Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Speaking up behavior and cognitive bias in hand hygiene: Competences of German-speaking medical students

  • Stefan Bushuven ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    S.Bushuven@gmx.de

    Affiliations Institute for Anesthesiology, Intensive Care, Emergency Medicine and Pain Therapy, Hegau-Bodensee Hospital Singen and Hegau Jugendwerk Gailingen, Healthcare Association Constance (GLKN), Radolfzell, Germany, Institute for Hospital Hygiene and Infection Prevention, Healthcare Association Constance (GLKN), Radolfzell, Germany, Institute for Didactics and Educational Research in Medicine, Clinic of the University Munich, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany

  • Markus Dettenkofer,

    Roles Supervision, Validation

    Affiliation Institute for Hospital Hygiene and Infection Prevention, Healthcare Association Constance (GLKN), Radolfzell, Germany

  • Sonia Sippel,

    Roles Conceptualization

    Affiliation Institute of Medical Teaching and Medical Education Research, University Hospital Wuerzburg, Wuerzburg, Germany

  • Sarah Koenig,

    Roles Conceptualization, Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Institute of Medical Teaching and Medical Education Research, University Hospital Wuerzburg, Wuerzburg, Germany

  • Stefanie Bushuven,

    Roles Data curation

    Affiliation Clinic for Orthopedics, Hand- and Trauma surgery, Hegau-Bodensee-Hospital Singen, Healthcare Association Constance (GLKN), Radolfzell, Germany

  • Wulf Schneider-Brachert

    Roles Validation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Infection Control and Infectious Diseases, University Hospital Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany

Abstract

Introduction

Infection prevention and speaking up on errors are core qualities of health care providers. Heuristic effects (e.g. overconfidence) may impair behavior in daily routine, while speaking up can be inhibited by hierarchical barriers and medical team factors. Aim of this investigation was to determine, how medical students experience these difficulties for hand hygiene in daily routine.

Methods

On the base of prior investigations we developed a questionnaire with 5-point Likert ordinal scaled items and free text entries. This was tested for validity and reliability (Cronbach’s Alpha 0.89). Accredited German, Swiss and Austrian universities were contacted and medical students asked to participated in the anonymous online survey. Quantitative statistics used parametric and non-parametric tests and effect size calculations according to Lakens. Qualitative data was coded according to Janesick.

Results

1042 undergraduates of 12 universities participated. All rated their capabilities in hand hygiene and feedback reception higher than those of fellow students, nurses and physicians (p<0.001). Half of the participants rating themselves to be best educated, realized that faulty hand hygiene can be of lethal effect. Findings were independent from age, sex, academic course and university. Speaking-up in case of omitted hand hygiene was rated to be done seldomly and most rare on persons of higher hierarchic levels. Qualitative results of 164 entries showed four main themes: 1) Education methods in hand hygiene are insufficient, 2) Hierarchy barriers impair constructive work place culture 3) Hygiene and feedback are linked to medical ethics and 4) There is no consequence for breaking hygiene rules.

Discussion

Although partially limited by the selection bias, this study confirms the overconfidence-effects demonstrated in post-graduates in other settings and different professions. The independence from study progress suggests, that the effect occurs before start of the academic course with need for educational intervention at the very beginning. Qualitative data showed that used methods are insufficient and contradictory work place behavior in hospitals are frustrating. Even 20 years after “To err is human”, work place culture still is far away from the desirable.

Introduction

Patient safety is a core competency of all health care providers working in volatile, uncertain, complex and ambiguous working environments [1]. Medical error is a leading cause of in-hospital death and impairment [2], demanding for improvement in work place safety culture, individual habits, institutional high reliability and medical education of under- and postgraduates. Especially under-graduates (e.g. medical students) face substantial barriers and dilemma to speak up and give corrective feedback, in particular to their supervisors and educators [3, 4].

Aside from feedback skills, but noticeably linked to them, infection prevention and early treatment of health care associated infections are keystones of patient safety [5]. Especially hand hygiene plays a crucial role [5, 6]. Unfortunately, low adherence to hand hygiene protocols [6] and impaired commitment to educational lessons is challenging [7]. The causes for the reduced incentive motivation [8, 9] to participate in trainings remain unclear.

