walking. This study sought to determine how humans regulate stride-to-stride fluctuations in stepping movements during treadmill walking. We developed computational models based on pre-defined goal functions to compare if subjects, from each stride to the next, tried to maintain the same speed as the treadmill, or instead stay in the same position on the treadmill. Both strategies predicted average behaviors empirically indistinguishable from each other and from that of humans. These strategies, however, predicted very different stride-to-stride fluctuation dynamics. Comparisons to experimental data showed that human stepping movements were generally well-predicted by the speed-control model, but not by the position-control model. Human subjects also exhibited no indications they corrected deviations in absolute position only intermittently: i.e., closer to the boundaries of the treadmill. Thus, humans clearly do not adopt a control strategy whose primary goal is to maintain some constant absolute position on the treadmill. Instead, humans appear to regulate their stepping movements in a way most consistent with a strategy whose primary goal is to try to maintain the same speed as the treadmill at each consecutive stride. These findings have important implications both for understanding how biological systems regulate walking in general and for being able to harness these mechanisms to develop more effective rehabilitation interventions to improve locomotor performance.

Introduction

When humans or animals walk, they have to perform a very complex task, especially when walking in unpredictable environments [1,2]. The neural systems that regulate walking [3,4] must continually integrate multiple sensory inputs and coordinate motor outputs to numerous actuators to achieve efficient, stable, and adaptable locomotion. Both external disturbances [5,6] and internal physiological noise [7,8] make every step we take slightly different and thus contribute to the variability observed in walking movements. However, the underlying meaning and functional implications of this variability are not clear. On the one hand, increased intrinsic variability of some gait variables predicts an increased fall risk in the elderly [9,10]. Conversely, allowing or even purposefully imposing increased movement variability during robotic gait re-training can facilitate improved locomotor outcomes [1113] during rehabilitation. So whereas in some contexts, some forms of locomotor variability may be detrimental, in other contexts, variability has been shown to be beneficial. It is therefore critical to understand how the nervous system regulates such fluctuations when performing complex adaptive movements [14,15] like walking [16]. Doing so will yield important insights into the fundamental principles that govern these processes, which can in turn inform the development of more effective rehabilitation interventions.

Fluctuations in repeated human movements arise from external disturbances, but also from the physiological noise and redundancy that are inherent in biological systems. First, there are multiple sensory and motor sources of physiological noise [8,17]. Second, there are many more mechanical degrees of freedom than required to execute a single movement, many more muscles than required to actuate each degree of freedom, and so on. Redundancy makes biological systems inherently under-constrained and this gives rise to equifinality [15], where an infinite number of movement solutions all equally satisfy some task goal [1820]. In engineering, both noise and redundancy are most often viewed as disruptive elements to be overcome. It is widely believed humans thus seek to find unique solutions that are “optimal” according to some criterion [21,22]. These optimization approaches, however, have mainly focused on predicting average behavior, not on explaining the variability ubiquitously observed in movements like walking [23,24]. Conversely, mounting evidence suggests that inherent biological noise may benefit function, and in some instances may even be necessary to achieve optimal function [14,2527]. Likewise, redundancy and equifinality may help facilitate adaptability in motor performance [15,19,28,29]. Thus, understanding the interplay between variability, redundancy, and task performance is critical to understanding how humans perform skilled movements.

When humans walk, they need to adapt at every step (not just on average) to respond to externally and/or internally generated perturbations [30]. Both these perturbations themselves and the neuro-mechanical responses to them contribute to stride-to-stride variability observed in our movements [23,31]. Thus, the dynamical fluctuations observed in locomotor behavior likely reflect more than just simple random noise [30,32] and changes in the nature of these fluctuations can provide insights into neuromuscular control [31,33]. In particular, measures of variability that average deviations over many cycles cannot capture the corrective responses made from each cycle to the next. In computational models where the variables being controlled and the degree to which they are controlled (e.g., via gains) are specified a priori, imposing stochastically optimal control of a particular variable leads to uncorrelated fluctuations in the time series of that variable [16,20,34]. Conversely, under-correcting for deviations in a given variable leads to statistical persistence, whereas over-correcting leads to statistical anti-persistence [16,20,34]. Thus, differences in the correlation structure between different system outputs can yield direct insights into the degree to which each variable is controlled, independent of the variability exhibited by those variables.

During unconstrained overground walking, fluctuations in stride length (Ln), time (Tn), and speed (Sn) all exhibit strong statistical persistence [35], indicating relatively weak control [16,34] of all three parameters. When those same subjects walked in time with a metronome, their Tn became anti-persistent, but their Ln and Sn remained strongly persistent [35], indicating strong control over only Tn, as expected. When walking on a treadmill at fixed speed, humans exhibit slight anti-persistence of stride speed (Sn), but strong persistence for both Ln and Tn [16]. However, when walking on a treadmill in time with a metronome, humans tightly regulate all three primary gait variables [36], consistent with the way in which these variables are coupled (i.e., Sn = Ln/Tn) [16]. These experimental observations are all consistent with our computational framework and with our interpretation of how the fluctuation dynamics of these time series are related to control [16,20,34]. However, they do not by themselves directly rule out other possible alternatives.

While experiments are critical, conclusions drawn from experimental observations alone are often difficult to interpret in terms of underlying physiological processes. Deciphering the origins and implications of stride-to-stride fluctuations in human walking requires a comprehensive computational framework that can generate concrete, experimentally testable a priori hypotheses [20]. Building on just such a framework [14,15], we previously formulated goal functions [20] to give concrete mathematical form to hypotheses regarding the strategies humans might use to walk on a motor-driven treadmill [16]. Here, we extend that work by directly testing competing a priori hypotheses. In particular, in the sagittal plane, walking on a motor-driven treadmill only requires that subjects not “walk off” either its front or back end. Thus, over time, subjects must stay in the same average position (i.e., roughly “in the middle” of the treadmill), and must walk at the same average speed as the treadmill [16]. However, substantial fluctuations in both position and walking speed arise due to stride-to-stride changes in stride lengths and/or times, and these fluctuations can be sustained over multiple consecutive strides [16,23]. The question addressed here is how do people regulate these variations from each stride to the next?

One possible strategy humans could try is to match, at each stride, the same constant speed as the treadmill [16]. A distinct alternative strategy that has not previously been carefully considered is to try, at each stride, to remain in the same absolute position on the treadmill. Clearly, if the treadmill is operating at constant speed, then over time, trying to walk at the same speed as the treadmill will necessarily result in remaining in the same average absolute position. Likewise, trying to stay in the same position on the treadmill will necessarily result in walking at the same average speed as the treadmill. Thus, it is not at all clear how, or even if, these two strategies are fundamentally different. Indeed, traditional optimal control approaches will predict the exact same average movement behavior for both strategies and will therefore be inherently unable to distinguish these alternatives. Here, we hypothesize that at the level of stride-to-stride fluctuations, where control is actually exerted by humans, these two strategies are in fact quite different. We embed these two different strategies in a computational modeling framework that allows us to explicitly predict, a priori, what stride-to-stride fluctuation dynamics should result in each case. We then tested those predictions against experimental data from humans.

Materials and Methods

Treadmill Walking Defined

Fundamentally, the task of walking is about moving through one’s environment. To achieve forward motion, a bipedal walker (e.g., human, robot, etc.) must move a finite distance in a finite time at each consecutive stride. Thus, the most fundamental and natural observable quantities for walking are stride length (Ln) and stride time (Tn) at each stride n, together with the absolute position (Pn) they generate. When walking on a motor-driven treadmill operating at constant speed v, the walker’s displacement (i.e., change in position) relative to the inertial laboratory reference frame at each stride is ΔPn = LnvTn. Thus, one can typically reduce the description of walking to two independent observables, such as [Tn, Ln] [16]. Equivalent descriptions of walking dynamics using similar and/or related variables have been used for many years [37,38]. From the perspective of dynamical systems theory, these fundamental stride-to-stride observables arise as a projection of the full neuro-biomechanical system into the [Tn, Ln] plane of an impact Poincaré section [39] defined by consecutive heel strikes [16].

