Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Cognitive Performance Is Highly Stable over a 2-Year-Follow-Up in Chronic Kidney Disease Patients in a Dedicated Medical Environment

  • Janine Gronewold,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany

  • Olga Todica,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany

  • Ulla K. Seidel,

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany

  • Michaela Volsek,

    Affiliation Department of Nephrology, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany

  • Andreas Kribben,

    Affiliation Department of Nephrology, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany

  • Heike Bruck,

    Affiliation Department of Nephrology, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany

  • Dirk M. Hermann

    dirk.hermann@uk-essen.de

    Affiliation Department of Neurology, University Hospital Essen, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany

Abstract

Background

As kidney and brain functions decline with aging, chronic kidney disease (CKD) and dementia are becoming increasing health burdens worldwide. Among the risk factors for cognitive impairment, CKD is increasingly recognized. The precise impact of CKD on the development of cognitive impairment is poorly understood.

Methods

In the New Tools for the Prevention of Cardiovascular Disease in Chronic Kidney Disease (NTCVD) cohort, which was recruited in a dedicated nephrology department, we examined the 2-year course of cognitive performance in 120 patients (73 patients with CKD stages 3–5D, 47 control patients without CKD with similar vascular risk profile) using a comprehensive battery of 10 neuropsychological tests.

Results

Kidney function, vascular risk factors and cognitive performance were highly stable both in CKD and control patients. The summary score of cognitive performance in CKD patients was very similar at baseline (z = -0.63±0.76) and follow-up (z = -0.54±0.79, p = 0.113), as was cognitive performance in control patients (z = -0.01±0.59 and 0.01±0.70, p = 0.862, at baseline and follow-up, respectively). Total serum cholesterol (199.6±36.0 and 186.0±32.9, p = 0.005 in controls; 194.4±46.1 and 181.2±41.2, p = 0.008 in CKD) and common carotid intima-media thickness (0.87±0.18 and 0.84±0.17, p = 0.351 in controls; 0.88±0.21 and 0.82±0.16, p = 0.002 in CKD) moderately but significantly decreased during the follow-up. In multivariable regression analyses, high age (β = -0.28, 95%CI = -0.48 to 0.08, p = 0.007) predicted decrease in cognitive performance.

Conclusions

In this well-defined cohort receiving state-of-the-art therapy, cognitive performance did not decrease over 2 years. Our data emphasize the aspect of risk factor control, suggesting that dedicated medical care might prevent cognitive decline in CKD patients.

Introduction

Chronic kidney disease (CKD) is a growing health problem worldwide with prevalence estimates ranging from 23.4–35.8% in persons aged 64 years or older [1]. Exhibiting a high load of vascular risk factors [2], CKD patients frequently reveal cognitive impairment in cross-sectional studies [36]. In an own cross-sectional analysis within the New Tools for the Prevention of Cardiovascular Disease in Chronic Kidney Disease (NTCVD) cohort that compared 119 CKD patients with 54 control patients without CKD but with similar vascular risk profile, deficits in memory, information processing, executive functions, language and visuoconstruction were noted in 19–39% of CKD patients [7]. High patient age, high HbA1c and high fibrinogen in blood predicted cognitive deficits, suggesting a role of disturbed glucose control and inflammation in the development of cognitive deficits [7].

Cross-sectional studies provide valuable insights into associations, but not into causal links between CKD and cognitive performance. Longitudinal studies, on the other hand, mostly used short screening tools like the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) test or telephone interviews to assess cognitive performance [821]. These studies did not offer a consistent picture of the association between CKD and cognitive performance. So far, only two studies used a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery to measure cognitive impairment and cognitive decline: In the residential care facility-based prospective Rush Memory and Aging Project cohort that included 886 participants, Buchman et al. (2009) revealed that lower baseline kidney function indicated by lower estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) was associated with annual decline in a summary score of global cognition within 3.4±1.4 years follow-up [22]. Conversely, in the community-based Maine-Syracuse longitudinal study that included 590 participants, Davey et al. (2013) did not identify an association between baseline eGFR and change in cognitive function but only between change in eGFR and change in global cognitive function within 5 years of follow-up [23].

