Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Sustained Effects of Developmental Exposure to Ethanol on Zebrafish Anxiety-Like Behaviour

Abstract

In zebrafish developmentally exposed to ambient ethanol (20mM-50mM) 1–9 days post fertilization (dpf), the cortisol response to stress has been shown to be significantly attenuated in larvae, juveniles and 6 month old adults. These data are somewhat at variance with similar studies in mammals, which often show heightened stress responses. To test whether these cortisol data correlate with behavioural changes in treated animals, anxiety-like behaviour of zebrafish larvae (9dpf and 10dpf) and juveniles (23dpf) was tested in locomotor assays designed to this end. In open field tests treated animals were more exploratory, spending significantly less time at the periphery of the arena. Behavioural effects of developmental exposure to ethanol were sustained in 6-month-old adults, as judged by assessment of thigmotaxis, novel tank diving and scototaxis. Like larvae and juveniles, developmentally treated adults were generally more exploratory, and spent less time at the periphery of the arena in thigmotaxis tests, less time at the bottom of the tank in the novel tank diving tests, and less time in the dark area in scototaxis tests. The conclusion that ethanol-exposed animals showed less anxiety-like behaviour was validated by comparison with the effects of diazepam treatment, which in thigmotaxis and novel tank diving tests had similar effects to ethanol pretreatment. There is thus a possible link between the hypophyseal-pituitary-interrenal axis and the behavioural actions of developmental ethanol exposure. The mechanisms require further elucidation.

Introduction

The damaging effects of ethanol exposure during development in humans have been amply described, and its subsequent behavioural consequences, are part of a range of symptoms collectively known as Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder [1]. Some of these symptoms, like growth and facial defects, are apparent at an early age [2,3]. Others become clear in later life, for example children from alcoholic mothers are more likely to become drug addicts in adolescence or in adulthood, and may develop personality and psychotic disorders [4,5].

The mechanisms underlying such sustained effects of prenatal ethanol exposure in humans are still obscure, but it is now widely believed that the hypophyseal–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis is involved [6,7] since prenatal ethanol exposure frequently results in increased HPA tone and heightened HPA responsiveness in infancy which persists through adolescence into adulthood [6,8,9]. In rats too, animals prenatally exposed to ethanol show differences in HPA tone, revealed by elevated plasma ACTH and corticosterone levels in response to acute and chronic stressors.[1015]. Additionally, both in humans and rats developmental exposure to ethanol evokes changes in behavioural measures of stress-reactivity in later life [1624].

Effects of ethanol on the HPA axis are not only seen during development. In post-natally treated animals the HPA is invariably perturbed by alcohol, with severity depending on several factors including age of the animal, dose and duration of exposure [2527]. These and other studies have led to the widely accepted hypothesis that prenatal ethanol exposure induces long lasting adaptation at multiple levels within the HPA axis, resulting in alterations in both HPA drive and feedback regulation [17,28,29]. All of this raises many interesting questions, most notably what is the precise nature of the link between developmental alcohol exposure and the HPA?

It is clear that animal models replicate many of the human findings [3032]. While much value has been obtained from the study of various mammals, mostly rodents, others have sought answers in the zebrafish, in which tractability and transparency, conferring relative ease of use, are key advantages [3337]. However, in marked contrast to the previous studies in humans and mammalian models, in zebrafish it was found that after developmental exposure to ambient ethanol (20mM-50mM) 1–9 days post fertilization (dpf), the cortisol response to stress was not heightened but significantly attenuated in larvae, juveniles and 6 month old adults [38]. Accordingly, it is appropriate to determine whether this unexpected finding of a decreased cortisol response to stress is accompanied with reduced anxiety-like behaviour, as the concept of a linkage between the HPA and behaviour would suggest.

Several behavioural measures have been developed to assess stress or “anxiety” levels in zebrafish, including thigmotaxis (time spent at the edge of apparatus), scototaxis (time spent at the bright side of tanks), “freezing” and novel tank diving (time spent at the bottom of tanks) [3941], as well as tests for social interaction, or shoaling. Here we exploit established assays thought to reflect stress-related responses, to determine whether the clear effects of developmental ethanol exposure on the cortisol response of the HPI are associated with changes in behaviour.

Materials and Methods

Animal maintenance

All animal work was carried out following approval from the Queen Mary Research Ethics Committee, and under license from the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. Care was taken to minimize the numbers of animals used in this experiment in accordance with the ARRIVE guidelines (http://www.nc3rs.org.uk/page.asp?id=1357). All behavioural experiments were carried out with systematic variation and randomization of housing allocation by treatment, as suggested by Parker [42]. Where relevant animals were sacrificed using terminal anaethesia with tricaine or other approved anaesthetic.

