Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Visual and Olfactory Floral Cues of Campanula (Campanulaceae) and Their Significance for Host Recognition by an Oligolectic Bee Pollinator

Abstract

Oligolectic bees collect pollen from a few plants within a genus or family to rear their offspring, and are known to rely on visual and olfactory floral cues to recognize host plants. However, studies investigating whether oligolectic bees recognize distinct host plants by using shared floral cues are scarce. In the present study, we investigated in a comparative approach the visual and olfactory floral cues of six Campanula species, of which only Campanula lactiflora has never been reported as a pollen source of the oligolectic bee Ch. rapunculi. We hypothesized that the flowers of Campanula species visited by Ch. rapunculi share visual (i.e. color) and/or olfactory cues (scents) that give them a host-specific signature. To test this hypothesis, floral color and scent were studied by spectrophotometric and chemical analyses, respectively. Additionally, we performed bioassays within a flight cage to test the innate color preference of Ch. rapunculi. Our results show that Campanula flowers reflect the light predominantly in the UV-blue/blue bee-color space and that Ch. rapunculi displays a strong innate preference for these two colors. Furthermore, we recorded spiroacetals in the floral scent of all Campanula species, but Ca. lactiflora. Spiroacetals, rarely found as floral scent constituents but quite common among Campanula species, were recently shown to play a key function for host-flower recognition by Ch. rapunculi. We conclude that Campanula species share some visual and olfactory floral cues, and that neurological adaptations (i.e. vision and olfaction) of Ch. rapunculi innately drive their foraging flights toward host flowers. The significance of our findings for the evolution of pollen diet breadth in bees is discussed.

Introduction

When navigating the landscape, pollinators are frequently confronted with a staggering diversity of flowers in the complex floral market and have to make choices. Making the right decision (e.g. choosing resource-rich flowers) might benefit pollinators by optimizing foraging efficiency [1]. The decision may reflect either innate [2, 3] or learned preferences [4, 5] for some floral cues, which are possibly related to neurophysiological adaptations of pollinators (e.g. olfactory receptors) and their diet breadth [68]. For example, specialized (oligolectic) bees collect pollen only on plants of a given genus or family to rear their offspring [9, 10] and, consequently, must rely on host-specific floral cues to recognize host flowers unambiguously. Generalized (polylectic) bees, on the other hand, collect pollen on a vast array of plant species irrespective of phylogenetic relatedness, and might rely on more generalized floral cues [11].

Bees are known to use mainly visual (e.g. color) and olfactory floral cues (e.g. floral scents) to find and select food sources [12]. Generally, visual and olfactory cues work together in modulating bees' behavior, however, the relative importance each cue play varies greatly across associations (for a review see [13]). Visual cues alone are known to attract bees and are often used by them to discriminate between rewarding and non-rewarding flowers within a species [1417]. However, as compared to floral scent, visual cues are assumed to play a minor role in the discrimination of flowers among species [4, 18]. For example, when considering color, the most well investigated visual cue, flowers of species belonging to different genera or families are often very similar and may not be well discriminated by bees [1922]. Conversely, the potential infinite diversity of floral scents, due to either highly specific compounds or unique ratios of common compounds [23, 24], makes them highly specific cues that provide pollinators with an astonishing amount of information. Not surprisingly, there are increasing evidences that bees use floral volatiles to discriminate among flowers, both intra-specifically, such as among flowers of different plants, flowers within an inflorescence, and flowers with different amount of reward (e.g. pollen and nectar), and inter-specifically [19, 21, 2529]. More specifically, in associations involving specialists, such as those between some oligolectic bees and their host plants, floral scents are known to be the ultimate cue for host recognition [11].

Chelostoma rapunculi (Lepeletier 1841) (Fig 1) is a European oligolectic bee that collects pollen on several Campanula species [30, 31], and in the presence of host plants, both males and females restrict nectar gathering to these plants. The genus Campanula L. (Campanulaceae) comprises about 400 species of bellflowers, harebells, and starbells, whose flowers are predominantly violet-blue to the human eyes [32, 33]. In spite of its remarkable diversity, floral scent composition in the genus Campanula has been chemically characterized only in a single species, namely Ca. trachelium [34]. Recently, we showed that Ch. rapunculi innately prefers both visual and olfactory floral cues of Ca. trachelium over those of two other co-flowering non-host plants [20]. The exact cues involved in the innate visual preference by Ch. rapunculi remain unknown, even if some evidence point to color as important visual dimension [35]. In terms of olfactory cues, however, newly-emerged bees of Ch. rapunculi were shown to rely on spiroacetals to discriminate host from non-host flowers [34]. Spiroacetals form a distinct group of natural volatiles that are rarely encountered in floral scents [24]. In the floral scent bouquet of Ca. trachelium, however, six spiroacetals were found, some of them recorded for the first time as floral scent constituents [34].

thumbnail
Fig 1. Flowers of Campanula species used for the study and their oligolectic pollinator Chelostoma rapunculi.

(A) Ca. glomerata, (B) Ca. lactiflora (C) Ca. persicifolia alba, (D) Ca. persicifolia, (E) Ca. rapunculoides, (F) Ca. rotundifolia, and (G) Ca. trachelium. (G, H) Females of Ch. rapunculi gathering pollen on Ca. trachelium and Ca. glomerata, respectively. All photos by Paulo Milet-Pinheiro.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128577.g001

In the present study, we investigated in a comparative approach the visual (i.e. color reflectance spectra) and olfactory (i.e. scent bouquet) floral cues of five Campanula species which are used as pollen source by Ch. rapunculi (Ca. glomerata, Ca. persicifolia, Ca. rapunculoides, Ca. rotundifolia, and Ca. trachelium) and one species (Ca. lactiflora), which is not visited by this bee species [30, 36]. We predict that Campanula flowers visited by Ch. rapunculi share visual and/or olfactory cues that give them a host-specific signature, allowing bees to discriminate them from non-host flowers. More specifically, we hypothesize that 1) Campanula species, which are visited by Ch. rapunculi, have similar floral color reflectance, 2) Ch. rapunculi bees have an innate preference for the color of host species, and 3) Spiroacetals are constituents of the floral scent bouquet of the Campanula species, which are used by Ch. rapunculi as pollen source.

Materials and Methods

Plant species

Plant individuals of Ca. lactiflora, Ca. glomerata, Ca. persicifolia (blue and white variety), Ca. rapunculoides, Ca. rotundifolia, and Ca. trachelium (Fig 1) were cultivated to seedling stage in the greenhouses of the Department of Plant Systematics, University of Bayreuth, and of the Botanical Garden of Ulm, University of Ulm, both in Germany. Subsequently, plants were cultivated outdoors in flowerbeds, where they grew to maturity.