All above-mentioned studies examined cohorts of elderly subjects, which were recruited outside hospitals and which only partly exhibited CKD. These studies were not explicitly designed to analyze links between cognition and CKD. So far, only one study systematically compared 62 elderly CKD patients aged 78.1±3.6 years with 63 subjects without CKD with similar age and vascular risk profile that were recruited via a residential care facility, showing similar progression of cognitive deficits evaluated by the MMSE test and the clinical dementia rating (CDR) scale within 3 years follow-up in subjects with and without CKD [24]. In CKD patients, the MMSE score decreased by 4.7 points within the observation period [24]. Again, this study lacked a more thorough neuropsychological examination. Following our previous cross-sectional analyses in CKD patients, in which we evaluated links between vascular risk factors, cognition, emotion and quality of life [7, 25], we in the meantime completed a two-year follow-up examination in the NTCVD cohort. We now used this two-year follow-up to evaluate links between CKD and the longitudinal course of cognitive performance. Since the NTCVD cohort was recruited in a dedicated university department of nephrology, this cohort represents a sample of well-controlled CKD patients as well as control patients without CKD but with similar vascular risk profile receiving optimized treatment for CKD and for associated vascular risk factors and diseases.

Materials and Methods

Study cohort

The NTCVD study prospectively analyzed the effects of risk factors and markers of cardiovascular disease in patients with CKD [26]. In October 2008, 87 patients with CKD3-5 without hemodialysis, 32 CKD patients on hemodialysis (CKD5D) and 54 control patients with vascular risk factors but without CKD according to the Kidney Disease Outcomes Quality Initiative classification [27] were recruited at the Department of Nephrology of the University Hospital Essen with the help of local collaborating physicians and received detailed neuropsychological examinations [7, 25]. The study was approved by the ethical committee of the University Duisburg-Essen and all subjects gave written informed consent. Of the 173 patients enrolled at baseline, 9 patients (4 CKD3-5, 3 CKD5D and 2 controls) died between baseline and the 2-year follow-up, on which this longitudinal analysis is based. Of the remaining 164 patients, 30 patients underwent kidney transplantation and one patient newly required hemodialysis. These patients were excluded from the present analysis. Of the remaining 133 patients, 22 patients (16.5%) could not be examined at follow-up for the following reasons: 6 too ill, 12 no interest, 1 moved away from study region and 3 untraceable. Consequently, 120 patients (83.5% of 133 patients still available) were included into the present analysis (56 CKD3-5, 17 CKD5D and 47 control patients). A flow chart of the patient inclusion and exclusion is presented in Fig 1.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Flow chart of the NTCVD 2-year follow-up study.

Numbers of patients included and excluded in the follow-up analysis are shown. CKD, chronic kidney disease. NTCVD, New Tools for the Prevention of Cardiovascular Disease in Chronic Kidney Disease.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166530.g001

Medical history and patient examination

Patient examination has been described in detail before [7, 25, 28]. At the baseline examination and the 2-year follow-up, all patients underwent a standardized interview to collect socio-demographic and medical history data, followed by a physical and neuropsychological examination as well as laboratory tests. Medical history included information about the onset, etiology and treatment of CKD, known vascular risk factors, namely arterial hypertension, dyslipidemia, diabetes, and current smoking (defined as smoking during the last 6 months), and vascular diseases (coronary heart disease (CHD), stroke/transient ischemic attack (TIA), peripheral artery disease (PAD)), and current medications. Level of education was assessed categorically as none, secondary school, baccalaureate or university degree as well as continuously as years of school education.

Blood and urine samples were collected to evaluate CKD stage and vascular risk profile in addition to routine blood parameters. Total cholesterol, low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol, high-density lipoprotein (HDL) cholesterol, triglycerides, glycated hemoglobin (HbA1c), glucose, creatinine, hemoglobin, potassium and fibrinogen were measured in peripheral blood using standard laboratory techniques, and urine creatinine, urea and proteins were evaluated by nephelometry at the central laboratory of the University Hospital Essen. eGFR was calculated using the modification-of-diet-in-renal-disease (MDRD) formula. Standardized hip and waist circumference, height and weight were measured. Standardized height and weight measurements were used for calculating the body-mass index (BMI).

Noninvasive tests included the measurement of systolic and diastolic blood pressure. Arterial hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure ≥140 mmHg, diastolic blood pressure ≥90 mmHg, or prescription of antihypertensive drugs. Diabetes was coded in cases of physician–diagnosed diabetes, blood glucose ≥200 mg/dl or fasting glucose of ≥126 mg/dl, or prescription of antidiabetic medication. Dyslipidemia was defined as total cholesterol ≥240 mg/dl or LDL cholesterol ≥160 mg/dl, or prescription of lipid-lowering medication. Intima-media thickness (IMT) of both common carotid arteries and ankle-brachial index (ABI) were determined, as described before [7].