In addition to psychological and social factors like peer pressure [10] and Hawthorne’s effect [11], our work group identified an overconfidence effect for infection control practices and speaking up behavior in a regional [12] and nationwide study [13]. Overconfidence is a popular [14] and robust psychological effect of flawed self-assessment. It is innate to human behavior in health, forensics and economy [15, 16] and consists of three sub-effects: a) absolute overconfidence or overestimation (to rate one-self to be better than actual measurements demonstrate), b) relative overconfidence or overplacement (the belief to be competent above the average of a population or group) and c) overprecision (the excessive belief to be true in one’s own estimations) [17]. Main causes for these effects were identified as information deficits, neglect, errors of omission, incomplete feedback, self-focus and egocentrism [15] With deep knowledge about ourself, focus on ourself and overall less or inprecise information and “selfserving” assumptions about capabilities of others this leads to failing objectivity putting everybody at higher competence in comparison to others.

The condition to unintentionally rate one-self’s hygiene and communication abilities to be better than the competencies of other persons is likely to lower the intrinsic motivation to attend infection control trainings [9]: “Why should I go to a time-consuming training, if the others are more in need of it?. This may be additionally aggravated by the diffusion of responsibility (“Why should I clean my hands, when nobody does?”) [18] and corrupted model learning [19] may then further impair hand hygiene behavior.

This combination of unrealistic self-assessment, consequently reduced learning-motivation paired with de facto insufficient hygiene and feedback competences [13, 2022] and behavior in providing feedback [23] impairs patient safety and interprofessional collaboration.

Based on two published studies of our work group demonstrating overconfidence to be present already in post-graduates, we hypothesized, that the effect depends on the individual education level.

The aim of this international cross-sectional study was to evaluate the following hypotheses:

  1. Overestimation and overplacement for hand hygiene and feedback skills are present in medical students
  2. Overconfidence in hygiene, communication skills and subjective speaking-up behavior depend on semester, age, gender, preexisting vocational education and university curriculum

Additionally, quantitative and qualitative survey assessments were accomplished to get more insight into under-graduates’ perceptions and attitudes towards patient safety culture in infection prevention.

To do so, we investigated medical students’ experiences of speaking-up behavior of (other) under-graduates, physicians (of different educational and hierarchical level) and nurses in Germany, Switzerland and Austria.

Material and methods

From May 2018 to April 2019 we used an anonymous cross-sectional online survey previously tested on 51 medical students from the personal networks of the investigators for content and structural validity as well as reliability (Cronbach’s Alpha 0.89). The survey was designed by an interprofessional team of medical educators, microbiologists, hygiene experts and psychologists basing on the SATIS-1 questionnaire [12] with comparable reliability.

According to the Ethics Committee Stuttgart (Physicians Association Baden-Wurttemberg) under consideration of the Declaration of Helsinki no ethical approval of this voluntary and openly distributed study using anonymous data only is needed (Decision on 20th of September 2018, signed by Prof. Dr. Gerd Mikus). Participants were informed by the first page of the study about objectives and IP-Address anonymization by survey provider (Eunuvo GmbH, Zuerich)

The survey consisted of 36 German items (see Table 1). Items 1–9 evaluated demographic data and prior apprenticeships in a medical profession. Items 10–26 assessed the subjective personal and others’ competencies in infection prevention and communication using a 5-point Likert scale with no (0 points), poor [1], partial [2], fair [3] and complete [4] agreement. Items 27 and 28 were estimations of possible hazards of an omitted hand disinfection. In items 29–31, participants assessed identification of WHO indications for hand hygiene, adherence to hygiene protocols and the likelihood of corrective feedback to and by patients, relatives and non-medical (cleaners) and medical staff of different hierarchical levels (students, interns, consultants, heads of departments). Items 32 to 35 were questions about suggestions for medical education in hand hygiene and rankings of the importance of patient safety issues. Item 36 comprised a section for further written comments (character count unlimited) on infection prevention and feedback. To limit selection bias and to provide the possibility of a completer-non-completer comparison [24] main questions were located at the beginning of the survey.

thumbnail
Table 1. Survey items.

Items were presented in German language.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239444.t001

The survey was provided by the Enuvo GmbH, Zurich, via an online-line link. IP-addresses were blinded towards the investigators by the provider guaranteeing anonymity according to their code of conduct. The online-link was sent via email to the deans of study and student affairs and the heads of the department of hospital hygiene institutions of accredited 45 German, Swiss and Austrian medical faculties in Germany (n = 38), Switzerland (n = 3) and Austria (n = 4) addressing about 120.000 German speaking students. Numbers of medical students were evaluated according to webpages and university rankings.

Data processing, statistics and graphs were conducted using Microsoft Word, PowerPoint and Excel (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, USA) and XLSTATs (Addinsoft SARL, New York, USA).