All species selected for this study occur in north hemisphere and, at least in part, their distributions overlap with each other and with that of Ch. rapunculi. With the exception of Ca. lactiflora, which has a restricted distribution in the Turkey and Caucasus region [33], all other species are widely spread. Ca. glomerata, Ca. trachelium and Ca. rapunculoides are all common elements in the Eurasian region, Ca. rotundifolia is distributed throughout the circumpolar region, while Ca. persicifolia is found predominantly in central Europe [32].

Color measurements of Campanula flowers and bee color hexagon

The spectral reflection properties of the corolla of Ca. glomerata, Ca. lactiflora, Ca. persicifolia (blue and white variety), Ca. rapunculoides, Ca. rotundifolia, and Ca. trachelium, were recorded from 300 to 700 nm (the wavelength perceived by bees; [37]) using a Varian Cary 5 spectrophotometer equipped with a Praying Mantis accessory (Varian, Inc, Palo Alto, California). For each species, measurements were taken from the inner surface of the corolla of three plant individuals (N = 1 flower per individual). For each flower, two measurements were taken, one from the basis and the other from the tip of the corolla. Barium sulfate was used as white standard and the disconnected beam as black reference.

The mean reflections of the petals (built first from the measurements of the tip and basis of each flower and then from the three replicates per species) were used to determine the loci of corolla colors in the hexagon color space [38]. We applied the daylight irradiance spectrum D65 as standard and used the spectral sensitivity of honeybee’s photoreceptors [39]. Given that bees do not differ substantially in their visual sensory systems [37], we used the spectral sensitivity functions described for the honeybees as representative approximation for Chelostoma rapunculi [39]. The position of the color loci show how bees perceive the corollas through their ultraviolet, blue and green photoreceptors and through further processing of receptor signals in the central nervous system [40]. The color hexagon is separated in six different color sectors representing the different bee color spaces, i.e. UV, UV-blue, blue, blue-green, green, UV-green [41].

For comparison of the bee colors among the different Campanula species, the pairwise hexagon distances of color loci among species, as well as the distance of each color locus to its background (green leaves) was calculated [39]. The reflectance function of a typical green leaf was used as background color [41]. We opted to use this reflectance function because Campanula species frequently grow together with other plant species, meaning that the background might not represent only the reflectance function of their own leaves. Behavioral experiments with bumblebees trained to visit artificial flowers have demonstrated that color distances of 0.05 hexagon units are poorly discriminated, whereas distances greater than 0.1 are easily discriminated [42, 43].

Bioassays testing the role of visual cues

Pre-testing procedures.

To test innate responses of Ch. rapunculi to visual cues of flowers, we performed a series of behavioral assays using flower-inexperienced bees, which we define here as bees that had no previous contact with flowers. To assure this, trap nests filled with preimaginal Ch. rapunculi bees were placed at the end of May in an experimental flight cage, where no plants were available. Thus, as bees emerged they did not have the possibility to visit flowers, but rather only black sponge feeders saturated with sugar water (30%, fructose and glucose 1:1). We do not believe that the visual cues perceived by the bees during this time influenced our behavioral assays. Even in the absence of plants, bees flew actively in the flight cage, foraged for sugar water on feeders and mated. The trap nests filled with preimaginal bees were provisioned one year before by females collected at the surroundings of Ulm and Bayreuth, where some of the Campanula investigated do not occur naturally. However, during provisioning in the flight cage, individuals of all Campanula species were made available for the bees.

The experimental flight cage, located in the botanical garden of Ulm (Germany), consisted of a steel frame (7m x 3.5m x 2.2m large) covered with a fine mesh (stitch density of 1mm x 0.5mm) that was buried into the soil for a deep of 0.5 m. This setup allowed abiotic conditions inside the flight cage (e.g. day length, temperature, air humidity, and light conditions) similar to those found outside in nature.

Design of bioassays.

To establish whether color cues are involved somehow in the process of host-flower recognition by Ch. rapunculi, we performed a series of two choice bioassays testing the attractiveness of different colors to flower-inexperienced bees. Behavioral assays were performed first with inflorescences of the white and blue variety of Ca. persicifolia to reflect a context in which bees are frequently confronted in the nature. By doing this, we expected to test the role of color alone, since size and shape of the blue and white variety of Ca. persicifolia were similar (Milet-Pinheiro, unp. data). To investigate more accurately the role of color in the process of host recognition, we performed then further bioassays with artificial flowers.

For the two choice bioassay with inflorescences of Ca. persicifolia, we used transparent solid cylinders (39 cm in height and 9.5 cm in diameter) that allow bees to see, but not to smell the flowers (for a diagram see [44]). The cylinders were made from Makrolon because of its ultraviolet (UV) transparency. Inflorescences of both white and blue variety of Ca. persicifolia were each covered with a cylinder and offered simultaneously to the bees in the flight cage. The inflorescences of both varieties had six flowers each and were 20 cm in height.

For the bioassays with artificial flowers, we fashioned funnel-shaped flowers (colors: lilac, yellow, and white; dimensions: 4.5 cm in length and 2 cm in diameter at the top). Besides lilac, as a representative color of Campanula species, we choose yellow and white because of the representativeness of these colors in the nature [45]. In the flight cage, bees were offered a series of paired combinations of these artificial flowers: 1) white vs. lilac, 2) white vs. yellow, and 3) yellow vs. lilac. For each bioassay, three artificial flowers of each color were presented simultaneously "as an inflorescence" to the bees. The flowers were attached at the base on a thin wooden stick (length: 23 cm; diameter: 2 mm), which was fixed at the ground.

The bioassays with both inflorescences of Ca. persicifolia and artificial flowers were conducted for 30 min; the initial position of either cylinders or artificial flower groups was inverted after 15 min. Cylinders and groups of artificial flowers were placed 1.5 meter from each other. We always used new artificial flowers in each bioassay to avoid influence of footprints of bees in the decision of other bee individuals. All bees either approaching to (to a distance ≤ 5 cm) or landing on either artificial flowers or cylinders were caught with insect nets and stored in an icebox to avoid pseudo-replications (i.e. counting a response of a single bee more than once). At the end of the experiments, all bees stored in the icebox were released in the flight cage and could participate in subsequent tests. The biotests were performed on sunny days between 1000 h and 1400 h, coinciding with the time of highest foraging activity of bees. About 100 bee individuals (females and males together) were present in the flight cage for the bioassays.