For sagittal plane motion (i.e., forward progression), the primary requirement for walking on any treadmill is to simply not walk off of the belt. That is, assuming (without loss of generality) that the walker starts at the center of the treadmill (i.e., P0 = 0), at each subsequent consecutive stride, n, we require: (1) where LTM is the length of the treadmill belt whose midpoint position is zero (Fig 1A). This summation of all displacements across all previous consecutive strides thus defines the walker’s current absolute position at stride n and must be satisfied for all n ∊ {1,…,N} strides walked.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Regulating Stride Length (Ln) and Stride Time (Tn) When Walking on a Treadmill.

(A) Schematic figure of a person walking on a treadmill of total length LTM with the center position defined as zero. The only strict requirement of the task is that the person not walk off either the front (+LTM/2) or back (−LTM /2) end of the treadmill (Eq 1). (B) Example data for stride times (Tn) and stride lengths (Ln) for a typical trial for a typical subject. Each dot represents the particular [Tn, Ln] combination for one individual stride, n. The solid diagonal line indicates the set of all combinations of Ln and Tn that achieve the exact same speed, v. This line defines one possible Goal Equivalent Manifold (GEM) for walking (Eq 2): i.e., for walking while trying to achieve the goal of maintaining constant speed. Unit vectors then define directions perpendicular to (êP) and tangent to (êT) the GEM. Deviations δP and δT define the deviations of each data point in the êP and êT directions, respectively (see Ref. [16]).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124879.g001

Goal-Equivalent Strategies for Treadmill Walking

A key observation is that any sequence of Ln and Tn that satisfies Eq (1) will successfully accomplish the treadmill walking task. Many possible strategies for generating such a sequence exist [16]. Here, we consider two simple and intuitive candidate strategies, which we formulate as goal functions [15], F(Tn,Ln). First, one could choose Ln and Tn at each step to try to maintain the same constant speed as the treadmill. This strategy yields the following goal function: (2)

Alternatively, one could choose Ln and Tn at each step to try to maintain some constant absolute position on the treadmill. Assuming (without loss of generality) the desired position to be maintained is at the origin (i.e., 0 in Fig 1A), this yields the following goal function: (3)

Defining treadmill walking this way makes it clear that task performance is completely determined by the observables Ln and Tn, independent of the detailed dynamics for all other neuromotor and/or biomechanical degrees of freedom. Upon choosing either strategy, the walker’s task is then to drive the corresponding goal function as close to zero as possible, given inherent system noise. Any value of F(Tn,Ln) ≠ 0 represents a deviation from perfect performance for either goal. In both cases, the set of all possible combinations of [Tn, Ln] for which F(Tn,Ln) = 0 defines the corresponding Goal Equivalent Manifold (GEM) [15] for that strategy (e.g., Fig 1B). Critically, neither goal function represents a “constraint” on the dynamics, because neither condition represented by Eq (2) or (3) is required by Eq (1). Instead, these represent merely two of many possible movement strategies that could exist [16], including a variety of possible stochastic (i.e. “drunken”) gaits.

Importantly, the long-time average behaviors of these two candidate movement strategies are empirically indistinguishable. Also, each hypothesized GEM exists prior to, and independent of, any specific control policy people might adopt to regulate their stepping movements [15,16]. Thus, the question then becomes: in what way(s), if any, are these two strategies dynamically distinct? Answering this question requires predicting the stride-to-stride fluctuation dynamics that should arise from implementing either candidate walking strategy using a suitable controller.

Modelling Stride-to-Stride Regulation

We developed specific stochastic control models that directly implemented each strategy described above. Our intent was to develop phenomenological models that are as simple as possible, but yet still capture the key relevant features of stride-to-stride dynamics. The process for regulating stride-to-stride walking dynamics on the treadmill was modeled as a discrete map, written in the general form of an update equation: (4) where u(xn) is a vector of inputs from an optimal inter-stride controller (to be determined), xn is a suitable controller state variable for current stride n, and xn+1 is the corresponding state for the subsequent stride. Also in the above, I is the identity matrix, G is a diagonal matrix denoting additional gains, used as a convenient means to detune the system away from optimality [16], Nn is a diagonal multiplicative (i.e., motor output) noise matrix, and ηn is a vector representing sensory, perceptual, motor, or other noise sources arising from unmodeled aspects of the overall complex biodynamics of the system.

Operating at the level of experimental observables, we take the inter-trial controller state variable to be xn = [Tn,Ln]. This choice is motivated by the fact that the treadmill walking task is completely specified by conditions on Tn and Ln (Eqs 13): any controller will have to correct any value of [Tn, Ln] that deviates from the GEM: i.e., that does not satisfy F(Tn,Ln) = 0. We can thus think of Eq (4) as the “top level” of a hierarchical controller that makes adjustments between strides. The “bottom level” controller is implicit: it is the intra-stride component that acts to generate each Ln and Tn during each stride (e.g., [22,40]). In this sense, the inter-stride controller states [Tn, Ln] act as parameters specifying boundary conditions that any suitable intra-trial controller must match. In the absence of noise (ηn = 0 in Eq 4), additional control is unnecessary (u(xn) = 0), and the stride-to-stride dynamics in an impact Poincaré section for the complete system exhibit a fixed point where successive strides simply repeat: xn+1 = xn. Thus, Eq (4) captures the essential notion of a central pattern generator process [41,42] that yields repetitive limit cycle behavior [16,22].

The dimensionality of the intra-stride system is very large, including many more state variables (e.g., [40,42,43]) than needed to capture the passive mechanical aspects of gait [5,22,38,44]. Indeed, the dimension of task-relevant neuromotor and perceptual states could range from the hundreds to billions or more, depending on the scale of description. This very high-dimensional aspect of biological systems is indeed the fundamental endogenous source of the fluctuations observed in human movements. Thus, Ln and Tn are analogous to thermodynamic observables in statistical mechanics [45]: they are “macro-states” that represent the output of a complex system, each specific value of which can arise from a vast number of different “micro-states”. From this perspective, Eq (4) is similar to the discrete Langevin Eq in the study of Brownian motion [45]: it is a low-dimensional model of “macroscopic” dynamics driven by a noise term representing the effect of a vast number of internal, “microscopic” degrees-of-freedom.

Maintaining Constant Walking Speed

To implement a strategy that tries to maintain constant walking speed at each step, we note Eq 4 can be reduced to [16]: (5) where the gain and noise matrices from Eq 4 become in which the ε represent independent Gaussian random variables with zero mean and unit variance and the σ give the corresponding standard deviations of each. For this model, the error relative to the GEM, which is to be regulated, is taken directly from the relevant goal function (Eq 2):

(6)

Maintaining Constant Absolute Position

To implement a strategy that tries to maintain constant position on the treadmill at each step requires that we add a 3rd state variable to track the absolute position (i.e., net total displacement) on the treadmill: (7) where is the walker’s current absolute position at stride n, equivalently defined as net cumulative distance walked up until that stride. Note that no control is applied directly to Pn itself. While all three controller states are “observable” to human subjects, only Tn and Ln are “controllable”. The error relative to the GEM that is to be regulated for this model is then also taken directly from its relevant goal function (Eq 3):

(8)

Stochastic Optimal Control

The controller was modeled as an unbiased stochastic optimal single-step controller with direct error feedback, based on the Minimum Intervention Principle (MIP) [14,18], but modified to include the cost of deviating from a “preferred operating point”, [T*, L*], along the GEM [16]. This additional cost is added because perfect MIP control is based only on task error (Eq 6 and 8): it does not consider that human legs have finite length, or inertia, nor that humans adopt a combination of stride length and stride time that tends to minimize energetic cost on average [22,37,38]. Here, we set [T*, L*] to be the mean stride time and stride length, respectively [16]. Accordingly, we took the cost function to be the expected value of: (9)

The first term, αe2, depends on the definition of the goal-level error for the task [15]: i.e., either e = eSpd (Eq 6) for maintaining constant speed, or e = ePos (Eq 8) for maintaining constant position. Cost functions of the same form can in principle be used to test any other hypothesized task strategy formulated using a different candidate goal function. The second term in Eq (9), βp2, penalizes the Euclidean distance, p, of the state at each stride from the preferred operating point, [T*, L*] [16]. The costs e and p are both evaluated at the next stride, n+1, whereas the last two effort control terms in Eq (9), involving the control input u = [u1, u2], are evaluated at stride n. The positive constants α, β, γ, and δ are weights for the different costs in C.