Neuropsychological examination

The neuropsychological test battery comprised 10 standardized tests that included (a) the revised Wechsler memory scale (subtests digit and block span, forward and backward; assessment of verbal and spatial short-term and working memory) [29], (b) the trail making test part A (assessment of information processing speed) [30], (c) the trail making test part B (assessment of cognitive flexibility) [30], (d) the Stroop test (assessment of cognitive interference) [31], (e) the Regensburg word fluency test (subtests ‘animals’ and ‘s-words’; assessment of semantical and lexical word fluency) [32] and (f) the Rey–Osterrieth complex figure test (copy task, assessment of visuoconstructive function) [33]. To control for depression and anxiety level, the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale was applied [34].

In order to obtain a score for global cognition, z-scores were calculated for all 10 neuropsychological tests on the basis of norm values generated in the control group [7].

Thus, the mean value of control patients at the baseline examination was subtracted from each individual CKD patient score and divided by the standard deviation (SD) of control patients at baseline. A summary score of global cognitive performance was formed by computing mean values of these z-scores. For the TMT part A/B and Stroop tests, scores were reversed because lower scores indicate better performance.

Besides the summary score, cognitive subdomains were computed, namely memory (digit and block span, forward and backward), information processing speed (TMT part A), executive function (TMT part B, Stroop), language (word fluency ‘animals’ and ‘s-words’), and visuoconstructive function (Rey–Osterrieth complex figure copy task), by computing mean values of z-scores of the respective tests. Change in cognition was evaluated by subtracting the baseline from the follow-up performance, thus positive values indicate improvement in performance while negative values indicate cognitive decline.

Statistical analysis

Continuous data are presented as mean±SD values for normally distributed data and as median (Q1;Q3) for non-normally distributed data, categorical data as counts (%). To evaluate the effect of CKD and time on continuous medical and neuropsychological data, a mixed-design ANOVA with the between-subjects factor group (CKD3-5D vs controls) and the within-subjects factor time (baseline vs 2-year follow-up) with post-hoc unpaired or paired t-tests, respectively, for normally distributed data or Mann-Whitney tests and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, respectively, for non-normally distributed data was performed. Categorical data were analyzed with Chi-square or Fisher’s exact tests. To evaluate predictors of change in cognition, univariate and multivariable linear regressions (forced entry method) were calculated. The factors age, sex, years of school education, eGFR, hemoglobin, potassium, urine urea, urine total protein, urine albumin, systolic blood pressure, total cholesterol, HDL cholesterol, LDL cholesterol, HbA1c, smoking, BMI, fibrinogen, IMT, ABI, cardiovascular diseases (CHD, stroke/TIA, PAD), depression, anxiety, antidiabetics, antihypertensives, lipid-lowering drugs and baseline cognitive performance were first inserted unadjusted into these regressions. For urine urea, urine albumin and urine total protein, the natural logarithm was used to normalize these right-skewed variables. In case of eGFR, the value 7.5 ml/min/1.73 m2 was used for hemodialysis patients which represents half the cut-off for CKD5 patients, because eGFR is not valid in hemodialysis patients. In a next step we analyzed the influence on cognitive performance of (a) age, sex, education and eGFR (model 1), (b) age, sex, education, eGFR and baseline cognition (model 2), (c) age, sex, education, eGFR, vascular risk factors (systolic blood pressure, HbA1c, total cholesterol), fibrinogen and baseline cognition (model 3), (d) age, sex, education, eGFR, the subclinical atherosclerosis marker IMT and baseline cognition (model 4) and (e) age, sex, education, eGFR, vascular diseases (CHD, stroke/TIA, PAD) and baseline cognition (model 5). The remaining variables were not included into multivariable analyses, since they did not reveal associations with change in cognitive performance in univariate analyses (not shown). In order to analyze the influence of change of predictors from baseline to follow-up on change of cognition, we repeated the above-mentioned regressions using change values instead of baseline values. Cases with missing values were excluded listwise from the analysis. P-values <0.05 indicate statistical significance. All statistics were performed using SPSS19 for Windows (SPSS, Chicago, IL, U.S.A.).