Sampling of floral scents

To obtain scent samples for the chemical analyses, volatiles were collected from inflorescences of Ca. glomerata, Ca. lactiflora, Ca. persicifolia (blue and white variety), Ca. rapunculoides, and Ca. rotundifolia using standard dynamic headspace methods. Floral scents of Ca. trachelium had been collected and characterized before using the same methods [34]. Fresh inflorescences of three different plants (number of samples and flowers varied among species; Table 1) were enclosed in a polyester oven bag (20 x 30 cm; Toppits). The volatiles were trapped for 4 h in an adsorbent tube, through which air was flown at a rate of 200 mL min-1 using a membrane pump (G12/01 EB, Rietschle Thomas, Puchheim, Germany). The adsorbent tubes consisted of ChromatoProbe quartz microvials (GC/MS: length: 15 mm; inner diameter: 2 mm; Varian Inc., Palo Alto, CA, USA), cut at the closed end, and filled with a mixture of 1.5 mg Tenax-TA (mesh 60–80; Supelco, Bellefonte, Pennsylvania, USA) and 1.5 mg Carbotrap B (mesh 20–40, Supelco, Bellefonte, Pennsylvania, USA), which was held in the tubes using glass wool [46].

thumbnail
Table 1. Mean absolute and relative amount of volatile compounds in headspace samples collected from inflorescences of six Campanula species (one species with two morphs).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128577.t001

To control for non-floral (vegetative) volatiles and contaminants in the floral scent samples, headspace samples of non-flowering plants (N = 3 for each species) and blank controls (empty oven bags; N = 3 for each species) were also collected following the same methods as described above. All headspace samples were stored in screw cap vials at -20°C until chemical analyses.

Chemical analyses of floral scent samples

The headspace samples were analyzed on a Varian Saturn 2000 mass spectrometer coupled to a Varian 3800 gas chromatograph (GC/MS) equipped with a 1079 injector (Varian Inc., Palo Alto, CA, USA), which had been fitted with the ChromatoProbe kit (see [46]). A quartz microvial was loaded into the probe, which was then inserted into the modified GC injector. The injector split vent was opened and the injector heated to 40°C to flush any air from the system. After 2 min, the split vent was closed, and the injector heated to 200°C/min at a rate of 200°C min-1. This temperature was then held at 200°C for 4.2 min, after which the split vent was opened and the injector cooled down. Separation of compounds was achieved with a fused silica column ZB-5 (5% phenyl polysiloxane; 60 m long, inner diameter 0.25 mm, film thickness 0.25 μm, Phenomenex). Electronic flow control was used to maintain a constant helium carrier gas flow of 1.0 mL min-1. The GC oven temperature was held for 7 min at 40°C, then increased by 6°C per min to 250°C and held for 1 min. The MS interface worked at 260°C and the ion trap at 175°C. Mass spectra were taken at 70 eV (in EI mode) with a scanning speed of 1 scan sec-1 from m/z 30 to 350. The GC/MS data were processed using the Saturn Software package 5.2.1.

Identification of compounds was carried out using the NIST 08, Wiley 7, and Adams [47] mass spectral data bases, or the data base provided in MassFinder 3, and confirmed by comparison of retention times with published data [47]. Structure assignments of individual components were confirmed by comparison of mass spectra and GC retention times with those of commercially available standards.

The composition of volatiles collected from flowering plants was compared with that from non-flowering plants (vegetative parts). Floral scent volatiles were those detected either in higher amounts or exclusively in floral scent headspace samples. Volatiles detected in the empty bags were considered ambient contaminants and were omitted from the experimental samples.

Statistical analyses

To test for differences in bee responses between the paired treatments in each behavioral assay, we performed two-tailed exact binomial tests. Binomial tests were calculated using the spreadsheet provided by http://www.biostathandbook.com/exactgof.html (accessed 02 July 2013; see also [48]). Responses of male and female bees were pooled, as individuals of both sexes have been shown to respond similarly to artificial flowers and to visual cues of host flowers (Fisher's Exact tests: 0.21 < P < 0.71). The similar behavior of male and female of Ch. rapunculi to floral cues of host flowers had already been evidenced in a previous work [20].

Flower scent profiles of the investigated species were compared using both qualitative (presence/absence of compounds) and semi-quantitative (relative amount of compounds with respect to total peak area) approaches. For qualitative and semi-quantitative comparisons, we calculated the Sørensen and Bray-curtis similarity indices, respectively. These indices determine pairwise similarities among the individual samples. The relative ratios of compounds were transformed to their square root for the semi-quantitative analysis. After that, and based on the obtained similarity matrices (individual based matrices), we performed analyses of similarities (ANOSIM, 10,000 permutations) to test for differences in scent bouquet among species. ANOSIM is a commonly used multivariate procedure roughly analogous to ANOVA/MANOVA that operates directly on a (dis)similarity matrix. It yields a test statistic R that is a relative measure of separation among a priori defined groups. It is based on differences of mean ranks among and within groups. A R value of ‘0’ indicates completely random grouping, whereas a value of ‘1’indicates that samples within groups are more similar to each other than to any sample from a different group [49]. Non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS), based on the similarity matrices generated, were used to display graphically the qualitative and semi-quantitative differences in scent profile among species. Stress values indicate how well the two-dimensional plot represents relationships among samples in multidimensional space. Stress values below 0.15 indicate a good fit [49]. The software Primer 6.1.6 was used to calculate the similarity indices of Sørensen and Bray-Curtis and to perform the ANOSIM and NMDS analyses [49].

Results

Comparisons of floral color among Campanula species and artificial flowers

The comparison of corolla reflectance in Campanula species showed that the color loci of the investigated species form a gradient across the UV-blue- and blue-green color space in the bee-color hexagon (Fig 2). Taking into account the pairwise interspecific distances in color loci of Campanula flowers (S1 Table), three groups can be observed: 1) the UV-blue group, including Ca. glomerata and Ca. rapunculoides; 2) the blue group, including Ca. persicifolia, Ca. rotundifolia, and Ca. trachelium; and 3) the blue/blue-green intersection group, including Ca. lactiflora and Ca. persicifolia alba. Species within groups reflect color very similarly (i.e. the Euclidean distances in the color hexagon are < 0.1 units), while differing from all other Campanula species (i.e. Euclidean distances in the color hexagon are > 0.1 units). The corolla color of all species is easily detectable by bees against a green standard background (hexagon units > 0.1), when assuming that Chelostoma rapunculi behave the same as bumble bees (see Materials and Methods).

thumbnail
Fig 2. Corolla color loci of Campanula in the bee color hexagon.