Statistical testing included Kruskal-Wallis-tests, Kendall’s-Tau (τ) correlations and Friedman-tests adjusted for multiple testing and effect size (intraindividual Cohen’s Dz) calculation according to Lakens [25].

Qualitative analysis of the “comment section” was conducted using an inductive phenomenological single coder approach [26, 27]. Assignment of tags, coding and theme identification were conducted by the main investigator, an intensive care, hygiene and antimicrobial stewardship expert, post-graduate student in medical ethics with a masters’ degree in medical education and ISO 31000 certification in clinical risk management.

Results

Anonymized data sets are available using the Dryad data suppository (doi.org/10.5061/dryad.tx95x69vx).

Study population

Altogether 1042 medical students from 12 universities (1 Austrian and 11 German with 1610–3490 medical students hosting altogether about 28000 students,) participated. Thus, of all students internationally addressed, 23% got access to the survey. Of those exposed, 3.5% answered to the questionnaire. Thus, the study population consists of about 0.8% of medical students at German speaking universities.

All semesters (1st to 12th) were enrolled. A few data sets (n = 13) had to be excluded owing to substantial missing (see below) or even zero answers.

Of all students 673 answered all items 10 to 26 about one’s own and perceptions concerning others (see Fig 1). Participants not answering at least items 1 to 15 were excluded from the study.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Ratings by 673 students of all groups for self-assessment (own) and ratings of others (fellow students, physicians and nurses) for the three behaviors as indicated.

Brackets show significances for comparison of own competences (blue bars) with other persons’ corrected for multiple testing. In all cases comparing own and others’ behavior except for own and fellow students to speak up, data showed significance of p<0.001 corrected for multiple testing.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239444.g001

For subgroup analysis participants were allocated to four subgroups to assess for bias by preceding professional experiences, like apprenticeship in nursery, paramedic service and other medical professions (see Table 2):

  1. completers (group C; students without apprenticeship in any medical field completing the survey).
  2. completers plus (Group C+; students with apprenticeship in a medical field, completing the survey).
  3. non-completers (Group NC; students without apprenticeship in any medical field not completing the survey).
  4. non-completers plus (Group NC+; students with apprenticeship in a medical field, not completing the survey).
thumbnail
Table 2. Demographic data for the complete study population (n = 1042).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239444.t002

Detailed analysis showed, that students without prior education in a medical profession (C, NC) were significantly younger (p<0.001) than those with prior non-academic apprenticeship in a medical field (C+, NC+). Participants in the non-completer groups (NC and NC+) showed to be exposed earlier to hand hygiene trainings in university (p<0.003). Whereas exposure to the first hand hygiene training occurred at later time in groups C+ and NC+.

Main results on Hypothesis 1 (existence of overconfidence effects)

a) Overplacement.

Altogether 673 participants of all subgroups completely answered the items 10 to 26 about their perceptions on one’s own and others’ skills in hand hygiene and communication. Comparing themselves to others, students rated their adherence to hygiene protocols (mean 3.33) to be significantly better than those of fellow students (2.66), physicians (2.23) and nurses (2.40) with p <0.001 each (see Fig 1 and Table 2). For fellow students, this was reproducible for identification of hand hygiene indications (3.55 vs 2.88, p<0.001). For both comparisons between the participants and fellow students, effect sizes were high (Cohens Dz > 0.7).

Concerning omitted or erroneous hand disinfection, students rated to give rarely corrective feedback (mean 1.4 of maximum 4 points). This did not differ from their perception on their fellows correcting them (mean 1.41, p>0.05) and only slightly, but significantly (p<0.001) for estimations of physicians (1.7) and nurses (2.2) correcting them in these occasions.

In contrast, reception-skills for feedback were rated to be very good for own competences (mean 3.5), medium for fellows (2.5) and low for physicians (1.7) and nurses (1.9) with p <0.001 each and very strong effect sizes (Dz) of 1.14 for fellows, 1.8 for physicians and 1.67 for nurses.

b) Overestimation.

Participants of all groups agreeing “fairly” or “completely” to adhere to hand hygiene protocols (n = 893) estimated their own compliance rates to be 65% for WHO—indication 1 (before touching a patient), 75% for WHO–indication 2 (before an aseptic procedure), 95% after own contamination with body fluids (WHO—indication 3), 88% for WHO indication 4 (after leaving a patient) and 58% for WHO—Indication 5 (after contact to a patient’s bed). These students rated the worst credible harm of a missed hand disinfection for patient to be “lethal” (53%), “critical: with long-term impairment (28%), severe: resulting in a longer hospital stay (18%), minor (under 1%) and without any consequence (under 1%). See details in Fig 2.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Ratings by 893 students rating themselves to adhere fairly or completely to hygiene protocols.