We derived the control inputs u1 and u2 by solving a classic quadratic optimal control problem with an equality constraint [16,46]. The controllers were chosen to be optimal with respect to the expected value of Eq (9), . We further required u1 and u2 to be single step, unbiased controllers that sought to drive each respective goal function (either Eq 6 or Eq 8) to zero at each stride. This process yielded optimal control inputs obtained analytically as functions of the current controller state, xn (see Ref. [16] for additional details). For the speed controller (i.e., for state update Eq 5, with goal function specified by Eq 6), we obtained: (10)

Applying the same procedure to the position controller (i.e., for state update Eq 7, with the goal function specified by Eq 8), the resulting controller inputs have the form wi = ui + ũi, where the ui are the same controllers derived previously (i.e., Eq 10) but now with the addition of the following terms: (11)

These models illustrate that maintaining speed and position are directly related. Indeed, controlling speed is a special case of controlling position. Likewise, because Pn is observable but not directly controllable, controlling position inherently requires that we also control corresponding fluctuations in speed.

Simulated Walking Data

We generated simulated walking data for two versions of each of the two models (speed control and position control). First, to implement the strictly optimal MIP-based control models (“SMIP” and “PMIP”), we set β = 0 in Eq 9, so the cost function depended only on the goal-level error, e, and the effort terms, u1 and u2. We then also set the gain matrix in the stride map (Eq 4) to identity, G = I (i.e., g1 = g2 = 1 in Eq 5 or 7) [16]. Thus, these strictly optimal MIP controllers optimally corrected for deviations only with respect to each specific goal function (Eq 6 or Eq 8) implemented.

Second, we previously found that human walking dynamics were best described by a controller that included a preferred operating point that was also sub-optimal in that it slightly over-corrected errors with respect to the GEM [16]. To implement these over-correcting control models (“SOVC” and “POVC”), we set β = 2.79 in Eq (9) and we increase the controller gains to g1 = g2 = 1.24 in Eq (5) or (7), respectively [16]. Thus, these sub-optimal controllers tended to correct very strongly (indeed, slightly more than needed) for deviations away from each respective GEM and weakly for deviations along them.

All models were implemented assuming a walking speed of v = 1.21 m/s, which corresponded to the mean preferred walking speed of our human subjects (see below). We then chose the remaining parameter values by trial-and-error to approximate the experimentally observed stride speed variability. Thus, N and η were defined using σN1 = ση1 = 0.017 and σN2 = ση2 = 0.010. The remaining weights in the cost function (Eq 9) were set to α = γ = δ = 10. For the sub-optimal control models, we used T* = 1.105 s to correspond to the aggregate mean stride time of all human subjects and L* = vT*, where again v = 1.21 m/s. (See [16] for additional details). The primary qualitative aspects of our results were not sensitive to the particular choices of these parameters and no explicit attempts were made to rigorously “fit” our experimental data exactly.

For each of the four model configurations (SMIP, PMIP, SOVC, and POVC), we simulated 20 trials of walking of 1000 strides each to represent a single simulated “average” subject. Model outputs consisted of time series of stride times (Tn) and stride lengths (Ln). For each simulation, we computed the net cumulative distances walked at each step, to ensure no simulation “walked off” the treadmill [16].

Experimental Participants

The experimental data used here were drawn from the same data set as analyzed in [16] and are available from Dryad (http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.sk55m) [47]. Seventeen young healthy adults participated (Table 1). No participants reported any history of orthopedic problems, recent lower extremity injuries, any visible gait anomalies, or were taking medications that may have influenced their gait. Analyses of the variability [24], dynamic stability [31], and stepping dynamics [16] of these participants were reported previously.

Ethics Statement

All procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Texas at Austin. All participants signed approved written informed consent forms prior to participation.

Experimental Procedures

Subjects walked on a level motor-driven treadmill (Desmo S model, Woodway USA, Waukesha WI) while wearing comfortable walking shoes and a safety harness (Protecta International, Houston TX) that allowed natural arm swing [31]. Preferred self-selected walking speed (PWS) was determined using a standardized protocol [23]. Following a 2-minute rest, subjects completed two 5-minute walking trials at each of five speeds, presented in pseudo-random order. Subjects rested at least 2 minutes between each trial to prevent fatigue. Subjects were instructed to look ahead and avoid extraneous movements while walking. Because differences across walking speeds were described in detail elsewhere [24,31] and were minimal for analyses similar to those conducted here [16], only the data from the preferred walking speeds (PWS) were analyzed here. Data from 1 PWS trial from each of 2 subjects had to be discarded due to poor data quality.

Kinematic data regarding the movements of each foot were recorded at 60 Hz continuously for each entire 5 minute trial using an 8-camera Vicon 612 motion capture system (Oxford Metrics, UK) using methods previously described [24,31]. Raw marker data were then resampled to 1200 Hz to obtain more precise estimates of individual gait events. Stride times (Tn) were computed for each stride, n, as the time from one heel contact to the next ipsilateral heel contact. Step length was defined as the anterior-posterior distance between the heel and the contralateral heel at each heel contact, when both feet were in contact with the treadmill belt. Stride lengths (Ln) were computed as the sum of each pair of 2 consecutive step lengths that composed each stride. The 32 experimental trials analyzed here included an average of 275±17 (mean ± standard deviation) total strides each (range: 242–309).

Data Analyses

The primary gait variables obtained from each simulated or experimental walking trial consisted of time series of Ln and Tn for all strides within that trial. Time series of stride speeds (Sn) and absolute positions (Pn) for all strides for each trial were also calculated. Time series of stride speeds were calculated as: (12)

For each subject, average walking speed was computed as the average stride speed, , computed across all N strides within each trial. Absolute positions relative to the treadmill were computed for each stride by summing the stride-to-stride displacements over all previous consecutive strides: (13)

All analyses were then applied to these four time series for each simulated and experimental trial.

First, we computed the mean and standard deviation of each time series (Ln, Tn, Sn, and Pn). Of primary interest were the dynamics of those fluctuations perpendicular to each respective GEM (e.g., the “δP” deviations indicated in Fig 1B). For any GEM, the corresponding δP deviations are directly “goal relevant” precisely because they indicate errors with respect to the defined task goal. Here, for the constant speed task, fluctuations in Sn directly reflect δP deviations away from the speed GEM (Fig 1B; [16]). For the constant position task, fluctuations in Pn directly reflect δP deviations away from the position GEM.

Standard deviations only quantify the average magnitude of differences across all strides, regardless of temporal order, and thus yield no information about how each stride affects subsequent strides. Therefore, Detrended Fluctuation Analysis (DFA) [32,48,49] was used to define a lag-independent measure of statistical persistence across successive strides in each time series [16,34]. In brief, DFA calculates a scaling exponent α. Values of α > ½ indicate persistence [32,48]: deviations in one direction are more likely to be followed by deviations in the same direction. Values of α < ½ indicate anti-persistence: deviations in one direction are more likely to be followed by deviations in the opposite direction. A value of α = ½ indicates uncorrelated (i.e., white) noise. A value of α = 1½ indicates brown noise (i.e., integrated white noise) [32,48], equivalent to Brownian motion. In the context of control, variables not tightly regulated [16,34] typically exhibit stronger statistical persistence (α >> ½), whereas more tightly regulated variables typically exhibit fluctuations with α ≈ ½ [16,34]. Here, we predicted that trying to maintain approximately constant speed would yield α(Sn) very close to ½ and α(Pn) very close to 1½ (since displacement is the integral of velocity). Conversely, we predicted that trying to maintain approximately constant position would yield α(Pn) very close to ½ and hence α(Sn) << ½. Lastly, we predicted humans would exhibit fluctuation dynamics consistent with speed control [16], but contradictory to position control, even though both strategies predict the same overall average behavior.