Results

Study cohort

The characteristics of the total study cohort including 120 patients with complete neuropsychological data at baseline and follow-up (73 CKD and 47 control patients) are summarized in Table 1. The 53 patients (46 CKD and 7 control patients) who had only baseline and no follow-up investigation due to reasons shown in Fig 1 and were thus excluded from the present analysis, were significantly younger (57.7±16.8 vs 63.6±12.3 years, p = 0.020), had lower BMI (26.5±5.1 vs 28.2±5.0 kg/m2, p = 0.038), lower eGFR (14(7.5;43.3) vs 53.5(28.3;65.0) ml/min/1.73m2, p<0.001), higher creatinine (3.5(1.6;5.5) vs 1.3(1.1;2.3) mg/dl, p<0.001), lower hemoglobin (12.0±1.8 vs 13.5±1.5 g/dl, p<0.001), higher potassium (4.7±0.7 vs 4.6±0.6 mmol/l, p = 0.044), higher fibrinogen (435.5±120.1 vs 374.1±101.5 mg/dl, p = 0.002), higher urine albumin (784.1(114.1;1749.0) vs 35.2(17.9;128.9) mg/g creatinine, p<0.001) and higher urine protein (1307.7(611.0;3369.8) vs 187.3(124.5;389.8) mg/g creatinine, p<0.001) compared with the 120 patients with complete data at baseline and follow-up included in the present analysis.

thumbnail
Table 1. Charateristics of Control Patients and CKD Patients at Baseline and Follow-up.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166530.t001

The mean age of those CKD patients enrolled was 64.3±13.6 years (57.5% males). Age and sex distribution did not significantly differ from control patients. As expected, CKD patients had lower eGFR, lower hemoglobin, a higher blood creatinine, potassium and fibrinogen and higher urine albumin and urine protein than control patients. LDL cholesterol and diastolic blood pressure were also lower in CKD patients than in control patients. All other variables did not differ between the CKD and the control group. During the 2-year follow-up, eGFR remained very stable in CKD patients and controls. A significant decline in eGFR of >3 mL/min/1.73 m²/year was noted in 16.4% of CKD and 25.5% of control patients. In line with the tight control of CKD and comorbid risk factors, blood total cholesterol moderately decreased in CKD and in control patients, as did HDL cholesterol and intima-media thickness of the common carotid artery (IMT) in CKD patients. Interestingly, HbA1c slightly increased in CKD patients (both in CKD stages 3–5 and stage 5D), while blood glucose remained unchanged. There was no significant interaction between time and group for none of the variables of interest. As a consequence of the intensified patient treatment, a large percentage of subjects in both groups received antidiabetic, antihypertensive, lipid-lowering and antiplatelet drugs.

Cognitive performance

Results of the ten neuropsychological tests we used are presented in Table 2. Results for the summary score of global cognition, which comprised all ten tests, and cognitive domains are shown in Table 3. The participants who had only baseline and no follow-up investigation and were thus excluded from the present analysis did not significantly differ from the participants with complete data at baseline and follow-up included in the present analysis (not shown). As previously reported for the cross-sectional analysis [7], those CKD patients enrolled into the present analysis revealed significantly worse performance than controls in all tests except for the forward condition of the block span. In the longitudinal analysis however, CKD patients showed a similar course of cognitive performance compared to control patients. Notably in individual tests assessing working memory (block span backward), interference (Stroop test) and word fluency (‘animals’ and ‘s-words’), CKD patients even improved during the follow-up, which may reflect a consequence of test sophistication and practice effects. The summary score of global cognition and cognitive domains did not reveal any changes during the follow-up, except for language, which improved in CKD patients.

thumbnail
Table 2. Raw Values of Neuropsychological Tests of Control Patients and CKD patients at Baseline and Follow-up.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166530.t002

thumbnail
Table 3. z-values of Global Cognition and Cognitive Domains in Controls and CKD patients at Baseline and Follow-up.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166530.t003

In view of the absence of cognitive decline, we evaluated the statistical power of this observation in comparison to data presented in the literature. The only study hitherto reporting quantitative changes of cognitive performance in a longitudinal study using more detailed neuropsychological testing was that of Davey et al. [23], who described a decline of global cognitive performance of 0.075 z-values over 5 years, equivalent to a decrease of 0.03 z-values over 2 years. Based on the data presented in Table 3 we observed a change of global cognitive performance of 0.06 ± 0.45 z-values over 2 years in the total cohort. According to statistical power calculations, our study was suitable to detect the absence of cognitive deterioration with a statistical power (1-β) of 70.7% with an error probability (α-value) of 5% in a 120 patient cohort.