(a) Demonstrates the self-estimations in percent of all occasions of compliance to WHO- indications with WHO 1(before touching a patient), WHO 2 (Before aseptic procedures), WHO 3 (after possible contamination), WHO 4 (after touching a patient) and WHO 5 (after contact to the patient’s environment). (b) Shows for the same students their reflexing about the maximum possible hazard to a patient in case of an omitted hand disinfection: None (no harm), minor (no sustaining harm, no longer hospital stay), severe (no sustaining harm, but longer hospital stay), critical (life-long impairment) and lethal (death or permanent brain damage).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239444.g002

Likeliness of speaking up was rated to be poor: most students agreed only partially to correct visitors, cleaners and nursery students in case of omitted or faulty hand hygiene (mean 1.9–2.8). Concerning physicians, most rated to completely avoid speaking up on interns (41,1%), consultants (65%) and heads of departments (71%). Only 1.9%, 1% and 0.7% of the participants stated, that they would correct these persons.

Main results on Hypothesis 2 (dependency of overconfidence on the level of academic education)

Rating own and others’ capabilities in hand hygiene and feedback did not correlate with the current semester (τ > 0.05). For some responses concerning self and others, there were very poor significant correlations for age (τ< 0.05, with r ranging from 0.05 to 0.1). Estimations of the maximum credible harm to a patient also did not correlate to age and semester.

Participants of the completer group (most unaffected by preceding medical education, see Fig 3) showed no significant correlation between semester and “overconfidence” (operationalized as the difference between ratings for own and other’s competences) with one exception: semester and overconfidence for adherence to hygiene protocols correlated poorly (p = 0.027, τ = -0.073). In group C+ similar results could be found with a poor negative correlation for knowledge of indications of hygiene (p = 0.036, τ = -0.126).

thumbnail
Fig 3.

Correlations of the group C for semester (x-axis) and the mathematical difference between estimation of own and others’ competences (y-axis, positive numbers show, that students rated themselves above other, negative that they rated themselves inferior) for adherence to hygiene protocols (upper left, difference of item 10 and 16), knowledge for WHO indications (upper right, items 11 and 17), knowledge about disinfection agents (lower left, items 12 and 18) and the competence to speak up (lower right, items 13 and 19). Blue lines show negative, red positive correlations.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239444.g003

Completer/non-completer comparison

For ratings of own and others’ competencies and risks of omitted hand hygiene no differences between completers and non-completers[24] could be detected.

Further results on experienced speaking up behavior

Concerning a matrix question “who corrects whom” (Fig 4) in occasion of errors or omissions in hand hygiene, subjective ratings of the students showed, that highest correctional interventions were conducted by nurses. The group to be corrected least were post-graduate physicians, especially if they were of higher hierarchic level.

thumbnail
Fig 4. Correctional behavior perceived by medical students (groups C und C+ groups).

On the Y-axis the percentages of agreements for the question if a specific group (x-Axis) corrects others (colored columns) is shown. Most corrections on others and the own profession are perceived for nurses, while cleaners and visitors are not perceived to do corrections on others. Medical students themselves feel to be corrected by all others groups. Concerning physicians, they rated to be less corrected with rising levels of hierarchy.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0239444.g004

Further results on the perception of the hygiene curricula and impact on patient safety

Most participants (68%) agreed, that the first lessons in hand hygiene should be held in the very first semester and should be repeated once per semester (29.5%) or year (37.8%). Concerning rankings of patient safety competences participants rated infection prevention (1st place) and medication safety (2nd) to be most relevant for their own medical education. Diagnostics (3rd) and treatment of sepsis (4th) were followed by strategies to prevent unnecessary hospital admissions (5th) and cyber security (6th). For their education in hospital hygiene they rated the importance of hand hygiene (1st place) above skills in personal protective equipment (2nd), barrier precautions (3rd), diagnostic (4th) and therapy (5th) of infectious diseases, pre-analytics(6th), outbreak treatment (7th), surface disinfection (8th) and medical product decontamination (9th).

Qualitative data analysis

Altogether 164 free-text entries (word count 31911) were evaluated and 55 codes were assigned in an iterative process. We identified four main subjects:

1) Present education of physicians and students in hand hygiene is insufficient.