However, DFA analyses only capture the degree to which, on average, deviations were corrected in time (i.e., from stride-to-stride). They do not provide explicit information regarding how fluctuations depend on the controller state. A controller trying to maintain some constant average position, , on the treadmill (Eq 3), when observing any deviation away from on a given stride (i.e., ), should correct that deviation on the subsequent stride by making a corresponding change in position, ΔPn+1 = Pn+1Pn, in the opposite direction. Likewise, a controller trying to maintain some constant average speed, (Eq 2), when observing any deviation , should correct that deviation by a subsequent change in speed, ΔSn+1 = Sn+1Sn, in the opposite direction. We therefore constructed, for each controller and for our human subjects, plots of ΔPn+1 vs. and ΔSn+1 vs. , and computed the linear slopes (using least-squares regression) and strength of correlation (r2) for each corresponding relationship. We predicted that position control would yield slopes close to −1 for ΔPn+1 vs. with high correlation, whereas speed control would yield slopes close to −1 for ΔSn+1 vs. with high correlation, but slopes close to 0 with very low correlation for ΔPn+1 vs. . We predicted humans would exhibit stepping corrections consistent with speed control [16], but contradictory to position control.

Results

On average, all four model formulations predicted mean stride behavior essentially indistinguishable from that of humans (Fig 2). By construction, mean stride speeds (Sn) for each model matched the mean of the experimental results (Fig 2C). For the sub-optimal over-correcting models (POVC and SOVC), this also yielded very consistent mean stride lengths (Ln; Fig 2A) and stride times (Tn; Fig 2B). The pure MIP models (PMIP and SMIP) exhibited much greater between-trial variance in the mean values of Ln and Tn. This occurred because there was no “preferred” operating point defined (i.e., β = 0 in Eq 9) and therefore no cost associated with moving away from any particular operating point along the GEM. Fluctuations in Pn were already normalized to each trial’s own mean position, so mean Pn values (not shown) were all zero by construction.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Means of Basic Gait Variables.

(A) Stride Lengths (Ln), (B) Stride Times (Tn), and (C) Stride Speeds (Sn = Ln/Tn). In each sub-plot, data shown are for healthy human subjects (HU), the Position Control model with MIP control (PMIP), the Position Control model with “over-correcting” control (POVC), the Speed Control model with MIP control (SMIP), and the Speed Control model with “over-correcting” control (SOVC). For the HU data, error bars indicate between-subject ± standard deviations. For the model data, error bars indicate between-trial ± standard deviations. All models yielded mean stride parameters well within the experimental range. However, both MIP controllers (PMIP and SMIP) yielded much greater between-trial variance for both Ln (A) and Tn (B) than observed experimentally.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124879.g002

Both pure MIP models (PMIP and SMIP) also predicted much greater within-trial stride-to-stride variability (Fig 3A) and statistical persistence (Fig 3B) for both Ln and Tn than humans exhibited. Both pure MIP control models were thus substantially qualitatively different from humans. Conversely, in terms of Ln and Tn, both sub-optimal models, whether enacting speed control (SOVC) or position control (POVC), predicted both the variances (Fig 3A) and statistical persistence (Fig 3B) exhibited by humans equally well.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Variability (σ) and Statistical Persistence (α) of Stride Lengths (Ln) and Stride Times (Tn).

(A) Variability (within-trial standard deviations (σ) of humans (HU) and of each of the four model configurations (PMIP, POVC, SMIP, and SOVC). (B) DFA scaling exponents (α) of humans (HU) and of each of the four model configurations (PMIP, POVC, SMIP, and SOVC). For the HU data, error bars indicate between-subject ± standard deviations. For the model data, error bars indicate between-trial ± standard deviations. Both models that implemented pure MIP-type control (i.e., PMIP and SMIP), regardless of whether controlling for speed or position, yielded much higher levels of variance (A) and much greater degrees of statistical persistence (indeed, approaching random walk behavior: α ≈ 1.5) (B) in the typical walking parameters that are not directly controlled for (i.e., Ln and Tn) than did humans (HU).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124879.g003

Qualitatively, all four model formulations exhibited fluctuations in stride speeds (Sn) that appeared largely similar to those of humans (Fig 4). However, fluctuations in absolute positions (Pn) for both humans and for each speed control model (SMIP and SOVC) exhibited large-amplitude, low-frequency behavior, consistent with subjects “drifting” forward and backward on the treadmill over time [16]. Conversely, fluctuations in Pn for both position control models (PMIP and POVC) were very narrowly centered near Pn = 0 (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Example Time Series of Stride Speeds (Sn) and Absolute Position (Pn).

Data are shown for 240 consecutive strides from a typical trial for a typical human subject (HU) and for one typical trial each of each of the four model configurations (PMIP, POVC, SMIP, and SOVC). All four model configurations yielded Sn time series that initially appeared qualitatively similar both to each other and also to humans (HU). Conversely, both of the Position Control models (PMIP and POVC) qualitatively exhibited far less variance and far less “drift” in their absolute positions on the treadmill (Pn) than did humans (HU).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124879.g004

These qualitative observations were confirmed by our analyses (Fig 5). Both position control models (PMIP and POVC) exhibited slightly greater variability for Sn, but far less variability for Pn, than did humans (Fig 5A). Conversely, the speed control models (SMIP and SOVC) exhibited variability very similar to humans for Sn, but greater variability for Pn (Fig 5A). Most importantly, however, both speed control models (SMIP and SOVC) exhibited fluctuations in stride speed (Sn) that were either very near to, or slightly less than, α = ½, respectively (Fig 5B). This then yielded fluctuations in absolute position (Pn) that closely approximated Brownian motion (Fig 5B), as predicted. Conversely, both position control models (PMIP and POVC) exhibited fluctuations in absolute position (Pn) that were either very near to, or slightly less than, α = ½, respectively (Fig 5B). This then yielded fluctuations in speed (Sn) that exhibited extremely small α << ½ (Fig 5B). The fluctuation dynamics for Sn and Pn for both of the two speed control models closely approximated those of humans (Fig 5B). However, both position control models were qualitatively very different from humans.

thumbnail
Fig 5. Variability (σ) and Statistical Persistence (α) of Stride Speeds (Sn) and Absolute Positions (Pn).

(A) Variability (within-trial standard deviations (σ) of humans (HU) and of each of the four model configurations (PMIP, POVC, SMIP, and SOVC). (B) DFA scaling exponents (α) of humans (HU) and of each of the four model configurations (PMIP, POVC, SMIP, and SOVC). For the HU data, error bars indicate between-subject ± standard deviations. For the model data, error bars indicate between-trial ± standard deviations. Position control yielded uncorrelated fluctuations (α ≈ 0.5) of absolute positions (Pn) under optimal MIP control conditions (PMIP) and slightly anti-correlated fluctuations (α < 0.5) of Pn when over-correcting for position (POVC). This in turn yielded highly anti-correlated fluctuations (α < 0.1) of stride speeds (Sn). Speed control yielded uncorrelated fluctuations (α ≈ 0.5) of stride speeds (Sn) under optimal MIP control conditions (SMIP) and slightly anti-correlated fluctuations (α < 0.5) of Sn when over-correcting for position (SOVC). This in turn yielded strongly correlated fluctuations for absolute position (Pn), which approached random walk behavior: α ≈ 1.5), consistent with position reflecting integrated speed (Eq 1, 3, and 13). Both of the position control models (PMIP and POVC) exhibited fluctuation dynamics very different from those of humans (HU). The over-correcting speed control model (SOVC) exhibited fluctuation dynamics most consistent with humans.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124879.g005

Both position control models corrected deviations in absolute position, , by enacting opposing changes in position, ΔPn+1, on subsequent strides (Fig 6A). These models predicted slopes of ΔPn+1 vs. of ~ −1 for optimal (PMIP) and < −1 for sub-optimal (POVC) control (Fig 6B), with both exhibiting strong correlations (Fig 6C). As expected, both position control models also exhibited strong over-corrections for changes in speed (Fig 6D–6F). Conversely, the speed control models predicted slopes of ΔSn+1 vs. of ~ −1 for optimal (SMIP; Fig 6D–6E) and slightly < −1 for sub-optimal (SOVC) control (Fig 6E), with both exhibiting strong correlations (Fig 6F). These speed control models, however, predicted essentially no corrections for deviations in relative position (Fig 6A–6C). Humans exhibited slopes of slightly < −1 for ΔSn+1 vs. (Fig 6D–6E) with strong correlations (Fig 6F), but slopes of ~ 0 for ΔPn+1 vs. (Fig 6A–6B), with nearly zero correlations (Fig 6C). These experimental results were highly consistent with predictions of the speed control models, but substantially qualitatively different from predictions of either position control model.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Direct Correction of Errors in Position and Speed.