Corolla color loci of six Campanula species (one species with two color morphs) and artificial flowers plotted in a hexagon color space. EU, EB, EG: excitation of the UV, blue, and green receptor, respectively. The pairwise interspecific distances in color loci and the distances to the centre of the single species can be found in Supporting Information (S1 Table).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128577.g002

The color of lilac artificial flowers used in the bioassays resembles that of the flowers of Ca. glomerata and Ca. rapunculoides (hexagon units < 0.1). The color loci of the yellow and the white artificial flowers were in the UV-green and uncolored section of the hexagon, respectively. While the white artificial flowers bore some color similarity to flowers of Ca. persicifolia alba (hexagon units < 0.1), the yellow artificial flowers bore no color similarity to any of the Campanula flowers (> 0.1). Bees can discriminate colors of all artificial flowers among each other (S1 Table).

Behavioral assays

For the bioassays with inflorescences, flower-inexperienced bees showed a clear preference for the visual cues of Ca. persicifolia over those of Ca. persicifolia alba (Fig 3).

thumbnail
Fig 3. Behavioral assays testing attractiveness of inflorescences and artificial flowers to Chelostoma rapunculi bees.

Responses of flower-inexperienced bees of Chelostoma rapunculi (males and females together) to visual cues of either inflorescences of Campanula persicifolia or artificial flowers in behavioral assays performed in an experimental flight cage. Numbers above bars indicate the absolute number of responding bees. Difference in responses for each assay was assessed by an exact binominal test, P < 0.05 (*), P < 0.001 (***), not significant (n.s).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128577.g003

The innate preference of Ch. rapunculi for a color characteristic for Campanula was confirmed in the bioassays using artificial flowers. Lilac artificial flowers were more attractive than both yellow and white flowers, whereas no preference was detected when yellow and white flowers were tested against each other (Fig 3).

Comparison of floral scents among Campanula species

The total amount of scent trapped per flower and per hour ranged from 0.7 ng in Ca. rotundifolia to 6 ng in Ca. persicifolia (Table 1). We detected 118 compounds in the scent bouquet of the different Campanula species, from which 77 could be identified (Table 1; S2 Table). Altogether, representatives of seven compound classes were recorded in the different Campanula species: sesquiterpenes (47 compounds), monoterpenes (21), aromatics (12), spiroacetals (6), aliphatics (4), N-compounds (4), and irregular terpenes (2); the class of 22 compounds could not be determined. Campanula glomerata and Ca. rotundifolia had representatives from all compound classes, while Ca. persicifolia, Ca. persicifolia alba, Ca. rapunculoides, and Ca. trachelium from six. The number of floral volatile compounds produced by each species varied considerably, ranging from 27 in Ca. persicifolia alba to 55 in Ca. trachelium. There was only one compound, namely 2-phenylethanol, which was recorded in all analyzed species. Additional compounds frequently recorded were (E)-conophthorin, 4-oxoisophorone, and α-ylangene (in five species each). The scent bouquet of the Campanula species was generally dominated by a few compounds emitted in large amounts and several compounds emitted in low amounts. The relative contribution to the total flower scent discharge of the five most abundant compounds of each species ranged from 42.4% in Ca. glomerata to 93.3% in Ca. lactiflora (Table 1).

Spiroacetals (at least two per species) were recorded in all species, except for Ca. lactiflora. The most common spiroacetal was (E)-conophthorin (recorded in five species) followed by (E)-7-Ethyl-1,6-dioxaspiro[4.5]decane (3 spp.), and 1,6-Dioxaspiro[4.5]decane and (Z)-conophthorin (2 spp. each). (E)-7-Ethyl-1,6-dioxaspiro[4.5]decane and (E)-2-Methyl-1,7-dioxaspiro[5.5]undecane were recorded in only one species each. Spiroacetals were minor compounds in all species, normally responding to relative amounts lesser than 1%, with the exception of (E)-conophthorin in Ca. glomerata that responded to about 6% of the total scent discharge. None of the spiroacetals was recorded in all samples of the species in which they were reported (S2 Table).

Multivariate analyses evidenced significant differences in both qualitative (presence/absence of compounds; ANOSIM global R = 0.982, P < 0.01) and semi-quantitative (relative amount of compounds; ANOSIM global R = 0.985, P < 0.01) composition of floral scents among species. Composition of floral scents differed significantly for all pairs of species tested (R = 1 for all pairs and P < 0.05 for both qualitative and semi-quantitative comparisons), meaning that each species produces its own scent profile (Fig 4). Qualitative (R = 0.68, P < 0.01) and semi-quantitative differences (R = 0.76, P < 0.01) in scent composition were evident even between the blue and white variation of Ca. persicifolia. With the exception of Ca. persicifolia, floral scent samples of the Campanula species used by Ch. rapunculi as pollen source were plotted more close to each other when compared to the samples of the non-host plant Ca. lactiflora (Fig 4).

thumbnail
Fig 4. Nonmetric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) of floral scent composition in Campanula.

Comparison of floral scent bouquets among six Campanula species (one species with two color morphs) based on qualitative Bray—Curtis similarities plotted in a non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS). The NMDS plot based on semi-quantitative similarities is not shown since samples clustered similarly (see text).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128577.g004

Discussion

In the present study, we hypothesized that Campanula species share visual and olfactory floral cues that could be used as host-recognition cues by oligolectic bees of Ch. rapunculi. In corroboration to our hypothesis, we found that the flowers of most Campanula species were grouped into the UV-blue and blue bee-color space and that these colors are more attractive than others (e.g. UV-green and blue-green) to the Campanula pollen-specialist bee Ch. rapunculi. In terms of olfactory cues, we found that floral scent bouquets are species-specific and that highly specific volatiles, i.e. spiroacetals, which were shown to be the key compounds in Ca. trachelium for host recognition of C. rapunculi [34], are emitted by all species investigated with the exception of Ca. lactiflora. Interestingly, among the Campanula species investigated, this is the only one that is not used by Ch. rapunculi females as pollen source.