Students reported about methodically weak hygiene lectures (“boring”, “hygiene is a dry field”, “false didactic concepts”, “better use of emotional motives”, “there should be relevant tests and trainings on simulations”) and especially, that the first exposure to infection control mainly is located after the first contact to patients. Many participants argued, that the first hygiene training should take place “before first patient contact in the care traineeship”. A second common message was, that there are profound differences between professions in quality and quantity of hygiene training (nurses, physiotherapists, paramedics). Additionally, many reported about weak attitudes in hygiene culture like “arbitrariness of rules” and “sloppy hygiene” of physicians linked to age and hierarchical status (“the older the doc, the sloppier hand hygiene”, “interns are frowzy with hand hygiene, but I don’t even want to speak about consultants”). Low attitude of fellows and physicians and active ignorance of rules were common tags (“some doctors think, they are immune to transmit microbes”, “ignorance”, “hygiene is a medical stepchild”, “There is a fellow, who never washes his hands after urination”), sometimes related to economic reasons and ethical issues (“hygiene rules and decontamination of operation-theatres are actively sacrificed to stick to the schedule”).

2) Implementation of feedback-culture is inhibited by hierarchy and clinical tribalism.

Many students reported about cultural barriers concerning feedback. “Pecking order”, “nobody dares to correct the boss”, “hygiene would profit a lot, if the structures in hospitals would not be defined by might and hierarchy” and “physicians do not want feedback” were common answers. Additionally, clinical tribalism effect [28] (a delimitation of professionals: “we against them”) could be detected, especially in experienced students (C+): “during my work as a nurse […] I experienced, that doctors did not want me to show them how to catheterize a patient”, “I have difficulties to speak up on nurses. Mainly, they are not speaking highly of us. […] It’s not easy for them to be corrected by us”, “I my first job so far, I did not experience colleagues to be ‘pissed-off’ if hygiene rules were explained. […] This I experienced first in med school.

3) Hygiene and feedback are issues of medical ethics.

Some students reported about ethical issues linked to hand-hygiene. One person suggested to implement Aristotelian ethics (“every passionate doctor can be enthused for his specialty and helps his patient.”) or Kantian (deontological) approaches (categorical imperative) to education and feedback (“What would you do, if your children, partner, parents or grandparents would [be affected]”).

4) There is no consequence for breaking hygiene rules.

A lot of comments, especially if students reported about non-adherence to hygiene rules were marked with exclamations marks, coded by us as signs of emotional reactions. Further, emotions like “fear” and anger and a feeling of frustration and helplessness were reported (“Critical thinking and criticism and to open your eyes and your mouth are not taught in systems teaching for learning of details and tests. But you do, what you are expected to do. Sadly.”). Further they reported about missing consequences in terms of incompliance to hygiene rules (“There is no consequence, if hygiene rules are broken. This is why they are violated.”) and the need for more regularly controls (“Surprising visits of hygiene experts” and “experts disguised as students”), control systems (“Control system with penalties”) and education tests. Last but not least, the discrepancy of hygiene rules and real behavior of role models, and the impact on medical education was a very common answer (“The boss should respect the hygiene rules”, “you cannot expect students to put something into practice, they never got demonstrated”, “you are learning hygienic behavior from idols, not lectures”).

Discussion

To our knowledge this is the first study in medical students combining assessments of own and others’ competencies for hand hygiene and speaking-up competences.

Main results and hypotheses

Under consideration of the selection bias (see below) hypothesis One (presence of overconfidence) could be confirmed for hand hygiene and feedback reception, but not for feedback provision. Participants rated themselves significantly superior to fellow students in hand hygiene and feedback reception competences (overplacement), but not in Speaking-Up behavior. As overconfidence occurs mainly in abilities perceived to be easy [15], this may indicate that students experience hand hygiene to be of low, but speaking up of high difficulty. The result of rating feedback provision skills not above others competences is contrary to our study in postgraduates [13], where overplacement could be shown. An explanation for this could be, that under-graduates experience barriers and helplessness in speaking up more often than postgraduates do, who correct others more often.

Only 54% of the participants rating themselves to be good or very good trained in hygiene realized the potentially lethal relevance of an omitted hand disinfection. This indicates either for overestimation [17] (with de facto missing knowledge) for the whole competency, or relevant deficits in attitude. Nevertheless, these results confirm prior findings of our working groups in a regional [12] and national [13] post-graduate settings and further results in medical [2022] and non-medical [14, 15, 29] context.