Predictors for change in cognition

To evaluate predictors of cognitive changes within the 2-year follow-up, univariate and multivariable linear regressions were computed using the difference value of the summary score of global cognitive performance at follow-up and baseline (follow-up minus baseline, Tables 4 and 5). In univariate regressions, high IMT (β = -0.18, 95% CI = -0.36 to -0.01, p = 0.049) and high baseline cognitive performance (β = -0.21, 95% CI = -0.39 to -0.03, p = 0.023) predicted decrease in global cognitive performance, while all other variables did not have a significant effect. In multivariable regressions, the factors high age (β = -0.28, 95% CI = -0.48 to -0.08, p = 0.007; see model 5) and high baseline cognitive performance (β = -0.37, 95% CI = -0.58 to -0.17, p = 0.001; see model 5) predicted decrease in cognitive performance, while cardiovascular risk factors (systolic blood pressure, HbA1c, total cholesterol; see model 3), fibrinogen (see model 3), IMT (see model 4) and cardiovascular diseases (CHD, stroke/TIA, PAD, see model 5) were no significant predictors. Since Davey et al. (2013) [23] reported that change of eGFR but not baseline eGFR predicted changes of cognitive performance during the follow-up, we also analyzed how the changes of eGFR, changes of risk factors and changes of atherosclerosis markers, as above, predicted the course of cognitive performance between baseline and follow-up. These analyses did not reveal any links between changes of the above variables and change of cognitive performance, neither in univariate nor multivariable analyses (not shown).

thumbnail
Table 4. Baseline Predictors for Change of Global Cognitive Performance from Baseline to Follow-up in the Total Cohort: Unadjusted and Simple Multivariable Models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166530.t004

thumbnail
Table 5. Baseline Predictors for Change of Global Cognitive Performance from Baseline to Follow-up in the Total Cohort: More Complex Multivariable Models.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166530.t005

Discussion

In a well-defined cohort of CKD patients that we recruited in a university department of nephrology together with a sample of patients without CKD with very similar vascular risk profile, we herein demonstrate that cognitive performance remains highly stable over a 2-year follow-up. Indeed, cognitive performance did not decrease during the observation period in CKD patients, but rather improved as a consequence of test sophistication and practice [35], which reached significance in the language domain. As a consequence of dedicated patient care, total serum cholesterol and common carotid intima-media thickness significantly decreased during the follow-up, while all other risk factors largely remained unchanged. In multivariable regression studies, high age predicted decrease of cognitive performance during the follow-up period. Until now, the majority of studies examining cognitive performance in elderly subjects with CKD reported deterioration of cognitive performance in longitudinal analyses [36]. In these studies, participants were recruited outside hospital environments and as such many subjects were not clinically ill. Besides, the participants of these studies only partly suffered from CKD. Since cognitive performance related to CKD was not the primary focus of these studies, cognition was assessed by rather simple screening tools, such as the MMSE [11, 12, 18, 19] or 3MS [14] or evaluated only via telephone interviews [13, 15].

The only two studies which used a comprehensive neuropsychological test battery to measure cognitive performance in residential care facility or community-based cohorts reported diverse findings [22, 23]. Notably, only one of these studies reported magnitudes of cognitive changes over time [23]. In 886 dementia-free participants from the Rush Memory and Aging Project cohort, which was recruited in residential care facilities to identify risk factors for Alzheimer's disease, Buchman et al. [22] reported a significant association between low baseline eGFR and decline of cognitive performance. This paper did not provide information about the absolute magnitude of cognitive decline during the follow-up. The follow-up period was slightly longer (3.4±1.4 years) and the cohort was much older (mean age 80.6±7.5 years) than in our study. High age predicted decrease in cognitive performance in multivariable linear regression analyses in our study. The different age of cohorts most likely explains the diverse observations regarding cognitive development in the Rush Memory and Aging Project cohort and the NTCVD cohort.