Zebrafish (Danio rerio) (Tuebingen wild type) were kept on a constant 14h:10h light:dark cycle at 28°C and fed 3 times a day with flake food and brine shrimp. All fish were bred and reared in the aquarium facility at Queen Mary University of London, licensed by the UK Home Office. Fish water used was prepared by dissolving sodium bicarbonate (0.9mM), calcium sulphate (0.05mM) and marine salts (0.018g/l)(Sigma, Poole, UK).

Embryos were separated from unfertilized ova and selected at the 8-cell stage to minimise age differences. For further accuracy embryos were staged using head-trunk angle and the optic vesicle length at 24 hours post fertilisation (hpf) [43]. They were then grouped in Petri dishes (Sterilin, Newport Gwent, UK) containing 50 embryos in 40ml of fish water for each treatment (ethanol or control) and reared in an incubator set at 28°. Embryos were collected and treated on 3 separate days.

Larvae were fed with Zmsystems ZM-000 high protein food particles (Tecniplast UK, London) from 5dpf-10dpf, ZM-100 and paramecium from 11dpf-14dpf, and ZM-200 and brineshrimp from 14dpf-30dpf. At one month of age, animals were transferred into aquaria where they were fed ZMsystems flake food and brineshrimp. For developmental ethanol exposure, treated larvae were exposed from 1–9dpf to 20mM or 50mM GPR ethanol (VWR, Lutterworth UK). Controls were handled similarly, but ethanol was omitted. For each experiment, control and experimental larvae were age and size matched: adults were sex matched in addition.

Image collection

For zebrafish larval and juvenile thigmotaxis assays, a high-throughput imaging system for automated analysis was used. This comprises a 15-megapixel infrared Imaginsource digital camera DMK21AF04 attached to the lower shelf of an acrylic cabinet to allow filming from below the testing plate placed on the upper shelf. Either a 12-well plate (for 9dpf and 10dpf larvae) or a 6-well plate (for 23dpf juveniles) was used for this assay.

For the automated analysis of zebrafish adult behaviour a 15-megapixel SONY digital camera was used to film from above the tank, except in the case of the novel tank diving assay in which filming was from the side. Captured footage was automatically analysed using EthoVision XT 10 (Noldus, Wageningen).

Thigmotaxis

Larvae and juveniles.

Methods followed those of Richendrfer et al [44]. Larvae or juveniles were raised in Petri dishes or nursery tanks. One hour prior to the assay, animals were transferred in 6-well plates by pipetting with no more than 12 larvae or 3 juveniles per well. Wells were filled with 10ml of fish water. Experimental animals were transferred by pipetting them individually to a well in a 12-well plate (22mm diameter, used for 9dpf larvae) or a 6-well plate (34mm diameter, used for 23dpf juveniles), and immediately placed on the recording apparatus as described above.

The automated software (EthoVision) analyses the time spent at the outer zone of the well in the 6-well plate. The outer zone was defined as the region 4mm (average body length of a 9dpf larva) or 8mm (average body length of a 23dpf juvenile) from the edge of the well. Larvae were filmed for 5–10min, and juveniles for 5min. For characterization of the effects of diazepam on thigmotaxis, larvae were pretreated with 0.1mg/l diazepam (cf. reference [44]) for 6 minutes prior to thigmotaxis assessment. Diazepam was present at the same concentration throughout the thigmotaxis assay.

Adults.

Experimental and control adults were housed in adjacent compartments of the same tank for 2 weeks prior to the start of the assay. Thigmotaxis was assayed using white opaque polypropylene circular tanks (410mm height x 320mm diameter) filled with 2l of fish water. The outer zone was defined as the region 4cm (the average length of an adult fish) from the edge of the tank, and the time spent in this zone was determined.

Tests were performed during the light phase at least 2 hours after lights on and feeding, between the hours of 11.00am and 5.00pm. Animals were carefully transferred into the tanks using hand-nets and immediately filmed for 6 minutes. Ethanol treated and control groups were tested alternately in four identical tanks. For characterization of the effects of diazepam on adult thigmotaxis, zebrafish were pre-incubated in 5mg/l diazepam (cf reference [45]) for 6 minutes prior to thigmotaxis assessment. Diazepam was present at the same concentration throughout the thigmotaxis assay.

Novel tank diving

Methods followed those of Parker et al. [37]. Experimental and control adults were housed in adjacent compartments of the same tank, for 2 weeks prior to the start of the assay. Novel tank diving was then assayed using trapezoid tanks (152mm height, 279mm top length/225mm bottom length, 71mm width) filled with 1.5l fish water.