The second hypothesis (education-dependence of overconfidence) could not be confirmed. Ratings were independent from level of education, especially in the “naïve” group C. For this study it was our motivation, to identify the “onset” of overconfidence for an early curricular intervention. Consequently, it seems to be reasonable to say that overconfidence develops after the first exposure to the proficiency [15]. In Germany, medical students have to complete a hospital traineeship before admission to university or within the first four semesters. It is likely, that overconfidence is acquired in this time prior to regular patient contact. Consequently, a governmental structured pre-academic traineeship using reflective methods, known to potentially lower overconfidence [29, 30], and focusing on hand hygiene and feedback may be indicated.

We did not find any other correlations of high reliability (τ > 0.6) which is consistent with prior general findings [15, 17, 31] and results of our research in medical post-graduates [13]. With overconfidence being an ubiquitarian effect independent from academic progress, a change for curricular response to compensate the effect [32] may not be of satisfactory effect. Instead, further research could concentrate on the first exposure to hand hygiene in clinical settings, which may be the cause for the overconfidence, if the attitude learning dimension [33] is not addressed properly.

Speaking-up behavior and patient safety

Our results show an overall low readiness to speak up, even towards persons hierarchically subordinated in clinical settings. Additionally, medical students face intense barriers to speak up on physicians of all hierarchies. These findings are consistent with a study by Schwappach et al. [34] and others [35, 36]: Hierarchic structures with fear for negative consequences or being wrong in case of given feedback were mentioned several times and seem to more powerful than the ethical codex to protect patients from any potential harm. Thus, even two decades after the groundbreaking publication “To Err is human” [37], a constructive human factor management has not progressed to the desirable level. As literature shows, this is not limited to German work places [38] or infection prevention [39, 40] and is moreover a general effect.

On the other hand, students mentioned the needs for change in human factor management several times in the comments section. Conclusively, medical education of pre- and postgraduates is a desired and demanded key element to change mindsets towards a constructive working culture and team interaction. To do so, medical staff of all educational levels have to develop and maintain feedback strategies [4143] and the attitude and motivation to use them.

Qualitative results

In this study, four main themes could be identified. The first theme (insufficiency of medical training in infection prevention) showed high resistances of students and medical staff towards hygiene trainings. These were mentioned to be “boring”, “annoying” and “dry” which is consistent with finding from 1994 [44]. Those could be overcome by more attractive and “gameful” formats [45, 46]. Second, hierarchical barriers and clinical tribalism (“we against them”) reduce the possibility of feedback and intervention, as discussed above. The third main theme, shows demand for ongoing research towards ethical considerations and of dilemmatic factors leading to a potentially “social maladaptation” to actively accept impairment of patient safety due to hierarchical issues. The fourth mentioned, that students get frustrated as they feel not to be part of the staff and that feedback is not of relevance if someone speaks up. With medical students known to be distressed due to intensive psychosocial pressure and even burn-out [47] our findings suggest, that they have to weigh, if hierarchical unwelcome speaking up is too resource-consuming and thus further frustrating.

Conclusively, the combination of students being overconfident for hygiene skills, the experience that non-adherence to hygiene protocols is without consequence, speaking-up is potentially linked to punishment and frustration, it is likely that motivation to visit hygiene trainings is reduced, especially if these are “dry”, “boring” and “far away from reality”. Thus, our initial question has to be changed to “Why should I go to a time-consuming and boring training for methods and rules I already mastered and which are not followed by my teachers themselves?

Results of students’ educational demands

As the results suggests, under-graduates themselves recommended early training in hygiene and feedback, before first contact to patient or skills with demand for hygiene management. Further evaluation showed, that participants see high demand for infection prevention and medication safety skills. For hand hygiene, participants clearly stated that this skill should be trained annually from the very beginning. Additionally, the results depict high demand for education in use of personal protective equipment and management of multi-drug resistant organisms.

Limitations

About 0.8% of the German-speaking medical students participated in our survey [48] leading to the question of representativeness. It seems plausible, that those interested in hygiene, participated in this study and may be de facto better than those who did not. This is known as the self-selection recruitment and non-response bias [49] limiting most open survey studies.

To compensate, we used the completer/non-completer approach [24], a comparison to our preexisting studies [12, 13] and we investigated external literature [17, 29]. Following findings and arguments lower the selection bias (without ruling in out) and the counter-hypothesis, that motivated participants are de facto better in their competences:

  1. The completer/non-completer approach showed no difference between those motivated to complete the survey and those not. If interest in the topic would be an argument for competence, those not interested (non-completers), should be of lesser performance in the tests for overestimation and should show less overconfidence. This was not the case.
  2. Motivation to contribute to surveys is not depending on interest in the subject only, but also on politeness and endorsement by providers [24]. We do not know that for this investigation.
  3. A post-hoc analysis of the participants response rate in groups “C” showed a return rate for contributing universities between 0.12% and 4.1% of the addressed students in these institutions. This would mean, that in those universities with low response rates, students would be less motivated for hygiene trainings. According to the counter-hypothesis they should be worse, also in identification of risks. For items representing the knowledge concerning risks for omitted hand hygiene (item 27 and 28) and self-estimation for compliance (items 29 and 30), we did not detect any difference between students of different universities.