(A) Exemplary plots of how errors in relative position were corrected on each subsequent stride (ΔPn+1 = Pn+1Pn). Data are shown from 1 trial each for each of the two sub-optimal control model configurations (POVC and SOVC) and for 1 typical human subject. Diagonal lines indicate least-squares fits to each data set. “Perfect” error correction would yield a relationship with a slope of −1 and a strong correlation. (B) Summary results for the slopes of these relationships for all trials for both humans (HU) and for each of the four model configurations (PMIP, POVC, SMIP, and SOVC). For the HU data, error bars indicate between-subject ± standard deviations. For the model data, error bars indicate between-trial ± standard deviations. (C) Summary results for the strength of correlation (r2) of these relationships [error bars are defined in the same manner as in part (B)]. While both position control models (PMIP and POVC) exhibited steep slopes (B) with strong correlations (C), both humans and both speed control models (SMIP and SOVC) exhibited nearly zero slopes with extremely weak correlations, consistent with not correcting deviations in position. (D) Exemplary plots of how errors in relative speed were corrected on each subsequent stride (ΔSn+1 = Sn+1Sn). Data are shown in the same manner as in (A). As in (A), “perfect” error correction would yield a relationship with a slope of −1 and a strong correlation. (E) Summary results for the slopes of these relationships for all trials for both humans (HU) and for each of the four model configurations (PMIP, POVC, SMIP, and SOVC) [error bars are defined in the same manner as in part (B)]. (F) Summary results for the strength of correlation (r2) of these relationships [error bars are defined in the same manner as in part (B)]. Human subjects exhibited nearly perfect corrections (i.e., slope ≈ −1: (E)) for errors in speed with strong correlations (F), as did both of the speed control models (SMIP and SOVC).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0124879.g006

Discussion

Increased locomotor variability can lead to detrimental consequences like increased energetic cost [50] or increased fall risk [10]. Conversely, exploiting redundancy can facilitate motor performance [19,28] and may help explain why encouraging variability accelerates improvements in robot-assisted gait re-training [11,12]. Here, we adopted a model-based approach that allowed us to test concrete a priori hypotheses [14,16,20] to determine how humans regulate sagittal plane stepping movements as they walk on a motor-driven treadmill. We defined explicit goal functions [15,20] to compare two candidate control strategies: i.e., regulating stride-to-stride speed vs. absolute position. Both strategies are equally plausible and yielded average behavior indistinguishable both from each other and from humans (Fig 2). However, these strategies predicted very different patterns of stride-to-stride fluctuation dynamics in the relevant variables (Figs 36). Equivalent analyses of experimental data yielded results well-predicted by the speed-control model (SOVC), but not by either position-control model (PMIP or POVC). These simulation results are consistent with the notion that humans do not try to stay in the same absolute position on the treadmill at each stride, but instead try, at least primarily, to maintain the same speed as the treadmill. This work directly extends a computational framework developed for studying trial-to-trial fluctuation dynamics in repetitive human movements [15,20], including walking [16]. Our method adapts approaches independently developed using time series analyses [30,48], redundancy [15], and feedback control [18] and unifies these ideas under a coherent theoretical framework [20]. In contrast to approaches that seek to identify single “optimal” average solutions [21,22], or that only analyze “variability” [7,14], the present results clearly demonstrate that one must account for trial-to-trial (here, stride-to-stride) fluctuation dynamics [28,29,51] to identify appropriate control policies.

In theory, there are numerous ways humans might control their stepping movements, since any strategy that satisfies Eq. (1) could be allowed. Several plausible alternatives beyond those presented here were previously discussed in [16]. Yet another alternative the present work suggests might be to adopt “lazy” position control. Participants could allow their position to drift uncorrected anywhere within the middle of the treadmill belt and only correct position errors when they walked “too close” to either end of the treadmill belt (e.g., Fig 1A). Effectively: if you are too far forward, move back; if you are too far back, move forward. Indeed, such a strategy, a form of deadband control for position, would exemplify the Minimum Intervention Principle [14,18]. The analyses in Fig 6 address this possibility. Humans adopting such a strategy would be expected to take shorter and/or slower strides at more forward positions (i.e. at large positive values of relative position, P′n) to enact larger negative changes in position (ΔPn+1). At more backward positions (i.e. at large negative values of P′n), participants would be expected to take longer and/or faster strides to enact larger positive ΔPn+1. For intermediate values of P′n, with such a deadband controller, participants would be expected to exhibit little or no correction for position errors (i.e., ΔPn+1 ≈ 0). Together, the overall result would be a distribution of data points, unlike those in Fig 6A, that would be flat in the middle, but would curve sharply up on the left and sharply down on the right. However, no such nonlinearity was observed (Fig 6A; Human). Indeed, human subjects exhibited a relationship between P′n and ΔPn+1 that was in fact quite flat (i.e., ΔPn+1 ≈ 0) across the entire range of positions explored by all subjects tested.

Similarly, Wang and Srinivasan [52] used experimental data of current foot and pelvis states to predict subsequent anterior and lateral foot placements. Adding current position on the treadmill as an additional predictor in their regression models explained little to no (< 5%) additional variance in foot placement. They also concluded that healthy humans do not try to maintain absolute position on the treadmill (what they called “station-keeping”). Their participants walked on treadmills shorter (1.27 or 1.52 m long) than that used here (1.73 m long), demonstrating that healthy humans do not tightly regulate absolute position even when walking on shorter treadmills. Thus, while such an intermittent, boundary-driven, deadband position control policy might be a very rational and simple strategy to adopt, healthy humans do not use this type of control.

Likewise, the models derived here all employed single-stride direct error feedback control. However, there is no physiological necessity to fully correct movement errors over only a single stride. Another less restrictive approach would be to correct errors more slowly over multiple strides. Indeed, some have derived control policies for robots that regulate walking over many consecutive steps [53,54]. However, our purpose here was to identify the simplest possible model(s) that could capture the primary features of inter-stride fluctuations observed in our experiments. We therefore chose to start with k-step optimal controllers and our review of the literature suggested k = 1 should provide a sufficient first approximation. Indeed, mechanically stable walking can be maintained by implementing control only one step at a time [55,56]. Physiologically, the intrinsic timescales of walking (i.e., roughly 1000–1200 ms/stride ≈ 500–600 ms/step; Fig 2B) allow ample time for even slow reflex responses [4] to operate. In humans, single-step control strategies predict subsequent foot placements during both stepping [57,58] and walking [52,59] quite well. When humans are released from a forward lean [60] or perturbed while walking [61,62], they recover in ≤ 3 steps (i.e., ≤ 1½ strides) and can also modify their stepping responses within a single step (½ stride) [63]. Additionally, visual input is known to be essential for regulating walking [3,6466]. In multiple experiments where humans were asked to change direction [67,68], step onto specific targets [69,70], avoid obstacles in their path [64,71], walk across complex terrains [65,72,73], or navigate environments with large numbers of densely packed visual targets/obstacles [74,75], visual information about these targets, obstacles, and/or environments was acquired only 1–3 steps (i.e., ½-1½ strides) in advance. Regulating movements only a few steps at a time in this way is consistent with the fact that humans must navigate noisy environments using a noisy neuromotor system [8,27] that must contend with multiple forms and levels of uncertainty [76]. Such a situation makes predicting motor outcomes farther into the future impractical. The simple and parsimonious single-stride control models presented here adequately captured the primary fluctuation dynamics observed in our human subjects during unperturbed walking, consistent with this large body of experimental evidence. While the inter-stride dynamics of human walking may exhibit some features that, in some contexts, would require multi-stride controllers to explain, exploration of such models is left to future work.