Floral color in Campanula and its significance for host location by Chelostoma rapunculi bees

The color analyses performed in this study evidenced that the corolla floral reflectance of the Campanula species form a gradient across the UV-blue and blue-green bee-color space. These results are in congruence with those in the floral reflectance database (FReD; [45]), where seven of the ten further Campanula species investigated were shown to reflect the light in the UV-blue, two in the blue, and one in the blue-green bee-color space. This clearly shows that the UV-blue and blue are the most representative bee-colors in the genus Campanula. Color is crucial in flower location by bees as a whole [2, 12] and in the perspective of the oligolectic pollinator Ch. rapunculi it seems to have a strong significance for host location. The set of bioassays showed that flower-inexperienced bees of Ch. rapunculi have an innate preference exactly for the two bee-color categories which are most representative for flowers of their Campanula host plants. Chelostoma rapunculi is a strict oligolectic species that does not provide nests in the absence of Campanula host flowers [20, 34]. Therefore, host location is crucial for a successful brood provision and our findings indicate that color has a strong significance in this sense, i.e. the innate color preference of Ch. rapunculi would tune its foraging activity to UV-blue/blue colors, increasing the chance of locating Campanula host flowers. While our results clearly indicate that color is important for host location by Ch. rapunculi, its significance for host recognition is questionable. The UV-blue and blue-bee colors of Campanula flowers is shared by several other plants belonging to different genera and families (see the floral reflectance database, [45]), suggesting that color alone would not be specific enough to allow unambiguously host recognition (see also [20, 26, 50]). Thus, further cues other than color might be necessary to allow recognition of Campanula host flowers by Ch. rapunculi.

Floral scent in Campanula and its significance for host recognition by Chelostoma rapunculi bees

In recent investigations, we showed that newly-emerged Ch. rapunculi bees differentiate Ca. trachelium from non-host species of other plant groups using floral scents and that unusual floral scent compounds, i.e. spiroacetals, are used by these bees for host recognition [20, 34]. Since Ch. rapunculi bees visit several Campanula species to collect pollen, we hypothesized that the presence of spiroacetals would be a common trait typifying Campanula host plants. In corroboration with this hypothesis, we found spiroacetals as constituents of the scent bouquet of five among the six species of Campanula investigated. Not surprisingly, the only species that does not emit spiroacetals, i.e. Ca. lactiflora, has never been reported as pollen source of Ch. rapunculi. In support of this finding, Ch. rapunculi bees did not visit flowers of Ca. lactiflora in our flight cage, even in the absence of host plants (Milet-Pinheiro, unp. data). Spiroacetals are relatively widespread volatiles in nature, occurring, for example, in mammals [51], insects [52], and in the bark of many conifer trees [5355]. In contrast, they are rarely encountered in floral scents [24], and thus might give a distinct, unique floral scent identity to the Campanula species that are visited by Ch. rapunculi.

In the present study, we recorded six spiroacetals, of which four (i.e. 1,6-Dioxaspiro [4.5] decane, E-2-Methyl-1,7-dioxaspiro[5.5]undecane, E-7-ethyl-1,6-dioxaspiro[4.5]decane, Z-7-Ethyl-1,6-dioxaspiro[4.5]decane), have never been reported as constituents of floral scents in plants other than Campanula [34]. In contrast, the (E)- and (Z)-conophthorin isomers are reported in the floral scents of representatives of 13 and 5 plant families, respectively [24]. Curiously, (E)-conophthorin was the unique of these compounds emitted by all host plants of Ch. rapunculi, meaning that this would be the most host-typifying spiroacetal. In this scenario, one might argue that this compound would not be host-specific enough for allowing unambiguous recognition by Ch. rapunculi. A more comprehensive screening, however, indicates that (E)-conophthorin is rather unusual in the context in which Ch. rapunculi forages; the plants that emit this compound as floral scent constituent either do not occur syntopically (most are tropical representatives of Lecythidaceae, Passifloraceae, Orchidaceae, Moraceae, and Solanaceae) or do not bloom simultaneously with host plants of Ch. rapunculi. This context-dependent specificity of rather common compounds has also been found in other associations involving oligolectic bees, such as Andrena vaga (Andrenidae), Protodiscelis palpalis (Colletidae), and Peponapis pruinosa (Apidae). In these cases, the key compounds involved in the attraction of the bees, respectively, 1,4-dimethoxybenzene [56], p-methylanisole [57], and 1,2,4-trimethoxybenzene [58] are reported in about 15, 20, and 5 families, respectively [24]. Together, the findings of these studies suggest that compounds, which are not necessarily unique as floral scent constituents in general, may be very unusual in certain context and indeed are involved in host-recognition by oligolectic bees.

The possibility that spiroacetals other than conophthorin are also involved in the host-recognition of Campanula flowers by host-naive Ch. rapunculi bees cannot be ruled out, since the attractiveness of these compounds was tested as a mixture [34]. Furthermore, although recorded in all species, (E)-conophthorin was absent in some samples. Similarly, the other spiroacetals were never recorded in all scent samples of a given species. This suggests that spiroacetals may be either not emitted by all plant individuals or emitted in amounts below the detection limit of the GC-MS. We believe that the second alternative is more likely, since spiroacetals are normally minor constituents of the astonishing weak floral scent bouquet of Campanula species. Additionally, electroantennographic analyses performed with Ch. rapunculi indicate that these bees can detect spiroacetals in concentrations as low as 1:10000000, a concentration that is below the detection limit of the GC-MS used for the scent analyses (Milet-Pinheiro et al. in prep.). Thus, it is well possible that Ch. rapunculi uses the other spiroacetals or a combination thereof to recognize host plants. The high specificity of all these compounds, mainly if they act in combination, would make host plants of Ch. rapunculi even more unambiguous. Behavioral assays testing the role of spiroacetals individually and in mixtures are necessary to shed more light on this complex and challenging puzzle of host recognition by Ch. rapunculi.

While the argumentation above may help to explain the absence of spiroacetals in some samples of the Campanula species that host Ch. rapunculi, it also brings some hesitation for their absence in the non-host Ca. lactiflora. However, different to the host plants of Ch. rapunculi, spiroacetals were not detected in any of the samples of Ca. lactiflora (even in trace amounts), in spite of the great number of individuals and flowers used, suggesting that they are indeed not produced by flowers of Ca. lactiflora. Additional support of the absence of spiroacetals in Ca. lactiflora is provided by the phylogeny. This species belongs to a clade that does not include host plants [59, 60]. Future studies should test for the presence of spiroacetals in other non-host species.

Studies investigating the role of floral scent in host recognition by oligolectic bees are very scarce. Nevertheless, the general tendency emerging so far suggests that oligolectic bees rely initially on a single or few compounds to recognize host flowers [26, 34, 50, 56, 57]. Oligolectic bees generally collect pollen from different species within a clade [61], which may vary considerably in floral scent composition [19, 21]. In the case of Campanula, the chemical analyses showed that both qualitative (presence/absence) and semi-quantitative (relative amount) scent composition of Campanula are species-specific, i.e. each species has its own scent profile. In consequence, single or few compounds that are shared by host plants might represent a much more predictable and reliable cue for host recognition by oligolectic bees in their first foraging trips than complex scent bouquet.