However, these clues do not break the potential bias [50], but limit it. Nevertheless, this study is a multicenter trial showing similar effects in different universities despite assumable different distribution and endorsements of the study and our results are in concordance with psychological investigations from different authors and in different populations.

But, even if participating “biased” students were among the “best” and “most interested”, our results of only 53% of them understanding the potentially lethal effects of omitted hand hygiene, most not speaking up, as well as their statements concerning hygiene education are alarming.

A minor limitation is, that we did not ask for the curricular methods of medical education in hygiene and communication skills differing between the universities. Thus, it may be possible, that there were students from a university less overconfident, but better educated leading to the same net results. Therefore, further work should evaluate the different methods of the universities involved to screen for a hidden effect of the curricula.

Third, we asked about omitted hand hygiene occasions, but not about the cause for it. For further studies, a differentiation between slips and lapses, mistakes and active violations should be considered [51].

Conclusion

Our results confirm the hypothesis that overconfidence effects in hygiene and feedback are present in medical students as they are in postgraduates. These effects are independent from age, gender, semester and prior education. For patient safety, students reported about hierarchical barriers, fear of punishment and absence of consequences for broken hygiene rules leading to frustration and disruption of theory and practice in infection prevention. For education, our study showed the suggestions to implement creative and appealing hygiene programs early in education with annually repetition paired with ongoing reduction of hierarchical barriers. However, our findings are limited by the selection bias especially concerning self-recruitment, so that further work should concentrate on this limitation. Altogether, all health care providers face the demand for change of mindsets concerning hand hygiene and speaking-up practice to develop a constructive work place culture and error management.

Acknowledgments

We thank all contributing universities and students for participating.