Similar to our study, Davey et al. [23] could not show an association between baseline eGFR and change in cognitive function in 590 subjects from the community-based Maine-Syracuse longitudinal cohort. In their study, a decline of global cognitive performance of 0.075 z-values was noted over 5 years follow-up, equivalent to a decrease of 0.03 z-values over 2 years. Despite sufficient statistical powering, we did not detect such decline in the present study. The mean age of their cohort was comparable to ours (61.1 years), but the risk factor profile was less severe. The follow-up period of 5 years was considerably longer than in our study. In contrast to our results, the authors observed a significant association between change of log-transformed eGFR and change of cognitive performance when adjusted for baseline cognition, renal function and vascular risk factors. A possible reason for this association, which we could not confirm in our study, is that kidney function was not stable in the Maine-Syracuse cohort, 36.9% of CKD patients experiencing a clinically relevant deterioration of eGFR defined by >3 mL/min/1.73 m²/year [23]. This percentage was significantly lower in our study (16.4% of CKD patients). Differences in the intensity of patient care might explain different observations between the Maine-Syracuse cohort and NTCVD cohort.

The only study that explicitly recruited CKD patients was published by Weng et al. [24]. In a cohort of 125 subjects recruited in a residential care facility, the authors reported no association between kidney function and cognitive decline, which is in accordance with our study. In comparison to our cohort, the participants of this study were much older (78.1±3.6 years). In contrast to our cohort, participants revealed a deterioration of cognitive function independent of eGFR that was stratified at a cutoff of 49 mL/min. The follow-up period in their study (3 years) was similar to our study, but cognitive performance was assessed only using the MMSE and CDR scale, while we used a battery of 10 neuropsychological tests for various cognitive domains that was administered by a clinical neuropsychologist. The different age of participants and the recruitment pathway (residential care facility vs. university hospital) most likely explain differences in the progression of cognitive deficits between this study and the NTCVD cohort.

Based on the combined evidence of the present and the above previous studies, the variables age and disease control may represent the most significant factors modulating cognitive deterioration in patients suffering from CKD. In the NTCVD cohort, multivariable regression analyses also provided evidence that high baseline cognitive performance predicted cognitive decline. Most likely, this effect is attributed to the so-called effect of regression to the mean in subjects with particularly strong or poor baseline test performance [37]. In elderly patients, poor cognitive performance and not good cognitive performance was repeatedly predictive of cognitive decline [24, 38, 39]. Interestingly, in the NTCVD cohort, high IMT, indicative of large-vessel atherosclerosis, and presence of cardiovascular diseases (CHD, stroke/TIA, PAD) exhibited trends for associations with cognitive decline (see Tables 4 and 5). Due to the limited sample size, these associations failed to reach significance in multivariable regression analyses.

The major strengths of this study are the well-defined sample of CKD patients and the detailed neuropsychological examination, which allowed us to detect subtle cognitive changes over time. Due to careful patient investigation, the follow-up rate was very high (83.5%). Despite this fact, biases related to patient dropouts cannot be excluded. The major weakness of this study is its relatively small sample size and the short 2-year follow-up period, which however had an adequate statistical power to detect rates of cognitive decline reported in the past [23] (see paragraph on cognitive performance in the results section). We decided to evaluate this follow-up period since we expected considerably higher dropout rates after longer follow-ups, which may have confounded the conclusions made in this sample of seriously ill patients. In the future, larger studies with longer follow-ups will be required to further dissect the mutual links between CKD and cognitive decline. Considering the impact of clinically overt diseases, there will be a need for clinical multicenter cohorts to evaluate how different treatment strategies may affect the course of cognitive performance.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF/NGFN 01GR0807), the Heinz Nixdorf Foundation and Dr Werner Jackstädt Foundation. We thank Gisela Behrendt for administrative and technical support.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: JG OT HB DMH.
  2. Data curation: JG.
  3. Formal analysis: JG DMH.
  4. Funding acquisition: HB DMH.
  5. Investigation: JG OT UKS MV AK HB DMH.
  6. Methodology: JG OT DMH.
  7. Project administration: JG OT UKS MV.
  8. Resources: JG OT UKS MV AK HB DMH.
  9. Software: JG.
  10. Supervision: JG DMH.
  11. Validation: JG DMH.
  12. Visualization: JG DMH.
  13. Writing – original draft: JG DMH.
  14. Writing – review & editing: JG OT UKS MV AK HB DMH.