Tests were performed during the light phase (9am-5pm). Animals were transferred to the novel tanks using hand-nets and immediately filmed for 5 minutes. Ethanol treated and control groups were tested alternately in identical tanks. Diving was assessed as the time spent in the lower third of the tank (approx. 50mm). For characterization of the effects of diazepam on novel tank diving, zebrafish were pre-incubated in 5mg/l diazepam (cf reference [45]) for 6 minutes prior to transfer to the novel tank environment. Diazepam was present at the same concentration throughout the novel tank diving assay.

Scototaxis

Methods followed those of [46]. Experimental and control adults were housed in adjacent compartments of the same tank for 2 weeks prior to the start of the assay. White opaque tanks (330mm length x 160mm width x 130mm height) containing 2l fish water were used. They were divided into two compartments by a black opaque acrylic divider, with a square hole in the middle (50 x 50mm). One side of the tank was exposed to light and the other side was darkened.

Animals were transferred using hand-nets into the lit side of the tanks and immediately filmed for 9 minutes on the lit side. Treated and control groups were tested alternately in identical tanks.

Statistics

Stress reactivity data were fitted to a linear mixed effects model [47] with fixed effects that included ‘ethanol dose’ and ‘time’ using R software (R Development Team). ‘Fish ID’ nested in ‘housing tank’ was included as a random effect. Distance travelled was entered as a covariate in all models to account for immobility and darting periods. The dependent variables were the period of time spent in the designated zones. Post-hoc Student’s T-tests were applied to characterise simple main effects and interactions.

Results

Thigmotaxis

Larvae and juveniles.

Control and treated animals were tested at 9dpf, 10dpf and 23dpf stages for differences in thigmotaxis, thus during ethanol treatment, and, their siblings, at 10dpf and 23dpf, one day or two weeks after treatment had ended. Decreased time spent at the periphery of the wells was observed at all stages (Fig 1), although there was no obvious dose relationship at the two concentrations of ethanol used. Zebrafish 9dpf larvae acutely exposed to diazepam (0.1mg/L) exhibited reduced time spent at the edge of wells (Fig 1G and 1H). There were no differences between the groups in the distances travelled. Experiments were repeated on 3 separate occasions using approximately 50 animals in each treatment group on each occasion

thumbnail
Fig 1.

Stress-related behaviour assessed by thigmotaxis in zebrafish larvae A,B) 9dpf, C,D) 10dpf, E,F) 23dpf juveniles. G,H) Effect of diazepam on larval stress-reactivity assessed by thigmotaxis. Time course of average time spent each minute at the edge of the apparatus (A, C, E), overall average time spent per minute at the edge of the apparatus (B, D, F). (A-D) Developmental ethanol exposure decreased thigmotaxis at both 9dpf (A,B: F 2,105 = 4.76, P<0.05) and 10dpf (C,D: F 2, 285 = 6.69, P<0.05), with the greatest difference between 20mM ethanol treatment and the control. Siblings of the same animals were raised for another 2 weeks and tested as 23 dpf juveniles (E,F). These juveniles exhibited a similar thigmotaxis response as at 9dpf, with decreased thigmotaxis in ethanol treated animals compared to controls (F 2,146 = 2.93, P<0.05). (G-H) Larvae acutely treated with diazepam for 6 minutes exhibited significantly reduced time spent at the edges of the wells compared to controls (F 1, 259 = 5.47, P<0.01). There were no significant differences in distance travelled. Post-hoc t-test: *** P<0.001, ** P<0.01.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148425.g001

Adults.

Adult zebrafish that had been treated developmentally with 20mM ethanol exhibited a decrease in time spent at the edge of the circular tank compared to controls (Fig 2), however, animals treated with 50mM ethanol showed no change. Zebrafish 6-month old adults acutely exposed to diazepam (5mg/L) exhibited reduced time spent at the edge of the tanks (Fig 2D). There were no differences between the groups in the distances travelled. Experiments were repeated on 6 separate occasions using approximately 20 animals in each treatment group on each occasion

thumbnail
Fig 2. Stress-reactivity of 6-month old zebrafish as assessed by thigmotaxis.