The present findings have several important implications. First, increased walking variability has been broadly implicated in predicting increased risk of falling in the elderly [9,10]. However, there remains considerable debate as to which gait measures should be included [77]. The confusion on this very important topic likely arises from the absence of a clear framework for understanding where the variability in different gait measures comes from and what its underlying purpose is. Variability arising from uncontrolled noise could be detrimental [6,78], whereas variability that reflects corrective control responses and/or exploration of alternative viable movement strategies is likely beneficial [16,29,79]. The approach presented here provides a theoretical, analytical, and experimental framework within which these critical distinctions can be tested. By determining the appropriate goal functions for efficient and stable walking, we can better identify which candidate body-state variables should be targeted for developing effective therapeutic interventions. As a specific example, developing more rigorous and less heuristic approaches to identifying relevant goal functions could lead to more efficient implementations of so-called “assist-as-needed” or “patient-cooperative” robotic gait retraining programs [12,13,80]. Similarly, such approaches could also be used to guide development of minimally complex control algorithms (e.g., [78,81,82]) that can allow bipedal robots to navigate more complex terrains [5,6] far more efficiently.

The present work focused specifically on the issue of how stepping movements are controlled between-strides. This work itself does not address the question of how control is organized at a neurophysiological and/or biomechanical level to achieve the within-stride generation of each stepping movement [16]. In this sense, while our work does provide a low-dimensional “template” for identifying features critical to between-stride control, these template control strategies still need to be “anchored” [83,84] within more elaborate, higher-dimensional biological models to identify the underlying neural and/or biomechanical mechanisms through which these control processes are enacted. Likewise, the present work only addresses how stepping movements are controlled in the sagittal plane. Both bipedal walking robots [78,81] and humans [2] are inherently more unstable in the mediolateral direction. While the present work does not address this question, the framework developed here could be extended to also account for lateral stepping movements. Finally, both speed control and position control very easily satisfied the basic requirement (Eq 1) of staying on the treadmill. However, the present results (Figs 35) make it clear that, of the types of models tested, human treadmill walking behavior is far more consistent with adopting a strategy that strongly prioritizes speed control over position control. Nevertheless, these results by themselves do not directly answer the question of why humans chose to do this or how such control is implemented physiologically. Answering these important questions will require a combination of additional more focused experiments and/or developing more strongly “anchored” [83,84] biological models.

It is also important to point out that although we used a stochastic optimal control framework as a convenient computational means to construct our control models, this approach is not unique. We could alternatively have chosen to build equivalent controller models using neural networks, or genetic algorithms, or some other approach (although, it must be added, at the cost of substantially increased model complexity). Additionally, and more importantly, one must not conflate the methods used to construct these models with the underlying biological phenomena being studied. We do not claim that because our models capture the primary aspects of our experimental data, that the human brain solves the same stochastic optimal control problem or solves it in the same way. It almost certainly does not. However, whatever physiological mechanisms are at play to produce the end result (i.e., our experimental data), that end result must be at least consistent with the models constructed here. Identifying the physiological mechanisms used to enact these control policies should evolve as part of the process of developing those more strongly “anchored” [83,84] biological models.

Likewise, the present work focused specifically on identifying how stepping movements are regulated during treadmill walking. This raises the question of what these results imply about overground walking. Indeed, there has been much debate over the years about how treadmill and overground walking are related. In theory, for an idealized treadmill (i.e. rigid walking surface and constant belt speed), there are no fundamental mechanical differences between the two tasks [85]. However, while several studies reported significant differences [8689], others have reported minimal or no differences between the two tasks for a range of relevant dependent measures [86,9092]. It is tempting then to think that perhaps the present findings, showing that healthy humans strongly regulate stride speed during treadmill walking, suggest that humans likely also naturally regulate walking speed in the same way during overground walking. However, this cannot be the case. Indeed, walking speed varies quite considerably over multiple time scales during overground walking [93]. More importantly, while stride-to-stride fluctuations in stride speeds (Sn) exhibit slight anti-persistence during treadmill walking (Fig 5B) and while our models confirm this to be a direct result of trying to maintain approximately constant speed across strides, experimental data from overground walking exhibit very strong statistical persistence for Sn [35] that is quite different from treadmill walking. These findings suggest that human locomotor control is very flexible and adaptable to changing task conditions and task demands. The present work demonstrates that a comprehensive model-based and fundamentally dynamical approach, based on testing experimental data against established theoretical predictions, is needed to delineate the differences in stepping control between different locomotor tasks.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Hyun Gu Kang and Dr. Deanna H. Gates for their assistance with data collection and initial processing, Dr. Nicole K. Bohnsack-McLagan for her assistance with data analyses, and Dr. Joby John for his assistance with implementation of the models.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: JBD JPC. Performed the experiments: JBD. Analyzed the data: JBD JPC. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: JBD JPC. Wrote the paper: JBD JPC.