References

  1. 1. Zhang QL, Rothenbacher D. Prevalence of chronic kidney disease in population-based studies: systematic review. BMC Public Health 2008;8:117. pmid:18405348
  2. 2. Excerpts from the United States Renal Data Systems 2002 annual report: Atlas of end-stage renal disease in the United States. Am J Kidney Dis 2003;41:S7–254.
  3. 3. Abdel-Kader K, Dew MA, Bhatnagar M, Argyropoulos C, Karpov I, Switzer G, et al. Numeracy skills in CKD: correlates and outcomes. Clinical J Am Soc of Nephrol 2010;5:1566–1573.
  4. 4. Kurella M, Chertow GM, Luan J, Yaffe K. Cognitive impairment in chronic kidney disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 2004;52:1863–1869. pmid:15507063
  5. 5. Nulsen RS, Yaqoob MM, Mahon A, Stoby-Fields M, Kelly M, Varagunam M. Prevalence of cognitive impairment in patients attending pre-dialysis clinic. J Ren Care 2008;34:121–126. pmid:18786078
  6. 6. Thornton WL, Shapiro RJ, Deria S, Gelb S, Hill A. Differential impact of age on verbal memory and executive functioning in chronic kidney disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13:344–353. pmid:17286891
  7. 7. Seidel UK, Gronewold J, Volsek M, Todica O, Kribben A, Bruck H, et al. The prevalence, severity, and association with HbA1c and fibrinogen of cognitive impairment in chronic kidney disease. Kidney Int 2014;85:693–702. pmid:24088956
  8. 8. Barzilay JI, Gao P, O'Donnell M, Mann JF, Anderson C, Fagard R, et al. Albuminuria and decline in cognitive function: The ONTARGET/TRANSCEND studies. Arch Intern Med 2011;171:142–150. pmid:21263104
  9. 9. Darsie B, Shlipak MG, Sarnak MJ, Katz R, Fitzpatrick AL, Odden MC. Kidney function and cognitive health in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study. Am J Epidemiol 2014;180:68–75. pmid:24844846
  10. 10. Etgen T, Sander D, Chonchol M, Briesenick C, Poppert H, Forstl H, et al. Chronic kidney disease is associated with incident cognitive impairment in the elderly: the INVADE study. Nephrol Dial Transplant 2009;24:3144–3150. pmid:19461010
  11. 11. Feng L, Yap KB, Yeoh LY, Ng TP. Kidney function and cognitive and functional decline in elderly adults: findings from the Singapore longitudinal aging study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2012;60:1208–1214. pmid:22702744
  12. 12. Jassal SK, Kritz-Silverstein D, Barrett-Connor E. A prospective study of albuminuria and cognitive function in older adults: the Rancho Bernardo study. Am J Epidemiol 2010;171:277–286. pmid:20061364
  13. 13. Khatri M, Nickolas T, Moon YP, Paik MC, Rundek T, Elkind MS, et al. CKD associates with cognitive decline. J Am Soc Nephrol 2009;20:2427–2432. pmid:19729443
  14. 14. Kurella M, Chertow GM, Fried LF, Cummings SR, Harris T, Simonsick E, et al. Chronic kidney disease and cognitive impairment in the elderly: the health, aging, and body composition study. J Am Soc Nephrol 2005;16:2127–2133. pmid:15888561
  15. 15. Kurella Tamura M, Muntner P, Wadley V, Cushman M, Zakai NA, Bradbury BD, et al. Albuminuria, kidney function, and the incidence of cognitive impairment among adults in the United States. Am J Kidney Dis 2011;58:756–763. pmid:21816528
  16. 16. Miwa K, Tanaka M, Okazaki S, Furukado S, Yagita Y, Sakaguchi M, et al. Chronic kidney disease is associated with dementia independent of cerebral small-vessel disease. Neurology 2014;82:1051–1057. pmid:24553427
  17. 17. Sajjad I, Grodstein F, Kang JH, Curhan GC, Lin J. Kidney dysfunction and cognitive decline in women. Clin J Am Soc Nephrol 2012;7:437–443. pmid:22241818
  18. 18. Sasaki Y, Marioni R, Kasai M, Ishii H, Yamaguchi S, Meguro K. Chronic kidney disease: a risk factor for dementia onset: a population-based study. The Osaki-Tajiri Project. J Am Geriat Soc 2011;59:1175–1181. pmid:21668914
  19. 19. Wang F, Zhang L, Liu L, Wang H. Level of kidney function correlates with cognitive decline. Am J Nephrol 2010;32:117–121. pmid:20714128