Interestingly, increasing evidences suggest that the innate chemical search-image of oligolectic bees changes to a more complex mixture of volatiles after bees acquire foraging experience on host flowers [11], indicating that bees might benefit of adding further compounds to their innate search-image. Pollen and nectar are known to be scented and in some cases its emitted volatiles can be distinguished from those of other flower parts [26, 62]. In the case of Ca. trachelium, scent bouquets of floral parts containing nectar and pollen are dominated by linalool and (E)-ß-ocimene, which are only attractive after foraging experience [34]. If Ch. rapunculi bees learn to associate these compounds (or other compounds) with pollen and/or nectar availability, adding them to their chemical search-image might facilitate selection of host flowers with plenty of resources, thereby increasing foraging efficiency (see also [29]). In the perspective of the plants, associative learning might assure flower constancy by pollinators with the benefit of reducing pollen loss while bees visit flowers of other species. Consequently, the species-specific nature of floral scents might be of great significance for plant reproductive isolation mainly among syntopic species, as is the case for several Campanula species. Further experimental studies are still necessary to establish to what extent changes in innate chemical search-image are mediated by associative learning in this and other oligolectic bees and to understand how this phenomenon affects reproductive isolation of plants.

Integration of visual and olfactory cues

As suggested above, visual and olfactory cues seem to play different, but synergistic roles, in the process of host recognition by Ch. rapunculi. In a previous study, we showed that decoupled visual and olfactory cues are attractive to Ch. rapunculi, but that a combination of these two sensory modalities is much more attractive than is either cue alone [20]. Furthermore, decoupled visual cues were found to be more attractive for bees than olfactory cues when these stimuli were tested against each other. This suggests that foraging of Ch. rapunculi bees is based first on visual cues, after which floral scents might come into play, and evidences the importance of multisensory integration for recognition of host flowers. Thus, as supported by our experiment, color might be interpreted as a foraging filter, which leads bees to preferentially approach UV-blue/blue bee-colored flowers, while floral scents provide more specific information, such as host identity and resource availability (see also [44]).

Multisensory integration in bees is believed to impact positively on foraging efficiency, by improving aspects such as accuracy, decision speed, and learning speed [13]. Besides in Ch. rapunculi, the impact of multisensory integration for host-flower recognition, as compared to single sensory modalities, has been tested in a few oligolectic species, namely Hoplitis adunca [19] and Macropis fulvipes [63]. The innate relative reliance on visual as compared to olfactory cues has been found to vary considerably between species. In H. adunca, for example, female bees respond to visual, but not to olfactory cues of host flowers. In contrast, M. fulvipes females respond to olfactory, but not to visual cues. In spite of the relative reliance on decoupled cues, the combination of visual and olfactory cues resulted always in an increased attractiveness to bees as compared to either cues alone in both species and this was true for host-naive and host-experienced bees. Overall, these studies indicate that the impact of decoupled visual and olfactory cues varies from one species to the other, but not the impact of these two cues together. This means that oligolectic bees might benefit of integrating different sensory modalities when foraging on the field.

Conclusions and Perspectives

Our results, together with those of recent studies investigating the basis of host recognition by Ch. rapunculi, suggest that this bee has neurological adaptations (vision and olfaction) that innately drive its foraging flights toward host flowers. Furthermore, the strong innate preference displayed by Ch. rapunculi to both olfactory and visual cues of Campanula provide strong evidence that host-plant preference is genetically based (see also [64]); while olfactory cues may be learned during the larval stage [65], visual cues (e.g. color) can only be learned during flower visits. Collectively, these findings strengthen the hypothesis that host-plant choice is genetically constrained in bees and that specific neurophysiological adaptations to host flowers may preclude oligolectic bees of collecting pollen in alternative hosts, thereby restricting diet breadth [8]. In the case of Ch. rapunculi, the strong innate preference for UV-blue/blue bee-color, but mainly for host-specific spiroacetal volatiles, might be preventing this bee to forage for pollen on plants other than Campanula. This is particularly critical for this bee species because its larvae do not develop (or experience high mortality) when reared on pollen of non-host plants [7].

This study represents a significant progress in the current understanding of the floral scent chemistry in the genus Campanula, as well as in its ecological significance, and opens interesting perspectives. In Central Europe, the spectrum of bees that visit flowers of Campanula is very diverse and includes both pollen-generalists, such as Western honeybee and several bumblebee species, and pollen-specialist species, among others Andrena curvungula and A. rufizona (Andrenidae), Chelostoma rapunculi, Ch. campanularum, Ch. distinctum (Megachilidae), Dufourea dentiventris, D. inermis (Halictidae), and Melitta haemorrhoidalis (Melittidae) [30, 59, 66]. In this scenario, it would be very interesting to investigate in a comparative approach how floral scents of Campanula are perceived by polylectic and oligolectic pollinators. Especially interesting would be a comparison in the sensitivity of polylectic and oligolectic species to both ubiquitous (e.g. 2-phenyl ethanol, ocimene) and unusual compounds (spiroacetals) using electroantennographic analyses and behavioral assays. Given the highly specific nature of spiroacetals in Campanula flowers, and their significance in host recognition by Ch. rapunculi, we believe that these volatiles might also be involved in host recognition by other Campanula oligoleges. In contrast, polylectic bees, which are less restrictive in pollen diet, might use ubiquitous compounds that represent a broader spectrum of flowering plants. Such a comparative study would give us a stronger grasp not only on the evolution of olfactory receptors in specialized and generalized pollinators but also of floral scents in the genus Campanula.

Supporting Information

S1 Table. Euclidean distances among flower color loci of Campanula species and artificial flowers.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128577.s001

(DOC)

S2 Table. Mean absolute and relative amount (%) of volatile compounds in all headspace samples collected from inflorescences of six Campanula species.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128577.s002

(XLS)

Acknowledgments

We thank Hans-Joachim Flügel (Lebendiges Bienenmuseum Knüllwald) and Jochen Fründ (Uni-versity of Göttingen, Germany) for providing nests of Chelostoma rapunculi, the team from the Botanical Garden of the University of Ulm for providing Campanula fields to rear the bees used in our experiments, Irmgard Schäffler for support in color measurements, and an anonymous referee for the valuable comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: PMP MA SD. Performed the experiments: PMP. Analyzed the data: PMP MA SD. Wrote the paper: PMP MA SD.