References

  1. 1. Gates DH, Wilken JM, Scott SJ, Sinitsky EH, Dingwell JB (2012) Kinematic Strategies for Walking Across a Destabilizing Rock Surface. Gait & Posture 35: 36–42.
  2. 2. McAndrew PM, Wilken JM, Dingwell JB (2011) Dynamic Stability of Human Walking in Visually and Mechanically Destabilizing Environments. Journal of Biomechanics 44: 644–649. pmid:21094944
  3. 3. Warren WH, Kay BA, Zosh WD, Duchon AP, Sahuc S (2001) Optic Flow is Used to Control Human Walking. Nature Neuroscience 4: 213–216. pmid:11175884
  4. 4. Rossignol S, Dubuc R, Gossard J-P (2006) Dynamic Sensorimotor Interactions in Locomotion. Physiological Reviews 86: 89–154. pmid:16371596
  5. 5. Su JL, Dingwell JB (2007) Dynamic Stability of Passive Dynamic Walking on an Irregular Surface. ASME Journal of Biomechanical Engineering 129: 802–810. pmid:18067383
  6. 6. Byl K, Tedrake R (2009) Metastable Walking Machines. The International Journal of Robotics Research 28: 1040–1064.
  7. 7. Harris CM, Wolpert DM (1998) Signal-Dependent Noise Determines Motor Planning. Nature 394: 780–784. pmid:9723616
  8. 8. Faisal AA, Selen LPJ, Wolpert DM (2008) Noise in the nervous system. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 292–303. pmid:18319728
  9. 9. Verghese J, Holtzer R, Lipton RB, Wang C (2009) Quantitative Gait Markers and Incident Fall Risk in Older Adults. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 64A: 896–901. pmid:19349593
  10. 10. Toebes MJP, Hoozemans MJM, Furrer R, Dekker J, van Dieën JH (2012) Local dynamic stability and variability of gait are associated with fall history in elderly subjects. Gait & Posture 36: 527–531.
  11. 11. Ziegler MD, Zhong H, Roy RR, Edgerton VR (2010) Why Variability Facilitates Spinal Learning. The Journal of Neuroscience 30: 10720–10726. pmid:20702702
  12. 12. Lewek MD, Cruz TH, Moore JL, Roth HR, Dhaher YY, Hornby TG (2009) Allowing Intralimb Kinematic Variability During Locomotor Training Poststroke Improves Kinematic Consistency: A Subgroup Analysis From a Randomized Clinical Trial. Physical Therapy 89: 829–839. pmid:19520734
  13. 13. Duschau-Wicke A, Caprez A, Riener R (2010) Patient-cooperative control increases active participation of individuals with SCI during robot-aided gait training. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 7: 43. pmid:20828422
  14. 14. Todorov E (2004) Optimality principles in sensorimotor control. Nature Neuroscience 7: 907–915. pmid:15332089
  15. 15. Cusumano JP, Cesari P (2006) Body-Goal Variability Mapping in an Aiming Task. Biological Cybernetics 94: 367–379. pmid:16501988
  16. 16. Dingwell JB, John J, Cusumano JP (2010) Do Humans Optimally Exploit Redundancy to Control Step Variability in Walking? PLoS Computational Biology 6: e1000856. pmid:20657664
  17. 17. Osborne LC, Lisberger SG, Bialek W (2005) A sensory source for motor variation. Nature 437: 412–416. pmid:16163357
  18. 18. Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nature Neuroscience 5: 1226–1235. pmid:12404008
  19. 19. Scott SH (2004) Optimal Feedback Control and the Neural Basis of Volitional Motor Control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5: 532–546. pmid:15208695
  20. 20. Cusumano JP, Dingwell JB (2013) Movement Variability Near Goal Equivalent Manifolds: Fluctuations, Control, and Model-Based Analysis. Human Movement Science 32: 899–923. pmid:24210574
  21. 21. Engelbrecht SE (2001) Minimum Principles in Motor Control. Journal of Mathematical Psychology 45: 497–542. pmid:11401453
  22. 22. Srinivasan M, Ruina A (2006) Computer optimization of a minimal biped model discovers walking and running. Nature 439: 72–75. pmid:16155564
  23. 23. Dingwell JB, Marin LC (2006) Kinematic Variability and Local Dynamic Stability of Upper Body Motions When Walking at Different Speeds. Journal of Biomechanics 39: 444–452. pmid:16389084
  24. 24. Kang HG, Dingwell JB (2008) Separating the Effects of Age and Speed on Gait Variability During Treadmill Walking. Gait & Posture 27: 572–577.
  25. 25. McDonnell MD, Ward LM (2011) The benefits of noise in neural systems: bridging theory and experiment. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 12: 415–426. pmid:21685932
  26. 26. Tumer EC, Brainard MS (2007) Performance variability enables adaptive plasticity of 'crystallized' adult birdsong. Nature 450: 1240–1244. pmid:18097411
  27. 27. Stein RB, Gossen ER, Jones KE (2005) Neuronal Variabiltiy: Noise or Part of the Signal? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6: 389–397. pmid:15861181
  28. 28. Rácz K, Valero-Cuevas F (2013) Spatio-temporal analysis reveals active control of both task-relevant and task-irrelevant variables. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 7: 155. pmid:24312045
  29. 29. van Beers RJ, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2013) Random walk of motor planning in task-irrelevant dimensions. Journal of Neurophysiology 109: 969–977. pmid:23175799
  30. 30. Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP (2000) Nonlinear Time Series Analysis of Normal and Pathological Human Walking. Chaos: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Nonlinear Science 10: 848–863.
  31. 31. Kang HG, Dingwell JB (2008) The Effects of Walking Speed, Strength and Range of Motion on Gait Stability in Healthy Older Adults. Journal of Biomechanics 41: 2899–2905. pmid:18790480
  32. 32. Hausdorff JM, Peng CK, Ladin Z, Wei JY, Goldberger AL (1995) Is Walking a Random Walk? Evidence for Long-Range Correlations in Stride Interval of Human Gait. Journal of Applied Physiology 78: 349–358. pmid:7713836
  33. 33. Hausdorff JM, Mitchell SL, Firtion R, Peng CK, Cudkowicz ME, Wei JY, et al. (1997) Altered Fractal Dynamics of Gait: Reduced Stride Interval Correlations with Aging and Huntington's Disease. Journal of Applied Physiology 82: 262–269. pmid:9029225
  34. 34. Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP (2010) Re-Interpreting Detrended Fluctuation Analyses of Stride-To-Stride Variability in Human Walking. Gait & Posture 32: 348–353.
  35. 35. Terrier P, Turner V, Schutz Y (2005) GPS analysis of human locomotion: Further evidence for long-range correlations in stride-to-stride fluctuations of gait parameters. Human Movement Science 24: 97–115. pmid:15896861
  36. 36. Terrier P, Dériaz O (2012) Persistent and anti-persistent pattern in stride-to-stride variability of treadmill walking: Influence of rhythmic auditory cueing. Human Movement Science 31: 1585–1597. pmid:23164626
  37. 37. Bertram JEA, Ruina A (2001) Multiple Walking Speed-Frequency Relations are Predicted by Constrained Optimization. Journal of Theoretical Biology 209: 445–453. pmid:11319893
  38. 38. Kuo AD (2001) A Simple Model of Bipedal Walking Predicts the Preferred Speed-Step Length Relationship. ASME Journal of Biomechanical Engineering 123: 264–269. pmid:11476370
  39. 39. Guckenheimer J, Holmes P (1983) Nonlinear Oscillations, Dynamical Systems, and Bifurcations of Vector Fields; Marsden JE, Sirovich L, John F, editors. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.
  40. 40. Manoonpong P, Geng T, Kulvicius T, Porr B, Wörgötter F (2007) Adaptive, Fast Walking in a Biped Robot under Neuronal Control and Learning. PLoS Computational Biology 3: e134. pmid:17630828
  41. 41. Golubitsky M, Stewart I, Buono P-L, Collins JJ (1999) Symmetry in Locomotor Central Pattern Generators and Animal Gaits. Nature 401: 693–695. pmid:10537106
  42. 42. Ijspeert AJ (2008) Central pattern generators for locomotion control in animals and robots: A review. Neural Networks 21: 642–653. pmid:18555958
  43. 43. Anderson FC, Pandy MG (2001) Dynamic Optimization of Human Walking. ASME Journal of Biomechanical Engineering 123: 381–390. pmid:11601721
  44. 44. Geyer H, Seyfarth A, Blickhan R (2006) Compliant leg behaviour explains basic dynamics of walking and running. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273: 2861–2867. pmid:17015312
  45. 45. Sethna JP (2006) Statistical Mechanics: Entropy, Order Parameters and Complexity. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  46. 46. Luenberger DG, Ye Y (2008) Linear and Nonlinear Programming. New York: Springer Science+Business Media.
  47. 47. Dingwell JB, John J, Cusumano JP (2010) Data From: Do Humans Optimally Exploit Redundancy to Control Step Variability in Walking? Dryad Digital Repository.