A) Time course of average time spent per minute at the edge of the apparatus, B) overall average time spent per minute at the edge of the apparatus, C) distance travelled during thigmotaxis, D) Effect of diazepam on adult zebrafish stress-reactivity assessed by thigmotaxis. A,B) Adult zebrafish that had been experimentally exposed to ethanol spent decreased time at the edge of the tank, (F 2, 127 = 3.09, P<0.05). There were no significant differences in distance travelled (C). Adults acutely exposed to diazepam exhibited reduced time spent at the edges of the tanks compared to controls (F 1, 4.98 = 5.44, P<0.001) (D). Post-hoc t-test, * P<0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148425.g002

Novel tank diving

Adults treated with ethanol during development exhibited decreased bottom dwelling in the novel tank. There were no differences between the groups in the distances travelled. (Fig 3C). Zebrafish 6-month old adults acutely exposed to diazepam (5mg/L) exhibited reduced time spent at the bottom of the tanks (Fig 3D). Experiments were repeated on 3 separate occasions using approximately 20 animals in each treatment group on each occasion

thumbnail
Fig 3. Stress-reactivity measured by novel tank diving in 6-month old adult zebrafish.

A) Time course of average time spent per minute at the bottom of the tank, B) overall average time spent per minute at the bottom of the tank each minute, C) mean distance travelled per minute during novel tank diving. D) Effect of diazepam on zebrafish stress-reactivity assessed by novel tank diving. A,B. Zebrafish that had been developmentally exposed to ethanol showed reduced bottom dwelling (F 2, 682 = 3.47, P<0.05). There were no significant differences in distance travelled (C). D. Diazepam also significantly reduced time spent by adults at the bottom of the tanks compared to controls (F 1, 408 = 5.45, P<0.001). Post hoc t-test, *** P<0.001.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148425.g003

Scototaxis

Early ethanol exposure caused an increase in time spent on the bright side of the tank compared to controls (Fig 4). This effect was more prominent with animals that had been developmentally exposed to 20mM ethanol. Due to assay limitations, animal tracking in the dark side of the tanks was not recorded; therefore distance travelled for scototaxis was not assessed. Experiments were repeated on 3 separate occasions using approximately 10 animals in each treatment group on each occasion

thumbnail
Fig 4. Stress-reactivity measured by scototaxis in 6-month old adult zebrafish A) Time course of average time spent at the bright side of the apparatus and B) overall average time spent at the bright side of the apparatus.

Adult zebrafish that had been developmentally exposed to ethanol spent more time on the bright side of the tank (F 2, 31 = 3.85, P<0.05). Post hoc t-test, ** P<0.01, * P<0.05.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0148425.g004

Discussion

The HPA axis, and extrahypothalamic CRH, are thought to be involved in adult responses to both acute and chronic ethanol exposure, and show characteristic responses to ethanol and other substance abuse in humans and in mammalian models [4850], and also in zebrafish [3337]. We recently demonstrated that developmental exposure to ethanol in zebrafish causes a sustained effect on whole body cortisol. In these experiments, the cortisol response to stress was dampened in both larvae and juveniles 1 day and 2 weeks after exposure, and also in adults, 6 months after treatment [38].

It is now clear from the present results that this change in cortisol response is accompanied by behavioural changes. The behavioural characteristics studied here are often interpreted as measures of fear or anxiety. Thus animals that are threatened by new surroundings show characteristic responses in thigmotaxis, scototaxis and tank diving that can be interpreted as strategies to minimise detection. In general, the ethanol-evoked changes shown in Figs 14 in thigmotaxis, scototaxis and novel tank diving, show exploratory movement indicating relative absence of stress, which fits well with the lower response to stress in the cortisol data [38]. This interpretation is supported by data shown in Figs 13 indicating diazepam treatment has a similar effect to reduce thigmotaxis, and tank diving as evoked by developmental ethanol exposure (Figs 14).

The association between these two sets of data, cortisol and behavioural does not necessarily imply causality. It is nevertheless striking how often these are linked.

Stress is a complex concept. It is impossible to quantitate, except through secondary indicators. Of these, cortisol is perhaps the most frequently used (or corticosterone in rats or mice). Caution must be exercised however, because its actual function in the response to stress remains obscure, and its use as a measure of stress is confounded by its regulation by other factors not obviously stress-related, such as the time of day [51]. For this and other reasons it may be unwise to extrapolate from the human species (in which stress, though widespread, remains a subjective phenomenon, not measurable by observers) to the zebrafish whose physiological situation and demands are very different.

Nevertheless, the similarities between mammalian and zebrafish responses to stressful situations have been used to promote the view that the zebrafish can be used as a model for human emotional states such as anxiety, resulting from stress [40,41,52]. This gains credibility from the actions of drugs, especially known anxiolytics such as diazepam, on zebrafish behaviour in response to stress [53,54] c.f. Figs 13. It is also supported by relating behavioural stress responses to whole body cortisol [55,56].