References

  1. 1. Heinrich B. Energetics of Pollination. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1975;6:139–70.
  2. 2. Lunau K, Maier EJ. Innate colour preferences of flower visitors. J Comp Physiol A. 1995;177:1–19.
  3. 3. Simonds V, Plowright CMS. How do bumblebees first find flowers? Unlearned approach responses and habituation. Anim Behav. 2004;67:379–86.
  4. 4. Wright GA, Schiestl FP. The evolution of floral scent: the influence of olfactory learning by insect pollinators on the honest signalling of floral rewards. Funct Ecol. 2009;23:841–51.
  5. 5. Molet M, Chittka L, Raine NE. How floral odours are learned inside the bumblebee (Bombus terrestris) nest. Naturwissenschaften. 2009;96:213–9. pmid:18949450
  6. 6. Dobson HEM, Peng YS. Digestion of pollen components by larvae of the flower-specialist bee Chelostoma florisomne (Hymenoptera: Megachilidae). J Insect Physiol. 1997;43:89–100. pmid:12769933
  7. 7. Praz CJ, Müller A, Dorn S. Specialized bees fail to develop on non-host pollen: do plants chemically protect their pollen. Ecology. 2008;89:795–804. pmid:18459342
  8. 8. Sedivy C, Praz CJ, Müller A, Widmer A, Dorn S. Patterns of host-plant choice in bees of the genus Chelostoma: the constraint hypothesis of host-range evolution in bees. Evolution. 2008;62:2487–507. pmid:18637958
  9. 9. Robertson C. Heterotropic bees. Ecology. 1925;6:412–36.
  10. 10. Cane JH, Sipes S. Characterizing floral specialization by bees: analytical methods and revised lexicon for oligolecty. In: Waser NM, Ollerton J, editors. Plant-pollinator interactions: from specialization to generalization. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; 2006. p. 99–121.
  11. 11. Dötterl S, Vereecken JN. The chemical ecology and evolution of bee-flower interactions: a review and perspectives. Can J Zool. 2010;88:668–97.
  12. 12. Chittka L, Raine NE. Recognition of flowers by pollinators. Curr Opin Plant Biol. 2006;9:428–35. pmid:16713328
  13. 13. Leonard AS, Masek P. Multisensory integration of colors and scents: insights from bees and flowers. J Comp Physiol A. 2014;200:463–74. pmid:24710696
  14. 14. Weiss MR. Floral colour changes as cues for pollinators. Nature. 1991;35:227–9.
  15. 15. Cresswell JE, Robertson AW. Discrimination by pollen-collecting bumblebees among differentially rewarding flowers of an alpine wildflower, Campanula rotundifolia (Campanulaceae). Oikos. 1994;69:304–8.
  16. 16. Goulson D, Chapman JW, Hughes WOH. Discrimination of unrewarding flowers by bees; direct detection of rewards and use of repellent scent marks. J Insect Behav. 2001;14:669–78.
  17. 17. Schlindwein C, Westerkamp C, Carvalho AT, Milet-Pinheiro P. Visual signalling of nectar-offering flowers and specific morphological traits favour robust bee pollinators in the mass-flowering tree Handroanthus impetiginosus (Bignoniaceae). Bot J Linn Soc. 2014;176:396–407.
  18. 18. Kunze J, Gumbert A. The combined effect of color and odor on flower choice behavior of bumblebees in flower mimic systems. Behav Ecol. 2001;12:447–56.
  19. 19. Burger H, Ayasse M, Häberlein CM, Schulz S, Dötterl S. Echium and Pontechium specific floral cues for host-plant recognition by the oligolectic bee Hoplitis adunca. S Afr J Bot. 2010;76:788–95.
  20. 20. Milet-Pinheiro P, Ayasse M, Schlindwein C, Dobson HEM, Dötterl S. Host location by visual and olfactory floral cues in an oligolectic bee: innate and learned behavior. Behav Ecol. 2012;23:531–8.
  21. 21. Schäffler I, Balao F, Dötterl S. Floral and vegetative cues in oil-secreting and non-oil-secreting Lysimachia species. Ann Bot. 2012;110:125–38. pmid:22634256
  22. 22. Papadopulos AST, Powell MP, Pupulin F, Warner J, Hawkins JA, Salamin N, et al. Convergent evolution of floral signals underlies the success of Neotropical orchids. Proc R Soc B. 2013;280:20130960.
  23. 23. Williams NH. Floral fragrances as cues in animal behavior. In: Jones EC, Little RJ, editors. Handbook of experimental pollination biology. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold; 1983. p. 50–72.
  24. 24. Knudsen JT, Eriksson R, Gershenzon J, Stahl B. Diversity and distribution of floral scent. Bot Rev. 2006;72:1–120.
  25. 25. Ayasse M, Schiestl FP, Paulus HF, Löfstedt C, Hansson B, Ibarra F, et al. Evolution of reproductive strategies in the sexually deceptive orchid Ophrys sphegodes: how does flower-specific variation of odor signals influence reproductive success? Evolution. 2000;54:1995–2006. pmid:11209776
  26. 26. Dobson HEM, Bergström G. The ecology of pollen odors. Plant Syst Evol. 2000;222:63–87.
  27. 27. Dobson HEM, Danielson EM, Van Wesep ID. Pollen odor chemicals as modulators of bumble bee foraging on Rosa rugosa Thunb. (Rosaceae). Plant Species Biol. 1999;14:153–66.
  28. 28. Giurfa M, Núñez J, Chittka L, Menzel R. Colour preferences of flower-naive honeybees. J Comp Physiol A. 1995;177:247–59.
  29. 29. Howell AD, Alarcón R. Osmia bees (Hymenoptera: Megachilidae) can detect nectar-rewarding flowers using olfactory cues. Anim Behav. 2007;74:199–205.
  30. 30. Westrich P. Die Wildbienen Baden-Württembergs: Spezieller Teil: Die Gattungen und Arten. Stuttgart: Eugen Ulmer; 1989. 437–972 p.
  31. 31. Müller A, Krebs A, Amiet F. Bienen: Mitteleuropäische Gattungen, Lebensweise, Beobahctung. München: Naturbuch-Verlag; 1997. 384 p.
  32. 32. Rosenbauer A. Campanulaceae. In: Sebald O, Seybold S, Philippi G, Wörz A, editors. Die Farn- und Blütenpflanzen Baden-Württembergs Band 5: Spezieller Teil (Spermatophyta, Unterklasse Asteridae) Buddlejaceae bis Caprifoliaceae. Stuttgart, Germany: Verlag Eugen Ulmer; 1996. p. 417–49.
  33. 33. Kovacic S. The genus Campanula L. (Campanulaceae) in Croatia, circum-adriatic and west balkan region. Acta Bot Croat. 2004;63:171–202.