  20. 20. Helmer C, Stengel B, Metzger M, Froissart M, Massy ZA, Tzourio C, et al. Chronic kidney disease, cognitive decline, and incident dementia: the 3C Study. Neurology 2011;77:2043–2051. pmid:22116945
  21. 21. Slinin Y, Paudel ML, Ishani A, Taylor BC, Yaffe K, Murray AM, et al. Kidney function and cognitive performance and decline in older men. J Am Geriatr Soc 2008;56:2082–2088. pmid:18795984
  22. 22. Buchman AS, Tanne D, Boyle PA, Shah RC, Leurgans SE, Bennett DA. Kidney function is associated with the rate of cognitive decline in the elderly. Neurology 2009;73:920–927. pmid:19657107
  23. 23. Davey A, Elias MF, Robbins MA, Seliger SL, Dore GA. Decline in renal functioning is associated with longitudinal decline in global cognitive functioning, abstract reasoning and verbal memory. Nephrol Dial Transplant 2013;28:1810–1819. pmid:23166308
  24. 24. Weng SC, Shu KH, Tang YJ, Sheu WH, Tarng DC, Wu MJ, et al. Progression of cognitive dysfunction in elderly chronic kidney disease patients in a veteran's institution in central Taiwan: a 3-year longitudinal study. Intern Med 2012;51:29–35. pmid:22214620
  25. 25. Seidel UK, Gronewold J, Volsek M, Todica O, Kribben A, Bruck H, et al. Physical, cognitive and emotional factors contributing to quality of life, functional health and participation in community dwelling in chronic kidney disease. PloS One 2014;9:e91176. pmid:24614180
  26. 26. Salem S, Bruck H, Bahlmann FH, Peter M, Passlick-Deetjen J, Kretschmer A, et al. Relationship between magnesium and clinical biomarkers on inhibition of vascular calcification. Am J Nephrol 2012;35:31–39. pmid:22179063
  27. 27. K/DOQI clinical practice guidelines for chronic kidney disease: evaluation, classification, and stratification. Am J Kidney Dis 2002;39:S1–S266. pmid:11904577
  28. 28. Gronewold J, Klafki HW, Baldelli E, Kaltwasser B, Seidel UK, Todica O, et al. Factors Responsible for Plasma beta-Amyloid Accumulation in Chronic Kidney Disease. Mol Neurobiol 2015 pmid:26019016
  29. 29. Härting C, Markowitsch H-J, Neufeld H, Calabrese P, Deisinger K, Kessler J. Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised Edition, German Edition. Bern: Huber 2000.
  30. 30. Reitan RM. Trail Making Test. Tucson, AZ: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory 1992.
  31. 31. Bäumler G. Farb-Wort-Interferenztest (FWIT) nach J.R. Stroop. Göttingen: Hogrefe 1985.
  32. 32. Aschenbrenner S, Tucha O, Lange KW. Regensburger Wortflüssigkeits-Test (RWT). Göttingen: Hogrefe 2000.
  33. 33. Russ M. Rey Complex Figure Test. Anleitung für die Anwendung im Frankfurter Neuropsychologischen Testprofil (FNTP). Available: http://www.fntp.de/mediapool/80/807841/data/CFT_Scoring.pdf. Accessed September 5, 2012.
  34. 34. Herrmann-Lingen C, Buss U, Snaith RP. Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale—Deutsche Version (HADS-D). Bern: Huber 2005.
  35. 35. Zehnder AE, Blasi S, Berres M, Spiegel R, Monsch AU. Lack of practice effects on neuropsychological tests as early cognitive markers of Alzheimer disease? Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 2007;22:416–426. pmid:17959877
  36. 36. Etgen T. Kidney disease as a determinant of cognitive decline and dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther 2015;7:29. pmid:25780391
  37. 37. Barnett AG, van der Pols JC, Dobson AJ. Regression to the mean: what it is and how to deal with it. Int J Epidemiol 2005;34:215–220. pmid:15333621
  38. 38. Geerlings MI, Jonker C, Bouter LM, Ader HJ, Schmand B. Association between memory complaints and incident Alzheimer's disease in elderly people with normal baseline cognition. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:531–537. pmid:10200730
  39. 39. Schofield PW, Marder K, Dooneief G, Jacobs DM, Sano M, Stern Y. Association of subjective memory complaints with subsequent cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly individuals with baseline cognitive impairment. Am J Psychiatry 1997;154:609–615. pmid:9137114