Using this interpretation then, the present results suggest that developmental ethanol exposure leads to a phenotype that is hyporeactive to stress, as evidenced by reduction in both behavioural measures of anxiety and cortisol levels. Going further, developmental exposure to ethanol not only appears to be anxiolytic, as judged by thigmotaxis in larvae and juveniles 1 day or 2 weeks following treatment, but also in adults some 6 months after the cessation of treatment as assessed by novel tank diving, thigmotaxis and scototaxis (Figs 14).

These data thus partly confirm but partly contrast with those of others. In mammals the behavioural and cortisol response to ethanol is complex. Acutely, ethanol stimulates corticosterone secretion in adult rats [25,57], but longer exposure is associated with depressed HPA activity [27]. Ethanol withdrawal produces anxiogenic symptoms, including elevated corticosteroid, in humans, rodents, and in zebrafish [27,5862]. Furthermore, developmental exposure to ethanol in rodents leads in later life to similarly enhanced HPA responses to stress as in the human species [31,63]

More pertinent to the present studies are those of Fernandes and Gerlai [64] and Bailey et al [65] both of whom examined long term effects of early developmental ethanol exposure on subsequent adult behaviour in zebrafish. The periods of exposure were far shorter than those used in the present study, just 2h at 24hpf [64], and for 2h at 8hpf or 3h at 24hpf [65] and the concentrations of ethanol used were up to 1% (171mM) [64], or 1–3% [65], thus approximately 5–15 times higher than the maximal concentration (50mM) used here. Fernandes and Gerlai [64] used a shoaling assay, and showed that adult fish (6 months) that had been exposed to higher concentrations significantly distanced themselves from a computer animated zebrafish shoal. Bailey et al [65] tested their treated animals at 2 months, and found significant effects in a tap startle assay, and in novel tank diving. Both of these tests showed greater activity in the treated animals relative to the controls. Bailey et al [65] also conducted novel tank diving assays and showed that treated animals were more exploratory and spent less time at the bottom of the tank, consistent with the results reported here.

There may be differences in interpretation of all such behavioural data, but ours tends to support the view that 1–9 day ethanol treatment has sustained actions that are primarily anxiolytic in zebrafish, thus contrasting with rodent data do not reflect those in rodents in which prenatal ethanol subsequently produces symptoms of anxiety and depression [29,66]. Clearly differences in handling and procedure can give variations in results, and we have found that use of somewhat different treatment protocols may give some data at variance with that described here ([37] and unpublished).

As mentioned in the introduction, we opted to use ethanol doses that reflect ethanol concentrations experienced by mammalian fetuses during development, which yielded in an anxiolytic phenotype. We acknowledge that different ethanol doses could potentially yield a different phenotype under this same exposure period, as it has occurred with mice and zebrafish during different points of development [67,68].

The more important point that we seek to emphasise in this paper however, is that taking the present data together with that from our previous study [38], there is a clear relationship between behaviour and HPI function, reflecting similar results in mammals [50,5860]. The mechanisms merit further investigation.

Acknowledgments

Caroline Brennan is a Royal Society Industry Research Fellow. Matteo Baiamonte held a BBSRC research studentship. The work was supported by grant G1000053 from the National Centre for replacement, refinement and reduction of animals in research, UK.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: GPV CHB. Performed the experiments: MB. Analyzed the data: MB MOP. Wrote the paper: GPV MOP MB CHB.