  48. 48. Peng C-K, Buldyrev SV, Goldberger AL, Havlin S, Sciortino F, Simons M, et al. (1992) Long-Range Correlations in Nucleotide Sequences. Nature 356: 168–170. pmid:1301010
  49. 49. Maraun D, Rust HW, Timmer J (2004) Tempting long-memory—on the interpretation of DFA results. Nonlinear Processes in Geophysics 11: 495–503.
  50. 50. O'Connor SM, Xu HZ, Kuo AD (2012) Energetic cost of walking with increased step variability. Gait & Posture 36: 102–107.
  51. 51. van Beers RJ (2009) Motor Learning Is Optimally Tuned to the Properties of Motor Noise. Neuron 63: 406–417. pmid:19679079
  52. 52. Wang Y, Srinivasan M (2014) Stepping in the direction of the fall: the next foot placement can be predicted from current upper body state in steady-state walking. Biology Letters 10: 20140405. pmid:25252834
  53. 53. Koolen T, de Boer T, Rebula J, Goswami A, Pratt J (2012) Capturability-based analysis and control of legged locomotion, Part 1: Theory and application to three simple gait models. The International Journal of Robotics Research 31: 1094–1113.
  54. 54. Pratt J, Koolen T, de Boer T, Rebula J, Cotton S, Carff J, et al. (2012) Capturability-based analysis and control of legged locomotion, Part 2: Application to M2V2, a lower-body humanoid. The International Journal of Robotics Research 31: 1117–1133.
  55. 55. Gubina F, Hemami H, McGhee RB (1974) On the Dynamic Stability of Biped Locomotion. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering BME-21: 102–108. pmid:4818794
  56. 56. Townsend MA (1985) Biped gait stabilization via foot placement. Journal of Biomechanics 18: 21–38. pmid:3980486
  57. 57. Lyon I, Day B (2005) Predictive control of body mass trajectory in a two-step sequence. Experimental Brain Research 161: 193–200. pmid:15480597
  58. 58. Lyon IN, Day BL (1997) Control of frontal plane body motion in human stepping. Experimental Brain Research 115: 345–356. pmid:9224862
  59. 59. Redfern MS, Schumann T (1994) A model of foot placement during gait. Journal of Biomechanics 27: 1339–1346. pmid:7798284
  60. 60. Cyr M-A, Smeesters C (2007) Instructions limiting the number of steps do not affect the kinetics of the threshold of balance recovery in younger adults. Journal of Biomechanics 40: 2857–2864. pmid:17544429
  61. 61. Forner-Cordero A, Koopman HFJM, van der Helm FCT (2003) Multiple-step strategies to recover from stumbling perturbations. Gait & Posture 18: 47–59.
  62. 62. Crenshaw JR, Rosenblatt NJ, Hurt CP, Grabiner MD (2012) The discriminant capabilities of stability measures, trunk kinematics, and step kinematics in classifying successful and failed compensatory stepping responses by young adults. Journal of Biomechanics 45: 129–133. pmid:22018682
  63. 63. Potocanac Z, de Bruin J, van der Veen S, Verschueren S, van Dieën J, Duysens J, et al. (2014) Fast online corrections of tripping responses. Experimental Brain Research 232: 3579–3590. pmid:25070085
  64. 64. Patla AE, Greig M (2006) Any way you look at it, successful obstacle negotiation needs visually guided on-line foot placement regulation during the approach phase. Neuroscience Letters 397: 110–114. pmid:16413969
  65. 65. Marigold DS (2008) Role of Peripheral Visual Cues in Online Visual Guidance of Locomotion. Exercise & Sport Sciences Reviews 36: 145–151.
  66. 66. Bruggeman H, Zosh W, Warren WH (2007) Optic Flow Drives Human Visuo-Locomotor Adaptation. Current Biology 17: 2035–2040. pmid:18023350
  67. 67. Patla AE, Adkin A, Ballard T (1999) Online steering: coordination and control of body center of mass, head and body reorientation. Experimental Brain Research 129: 629–634. pmid:10638436
  68. 68. Hollands MA, Patla AE, Vickers JN (2002) “Look where you’re going!”: gaze behaviour associated with maintaining and changing the direction of locomotion. Experimental Brain Research 143: 221–230. pmid:11880898
  69. 69. Patla AE, Vickers JN (2003) How far ahead do we look when required to step on specific locations in the travel path during locomotion? Experimental Brain Research 148: 133–138. pmid:12478404
  70. 70. Reynolds RF, Day BL (2005) amerc. The Journal of Physiology 569: 677–684. pmid:16179363
  71. 71. Moraes R, Allard F, Patla AE (2007) Validating Determinants for an Alternate Foot Placement Selection Algorithm During Human Locomotion in Cluttered Terrain. Journal of Neurophysiology 98: 1928–1940. pmid:17686917
  72. 72. Marigold DS, Patla AE (2007) Gaze fixation patterns for negotiating complex ground terrain. Neuroscience 144: 302–313. pmid:17055177
  73. 73. Franchak JM, Adolph KE (2010) Visually guided navigation: Head-mounted eye-tracking of natural locomotion in children and adults. Vision Research 50: 2766–2774. pmid:20932993
  74. 74. Matthis JS, Fajen BR (2013) Humans exploit the biomechanics of bipedal gait during visually guided walking over complex terrain. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 280.
  75. 75. Matthis JS, Fajen BR (2014) Visual control of foot placement when walking over complex terrain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 40: 106–115. pmid:23750964
  76. 76. Bach DR, Dolan RJ (2012) Knowing how much you don't know: a neural organization of uncertainty estimates. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13: 572–586. pmid:22781958
  77. 77. Bruijn SM, Meijer OG, Beek PJ, van Dieën JH (2013) Assessing the stability of human locomotion: a review of current measures. Journal of The Royal Society Interface 10: 20120999. pmid:23516062
  78. 78. Roos PE, Dingwell JB (2011) Influence of Simulated Neuromuscular Noise on the Dynamic Stability and Fall Risk in a 3D Dynamic Walking Model. Journal of Biomechanics 44: 1514–1520. pmid:21440895
  79. 79. Dingwell JB, Smallwood RF, Cusumano JP (2013) Trial-to-Trial Dynamics and Learning in a Generalized, Redundant Reaching Task. Journal of Neurophysiology 109: 225–237. pmid:23054607
  80. 80. Krishnan C, Ranganathan R, Dhaher YY, Rymer WZ (2013) A Pilot Study on the Feasibility of Robot-Aided Leg Motor Training to Facilitate Active Participation. PLoS ONE 8: e77370. pmid:24146986
  81. 81. Kuo AD (1999) Stabilization of Lateral Motion in Passive Dynamic Walking. The International Journal of Robotics Research 18: 917–930.
  82. 82. Wisse M, Schwab AL, Linde RQvd, Helm FCTvd (2005) How to Keep From Falling Forward: Elementary Swing Leg Action for Passive Dynamic Walkers. IEEE Transactions on Robotics 21: 393–401.
  83. 83. Dickinson MH, Farley CT, Full RJ, Koehl MA, Kram R, Lehman S (2000) How Animals Move: An Integrative View. Science Apr 7; 288: 100–106. pmid:10753108
  84. 84. Full RJ, Koditschek DE (1999) Templates and Anchors: Neuromechanical Hypothesis of Legged Locomotion. Journal of Experimental Biology 202: 3325–3332. pmid:10562515
  85. 85. van Ingen Schenau GJ (1980) Some Fundamental Aspects of the Biomechanics of Overground Versus Treadmill Locomotion. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 12: 257–261. pmid:7421475
  86. 86. Arsenault AB, Winter DA, Marteniuk RG (1986) Treadmill Versus Walkway Locomotion in Humans: An EMG Study. Egronomics 29: 665–676. pmid:3720723
  87. 87. Pailhous J, Bonnard M (1992) Steady-State Fluctuations of Human Walking. Behavioral and Brain Research 4: 181–190.
  88. 88. White SC, Yack HJ, Tucker CA, Lin HY (1998) Comparison of Vertical Ground Reaction Forces during Overground and Treadmill Walking. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise 30: 1537–1542. pmid:9789855
  89. 89. Rosenblatt NJ, Grabiner MD (2010) Measures of frontal plane stability during treadmill and overground walking. Gait & Posture 31: 380–384.
  90. 90. Riley PO, Paolini G, Della Croce U, Paylo KW, Kerrigan DC (2007) A kinematic and kinetic comparison of overground and treadmill walking in healthy subjects. Gait & Posture 26: 17–24.
  91. 91. Dingwell JB, Cusumano JP, Sternad D, Cavanagh PR (2001) Local Dynamic Stability Versus Kinematic Variability of Continuous Overground and Treadmill Walking. ASME Journal of Biomechanical Engineering 123: 27–32. pmid:11277298
  92. 92. Bollens B, Crevecoeur F, Nguyen V, Detrembleur C, Lejeune TM (2010) Does human gait exhibit comparable and reproducible long-range autocorrelations on level ground and on treadmill? Gait & Posture 32: 369–373.
  93. 93. Collins SH, Kuo AD (2013) Two Independent Contributions to Step Variability during Over-Ground Human Walking. PLoS ONE 8: e73597. pmid:24015308