  34. 34. Milet-Pinheiro P, Ayasse M, Dobson HEM, Schlindwein C, Francke W, Dötterl S. The chemical basis of host-plant recognition in a specialized bee pollinator. J Chem Ecol. 2013;39:1347–60. pmid:24233444
  35. 35. Nilsson LA. Mimesis of bellflower Campanula by the red helleborine orchid Cephalanthera rubra. Nature. 1983;305:799–800.
  36. 36. Müller A, Diener S, Schnyder S, Stutz K, Sedivy C, Dorn S. Quantitative pollen requirements of solitary bees: implications for bee conservation and the evolution of bee-flower relationships. Biol Conserv. 2006;130:604–15.
  37. 37. Peitsch D, Fietz A, Hertel H, Souza J, Ventura DF, Menzel R. The Spectral Input Systems of Hymenopteran Insects and Their Receptor-Based Colour Vision. J Comp Physiol A Sens Neural Behav Physiol. 1992;170:23–40.
  38. 38. Chittka L. The colour hexagon: a chromaticity diagram based on photoreceptor excitations as a generalized representation of colour opponency. J Comp Physiol A. 1992;170:533–43.
  39. 39. Chittka L, Kevan PG. Flower colour as advertisement. In: Dafni A, Kevan PG, Husband BC, editors. Practical Pollination Biology. Cambridge: Enviroquest Ltd; 2005. p. 157–96.
  40. 40. Raine NE, Chittka L. The adaptive significance of sensory bias in a foraging context: floral colour preferences in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Plos One. 2007;556:1–8.
  41. 41. Chittka L, Shmida A, Troje N, Menzel R. Ultraviolet as a componet of flowers reflections, and the colour perception of hymenoptera. Vision Res. 1994;34:1489–508. pmid:8023461
  42. 42. Dyer AG, Chittka L. Biological significance of distinguishing between similar colours in spectrally variable illumination: bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) as a case study. J Comp Physiol A. 2004;190:105–14. pmid:14652688
  43. 43. Dyer AG, Chittka L. Fine colour discrimination requires differential conditioning in bumblebees. Naturwissenschaften. 2004;91:224–7. pmid:15146269
  44. 44. Burger H, Dötterl S, Ayasse M. Host-plant finding and recognition by visual and olfactory floral cues in an oligolectic bee. Funct Ecol. 2010;24:1234–40.
  45. 45. Arnold SEJ, Faruq S, Savolainen V, McOwan PW, Chittka L. FReD: the floral reflectance database—A web portal for analyses of flower colour. Plos One. 2010;5:e14287. pmid:21170326
  46. 46. Dötterl S, Jürgens A. Spatial fragrance patterns in flowers of Silene latifolia: Lilac compounds as olfactory nectar guides? Plant Syst Evol. 2005;255:99–109.
  47. 47. Adams RP. Identification of Essential Oil Components by Gas Chromatography/Mass Spectrometry. Carol Stream, Illinois: Allured Publishing Corporation; 2007. https://doi.org/10.1002/cbdv.201100039 pmid:22083911
  48. 48. McDonald JH. Handbook of biological statistics. 2nd ed. Baltimore: Sparky House Publishing; 2009. 313 p.
  49. 49. Clarke KR, Gorley RN. Primer v6: User Manual/Tutorial. Plymouth: Primer-E; 2006. 91 p.
  50. 50. Burger H, Dötterl S, Häberlein C, Schulz S, Ayasse M. An arthropod deterrent attracts specialised bees to their host plants. Oecologia. 2012;168:727–36. pmid:21964494
  51. 51. Burger BV. Mammalian semiochemicals. Top Curr Chem. 2005;240:231–78.
  52. 52. Francke W, Kitching W. Spiroacetals in Insects. Curr Org Chem. 2001;5:233–51.
  53. 53. Francke W, Bartels J, Meyer H, Schröder F, Kohnle U, Baader E, et al. Semiochemicals from bark beetles: new results, remarks, and reflections. J Chem Ecol. 1995;21:1043–63. pmid:24234418
  54. 54. Huber DPW, Gries R, Borden JH, Pierce HD. Two Pheromones of Coniferophagous Bark Beetles Found in the Bark of Nonhost Angiosperms. J Chem Ecol. 1999;25:805–16.
  55. 55. Zhang Q-H, Schlyter F, Birgersson G. Bark volatiles from nonhost angiosperm trees of spruce bark beetle, Ips typographus (L.) (Coleoptera: Scolytidae): Chemical and electrophysiological analysis. Chemoecology. 2000;10:69–80.
  56. 56. Dötterl S, Füssel U, Jürgens A, Aas G. 1,4-Dimethoxybenzene, a floral scent compound in willows that attracts an oligolectic bee. J Chem Ecol. 2005;31:2993–8. pmid:16258713
  57. 57. Carvalho AT, Dötterl S, Schlindwein C. An aromatic volatile attracts oligolectic bee pollinators in an interdependent bee-plant relationship. J Chem Ecol. 2014.
  58. 58. Andrews ES, Theis N, Adler LS. Pollinator and herbivore attraction to Curcubita floral volatiles. J Chem Ecol. 2007;33:1682–91. pmid:17659427
  59. 59. Roquet C, Sáez L, Aldasoro JJ, Susanna A, Alarcón ML, Garcia-Jacas N. Natural Delineation, Molecular Phylogeny and Floral Evolution in Campanula. Syst Bot. 2008;33:203–17.
  60. 60. Mansion G, Parolly G, Crowl AA, Mavrodiev E, Cellinese N, Oganesian M, et al. How to handle speciose clades? Mass taxon-sampling as a strategy towards illuminating the natural history of Campanula (Campanuloideae). Plos One. 2012;7:e50076. pmid:23209646
  61. 61. Wcislo WT, Cane JH. Floral resource utilization by solitary bees (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) and exploitation of their stored foods by natural enemies. Annu Rev Entomol. 1996;41:257–86. pmid:15012330
  62. 62. Raguso RA. Why are some floral nectars scented? Ecology. 2004;85:1486–94. WOS:000222727500003.
  63. 63. Dötterl S, Milchreit K, Schäffler I. Behavioural plasticity and sex differences in host finding of a specialized bee species. J Comp Physiol A. 2011;197:1119–26. pmid:21904915
  64. 64. Praz CJ, Müller A, Dorn S. Host recognition in a pollen-specialist bee: evidence for a genetic basis. Apidologie. 2008;39:547–57.
  65. 65. Linsley EG. The ecology of solitary bees. Hilgardia. 1958;27:543–99.
  66. 66. Blionis GJ, Vokou D. Pollination ecology of Campanula species on Mt Olympos, Greece. Ecography. 2001;24:287–97.