References

  1. 1. Paintner A, Williams AD, Burd L (2012) Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders—implications for child neurology, part 1: prenatal exposure and dosimetry. Journal of child neurology 27: 258–263. pmid:22351188
  2. 2. Colangelo W, Jones DG (1982) The fetal alcohol syndrome: a review and assessment of the syndrome and its neurological sequelae. Prog Neurobiol 19: 271–314. pmid:6134303
  3. 3. Jones KL, Smith DW (1973) Recognition of the fetal alcohol syndrome in early infancy. Lancet 302: 999–1001. pmid:4127281
  4. 4. O'Connor MJ, Paley B (2009) Psychiatric conditions associated with prenatal alcohol exposure. Dev Disabil Res Rev 15: 225–234. pmid:19731386
  5. 5. Evrard SG (2010) Prenatal alcohol exposure as an etiological factor in neuropsychiatric diseases of childhood, adolescence and adulthood. Vertex 21: 260–265. pmid:21188313
  6. 6. Uban KA, Comeau WL, Ellis LA, Galea LA, Weinberg J (2013) Basal regulation of HPA and dopamine systems is altered differentially in males and females by prenatal alcohol exposure and chronic variable stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38: 1953–1966. pmid:23579081
  7. 7. Mead EA, Sarkar DK (2014) Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders and their transmission through genetic and epigenetic mechanisms. Front Genet 5: 154. pmid:24917878
  8. 8. Haley DW, Handmaker NS, Lowe J (2006) Infant stress reactivity and prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research 30: 2055–2064. pmid:17117971
  9. 9. Weinberg J, Sliwowska JH, Lan N, Hellemans KG (2008) Prenatal alcohol exposure: foetal programming, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and sex differences in outcome. J Neuroendocrinol 20: 470–488. pmid:18266938
  10. 10. Taylor AN, Branch BJ, Liu SH, Kokka N (1982) Long-term effects of fetal ethanol exposure on pituitary-adrenal response to stress. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 16: 585–589. pmid:7071091
  11. 11. Taylor AN, Branch BJ, Liu SH, Wiechmann AF, Hill MA, Kokka N (1981) Fetal exposure to ethanol enhances pituitary-adrenal and temperature responses to ethanol in adult rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 5: 237–246. pmid:7018304
  12. 12. Taylor AN, Branch BJ, Van Zuylen JE, Redei E (1988) Maternal alcohol consumption and stress responsiveness in offspring. Adv Exp Med Biol 245: 311–317. pmid:3067559
  13. 13. Weinberg J (1988) Hyperresponsiveness to stress: differential effects of prenatal ethanol on males and females. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 12: 647–652. pmid:3067609
  14. 14. Weinberg J (1992) Prenatal ethanol effects: sex differences in offspring stress responsiveness. Alcohol 9: 219–223. pmid:1605890
  15. 15. Osborn JA, Kim CK, Yu W, Herbert L, Weinberg J (1996) Fetal ethanol exposure alters pituitary-adrenal sensitivity to dexamethasone suppression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 21: 127–143. pmid:8774058
  16. 16. Haley DW, Handmaker NS, Lowe J (2006) Infant stress reactivity and prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 30: 2055–2064. pmid:17117971
  17. 17. Hellemans KG, Sliwowska JH, Verma P, Weinberg J (2010) Prenatal alcohol exposure: fetal programming and later life vulnerability to stress, depression and anxiety disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34: 791–807. pmid:19545588
  18. 18. Hellemans KG, Verma P, Yoon E, Yu WK, Young AH, Weinberg J (2010) Prenatal alcohol exposure and chronic mild stress differentially alter depressive- and anxiety-like behaviors in male and female offspring. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 34: 633–645. pmid:20102562
  19. 19. Hellemans KGC, Verma P, Yoon E, Yu W, Weinberg J (2008) Prenatal Alcohol Exposure Increases Vulnerability to Stress and Anxiety-Like Disorders in Adulthood. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1144: 154–175. pmid:19076375
  20. 20. Carneiro LM, Diogenes JP, Vasconcelos SM, Aragao GF, Noronha EC, Gomes PB, et al. (2005) Behavioral and neurochemical effects on rat offspring after prenatal exposure to ethanol. Neurotoxicol Teratol 27: 585–592. pmid:16039829
  21. 21. Slone JL, Redei EE (2002) Maternal alcohol and adrenalectomy: asynchrony of stress response and forced swim behavior. Neurotoxicol Teratol 24: 173–178. pmid:11943505
  22. 22. Slattery DA, Cryan JF (2012) Using the rat forced swim test to assess antidepressant-like activity in rodents. Nat Protocols 7: 1009–1014. pmid:22555240
  23. 23. Osborn JA, Kim CK, Steiger J, Weinberg J (1998) Prenatal ethanol exposure differentially alters behavior in males and females on the elevated plus maze. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 22: 685–696. pmid:9622451
  24. 24. Gabriel KI, Yu CL, Osborn JA, Weinberg J (2006) Prenatal ethanol exposure alters sensitivity to the effects of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) on behavior in the elevated plus-maze. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31: 1046–1056. pmid:16934947
  25. 25. Ellis FW (1966) Effect of ethanol on plasma corticosterone levels. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 153: 121–127. pmid:4288396
  26. 26. Boyd KN, Kumar S, O'Buckley TK, Morrow AL (2010) Chronic ethanol exposure produces tolerance to elevations in neuroactive steroids: mechanisms and reversal by exogenous ACTH. Journal of neurochemistry 115: 142–152. pmid:20633208
  27. 27. Richardson HN, Lee SY, O'Dell LE, Koob GF, Rivier CL (2008) Alcohol self-administration acutely stimulates the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, but alcohol dependence leads to a dampened neuroendocrine state. Eur J Neurosci 28: 1641–1653. pmid:18979677
  28. 28. Glavas MM, Ellis L, Yu WK, Weinberg J (2007) Effects of prenatal ethanol exposure on basal limbic-hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal regulation: role of corticosterone. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research 31: 1598–1610. pmid:17760789
  29. 29. Hellemans KG, Verma P, Yoon E, Yu WK, Young AH, Weinberg J (2010) Prenatal alcohol exposure and chronic mild stress differentially alter depressive- and anxiety-like behaviors in male and female offspring. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research 34: 633–645. pmid:20102562
  30. 30. Taylor AN, Branch BJ, Nelson LR, Lane LA, Poland RE (1986) Prenatal ethanol and ontogeny of pituitary-adrenal responses to ethanol and morphine. Alcohol 3: 255–259. pmid:3755954
  31. 31. Weinberg J, Taylor AN, Gianoulakis C (1996) Fetal ethanol exposure: hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal and beta-endorphin responses to repeated stress. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 20: 122–131. pmid:8651441
  32. 32. Kim CK, Giberson PK, Yu W, Zoeller RT, Weinberg J (1999) Effects of prenatal ethanol exposure on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to chronic cold stress in rats. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research 23: 301–310. pmid:10069560
  33. 33. Kily LJ, Cowe YC, Hussain O, Patel S, McElwaine S, Cotter FE, et al. (2008) Gene expression changes in a zebrafish model of drug dependency suggest conservation of neuro-adaptation pathways. J Exp Biol 211: 1623–1634. pmid:18456890
  34. 34. Brennan CH, Parmar A, Kily LK, Ananthathevan A, Doshi A, Patel S (2010) Conditioned place preference models of drug-dependence and relapse to drug seeking: studies with nicotine and ethanol. In: Kalueff AV, editor. Zebrafish models in neurobehavioral research. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  35. 35. Collier AD, Khan KM, Caramillo EM, Mohn RS, Echevarria DJ (2014) Zebrafish and conditioned place preference: A translational model of drug reward. Progress in neuro-psychopharmacology & biological psychiatry 55: 16–25.
  36. 36. Tran S, Gerlai R (2014) Recent advances with a novel model organism: Alcohol tolerance and sensitization in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Progress in neuro-psychopharmacology & biological psychiatry 55: 87–93.
  37. 37. Parker MO, Annan LV, Kanellopoulos AH, Brock AJ, Combe FJ, Baiamonte M, et al. (2014) The utility of zebrafish to study the mechanisms by which ethanol affects social behavior and anxiety during early brain development. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 55: 94–100. pmid:24690524
  38. 38. Baiamonte M, Brennan CH, Vinson GP (2015) Sustained action of developmental ethanol exposure on the cortisol response to stress in zebrafish larvae and adults. PloS one.
  39. 39. Grossman L, Utterback E, Stewart A, Gaikwad S, Chung KM, Suciu C, et al. (2010) Characterization of behavioral and endocrine effects of LSD on zebrafish. Behavioural brain research 214: 277–284. pmid:20561961
  40. 40. Maximino C, de Brito TM, da Silva Batista AW, Herculano AM, Morato S, Gouveia A Jr. (2010) Measuring anxiety in zebrafish: a critical review. Behav Brain Res 214: 157–171. pmid:20510300
  41. 41. Colwill RM, Creton R (2011) Imaging escape and avoidance behavior in zebrafish larvae. Rev Neurosci 22: 63–73. pmid:21572576
  42. 42. Parker MO (2015) Adult vertebrate behavioural aquatic toxicology: reliability and validity. Aquat Toxicol.
  43. 43. Kimmel CB, Ballard WW, Kimmel SR, Ullmann B, Schilling TF (1995) Stages of embryonic development of the zebrafish. Dev Dyn 203: 253–310. pmid:8589427
  44. 44. Richendrfer H, Pelkowski SD, Colwill RM, Creton R (2012) On the edge: pharmacological evidence for anxiety-related behavior in zebrafish larvae. Behavioural brain research 228: 99–106. pmid:22155488
  45. 45. Bencan Z, Sledge D, Levin ED (2009) Buspirone, chlordiazepoxide and diazepam effects in a zebrafish model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 94: 75–80. pmid:19643124
  46. 46. Maximino C, Marques de Brito T, Dias CA, Gouveia A Jr., Morato S (2010) Scototaxis as anxiety-like behavior in fish. Nat Protoc 5: 209–216. pmid:20134420
  47. 47. Lindstrom ML, Bates DM (1990) Nonlinear mixed effects models for repeated measures data. Biometrics 46: 673–687. pmid:2242409
  48. 48. Sarnyai Z, Shaham Y, Heinrichs SC (2001) The role of corticotropin-releasing factor in drug addiction. Pharmacological reviews 53: 209–243. pmid:11356984
  49. 49. Koob GF (2010) The role of CRF and CRF-related peptides in the dark side of addiction. Brain research 1314: 3–14. pmid:19912996
  50. 50. Vinson GP, Brennan CH (2013) Addiction and the adrenal cortex. Endocr Connect 2: R1–R14.
  51. 51. Vinson GP (2009) The adrenal cortex and life. Mol Cell Endocrinol 300: 2–6. pmid:18840500
  52. 52. Egan RJ, Bergner CL, Hart PC, Cachat JM, Canavello PR, Elegante MF, et al. (2009) Understanding behavioral and physiological phenotypes of stress and anxiety in zebrafish. Behav Brain Res 205: 38–44. pmid:19540270
  53. 53. Grossman L, Utterback E, Stewart A, Gaikwad S, Chung KM, Suciu C, et al. (2010) Characterization of behavioral and endocrine effects of LSD on zebrafish. Behav Brain Res 214: 277–284. pmid:20561961
  54. 54. Stewart A, Wu N, Cachat J, Hart P, Gaikwad S, Wong K, et al. (2011) Pharmacological modulation of anxiety-like phenotypes in adult zebrafish behavioral models. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 35: 1421–1431. pmid:21122812
  55. 55. Cachat J, Stewart A, Grossman L, Gaikwad S, Kadri F, Chung KM, et al. (2010) Measuring behavioral and endocrine responses to novelty stress in adult zebrafish. Nat Protoc 5: 1786–1799. pmid:21030954
  56. 56. Abreu MS, Koakoski G, Ferreira D, Oliveira TA, Rosa JG, Gusso D, et al. (2014) Diazepam and fluoxetine decrease the stress response in zebrafish. PLoS One 9: e103232. pmid:25054216
  57. 57. Boyd KN, Kumar S, O'Buckley TK, Porcu P, Morrow AL (2010) Ethanol induction of steroidogenesis in rat adrenal and brain is dependent upon pituitary ACTH release and de novo adrenal StAR synthesis. Journal of neurochemistry 112: 784–796. pmid:20021565
  58. 58. Marchesi C, Chiodera P, Ampollini P, Volpi R, Coiro V (1997) Beta-endorphin, adrenocorticotropic hormone and cortisol secretion in abstinent alcoholics. Psychiatry Res 72: 187–194. pmid:9406908
  59. 59. Keedwell PA, Poon L, Papadopoulos AS, Marshall EJ, Checkley SA (2001) Salivary cortisol measurements during a medically assisted alcohol withdrawal. Addict Biol 6: 247–256. pmid:11900603
  60. 60. Adinoff B, Ruether K, Krebaum S, Iranmanesh A, Williams MJ (2003) Increased salivary cortisol concentrations during chronic alcohol intoxication in a naturalistic clinical sample of men. Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research 27: 1420–1427. pmid:14506402
  61. 61. Rose AK, Shaw SG, Prendergast MA, Little HJ (2010) The importance of glucocorticoids in alcohol dependence and neurotoxicity. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 34: 2011–2018. pmid:21087289
  62. 62. Cachat J, Canavello P, Elegante M, Bartels B, Hart P, Bergner C, et al. (2010) Modeling withdrawal syndrome in zebrafish. Behav Brain Res 208: 371–376. pmid:20006651
  63. 63. Gangisetty O, Bekdash R, Maglakelidze G, Sarkar DK (2014) Fetal alcohol exposure alters proopiomelanocortin gene expression and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis function via increasing MeCP2 expression in the hypothalamus. PloS one 9: e113228. pmid:25409090
  64. 64. Fernandes Y, Gerlai R (2009) Long-term behavioral changes in response to early developmental exposure to ethanol in zebrafish. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 33: 601–609. pmid:19183139
  65. 65. Bailey JM, Oliveri AN, Zhang C, Frazier JM, Mackinnon S, Cole GJ, et al. (2015) Long-term behavioral impairment following acute embryonic ethanol exposure in zebrafish. Neurotoxicol Teratol 48C: 1–8.
  66. 66. Hellemans KG, Verma P, Yoon E, Yu W, Weinberg J (2008) Prenatal alcohol exposure increases vulnerability to stress and anxiety-like disorders in adulthood. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1144: 154–175. pmid:19076375
  67. 67. Schambra UB, Goldsmith J, Nunley K, Liu Y, Harirforoosh S, Schambra HM (2015) Low and moderate prenatal ethanol exposures of mice during gastrulation or neurulation delays neurobehavioral development. Neurotoxicol Teratol 51: 1–11. pmid:26171567
  68. 68. Carvan MJ 3rd, Loucks E, Weber DN, Williams FE (2004) Ethanol effects on the developing zebrafish: neurobehavior and skeletal morphogenesis. Neurotoxicol Teratol 26: 757–768. pmid:15451040