References

  1. 1. Till A., Dutta N., and McKimm J., Vertical leadership in highly complex and unpredictable health systems. Br J Hosp Med (Lond), 2016. 77(8): p. 471–5.
  2. 2. Makary M.A. and Daniel M., Medical error-the third leading cause of death in the US. BMJ, 2016. 353: p. i2139. pmid:27143499
  3. 3. Dwyer J. and Faber-Langendoen K., Speaking Up: An Ethical Action Exercise. Acad Med, 2018. 93(4): p. 602–605. pmid:29116984
  4. 4. Kiesewetter J., et al., German undergraduate medical students' attitudes and needs regarding medical errors and patient safety—a national survey in Germany. Med Teach, 2014. 36(6): p. 505–10. pmid:24597660
  5. 5. Pittet D., et al., The World Health Organization Guidelines on Hand Hygiene in Health Care and their consensus recommendations. Infect Control Hosp Epidemiol, 2009. 30(7): p. 611–22. pmid:19508124
  6. 6. WHO Guidelines on Hand Hygiene in Health Care: First Global Patient Safety Challenge Clean Care Is Safer Care. 2009, Geneva: World Health Organization.
  7. 7. Qasmi S.A., et al., Guiding hand hygiene interventions among future healthcare workers: implications of knowledge, attitudes, and social influences. Am J Infect Control, 2018.
  8. 8. Mubeen S.R., Norman, The MEasurement of Motivation with Science Students European Journal of Educational Research, 2014. 3(3): p. 129–144.
  9. 9. Pelaccia T. and Viau R., Motivation in medical education(). Med Teach, 2017. 39(2): p. 136–140. pmid:27866457
  10. 10. Haessler S., et al., Getting doctors to clean their hands: lead the followers. BMJ Qual Saf, 2012. 21(6): p. 499–502. pmid:22357778
  11. 11. Eckmanns T., et al., Compliance with antiseptic hand rub use in intensive care units: the Hawthorne effect. Infect Control Hosp Epidemiol, 2006. 27(9): p. 931–4. pmid:16941318
  12. 12. Bushuven S., et al., Overconfidence in infection control proficiency. Am J Infect Control, 2018.
  13. 13. Bushuven S., et al., Cognitive bias in professional hand hygiene and feedback: A national online-survey on overconfidence in Germany. Infect Control Hosp Epidemiol, 2019. 40(8): p. 943–946. pmid:31294685
  14. 14. Sandroni A., S.F., A Survey on Overconfidence. Mimeo 2004.
  15. 15. David Dunning C.H., Jerry M. Sula, Flawed Self-Assessment—Implications for Health, Education and the Workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2004. 5(3): p. 69–106. pmid:26158995
  16. 16. Malmendier U T.G., CEO Overconfidence and Corporate Investment. Journal of Finance, 2005. 60(6): p. 2661–2700.
  17. 17. Moore D.A. and Healy P.J., The trouble with overconfidence. Psychol Rev, 2008. 115(2): p. 502–17. pmid:18426301
  18. 18. Christensen S.S., Escape from the diffusion of responsibility: A review and guide for nurses. J Nurs Manag, 2019. 27(2): p. 264–270. pmid:30178904
  19. 19. Jochemsen-van der Leeuw H.G., et al., Learning from a role model: A cascade or whirlpool effect? Med Teach, 2015. 37(5): p. 482–9. pmid:25213300
  20. 20. van De Mortel T.F., et al., A comparison of the hand hygiene knowledge, beliefs and practices of Italian nursing and medical students. J Adv Nurs, 2012. 68(3): p. 569–79. pmid:21722171
  21. 21. Neo J.R., et al., Evidence-based practices to increase hand hygiene compliance in health care facilities: An integrated review. Am J Infect Control, 2016. 44(6): p. 691–704. pmid:27240800
  22. 22. Skodova M., et al., Hand hygiene technique quality evaluation in nursing and medicine students of two academic courses. Rev Lat Am Enfermagem, 2015. 23(4): p. 708–17. pmid:26444174
  23. 23. Schwappach D.L. and Gehring K., Silence that can be dangerous: a vignette study to assess healthcare professionals' likelihood of speaking up about safety concerns. PLoS One, 2014. 9(8): p. e104720. pmid:25116338
  24. 24. Hoerger M., Participant dropout as a function of survey length in internet-mediated university studies: implications for study design and voluntary participation in psychological research. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw, 2010. 13(6): p. 697–700. pmid:21142995
  25. 25. Lakens D., Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front Psychol, 2013. 4: p. 863. pmid:24324449
  26. 26. Janesick V., The Choregraphy of Qualitative Research: Minuets, Improvisation and Crytallization. Strategies of Qualitative Inquiery, 2003: p. 46–79.
  27. 27. Bradley E.H., Curry L.A., and Devers K.J., Qualitative data analysis for health services research: developing taxonomy, themes, and theory. Health Serv Res, 2007. 42(4): p. 1758–72. pmid:17286625
  28. 28. Braithwaite J., et al., The basis of clinical tribalism, hierarchy and stereotyping: a laboratory-controlled teamwork experiment. BMJ Open, 2016. 6(7): p. e012467. pmid:27473955
  29. 29. Sandroni A., A Survey on Overconfidence, Insurance and Self-Assessment Training Programs. 2018.
  30. 30. Brown J.G., FM, Coming to terms with failure: private self-enhancement and public self-effacement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1992. 28: p. 3–22.
  31. 31. Prims J.P. and Moore D.A., Overconfidence over the lifespan. Judgm Decis Mak, 2017. 12(1): p. 29–41. pmid:29861807
  32. 32. Emich K.J., But consider the alternative: the influence of positive affect on overconfidence. Cogn Emot, 2014. 28(8): p. 1382–97. pmid:24467720
  33. 33. Thomas P.A., et al., Curriculum Development for Medical Education JHU Press, 2016. 3.
  34. 34. Schwappach D.L., [When silence is dangerous: "Speaking-up" about safety concerns]. Z Evid Fortbild Qual Gesundhwes, 2016. 114: p. 5–12. pmid:27566265
  35. 35. Etchegaray J.M., et al., Barriers to Speaking Up About Patient Safety Concerns. J Patient Saf, 2017.
  36. 36. Landgren R., et al., Barriers of Pediatric Residents to Speaking Up About Patient Safety. Hosp Pediatr, 2016. 6(12): p. 738–743. pmid:27909093
  37. 37. Havens D.H. and Boroughs L., "To err is human": a report from the Institute of Medicine. J Pediatr Health Care, 2000. 14(2): p. 77–80. pmid:10736144
  38. 38. Brennan P.A. and Davidson M., Improving patient safety: we need to reduce hierarchy and empower junior doctors to speak up. BMJ, 2019. 366: p. l4461. pmid:31266748
  39. 39. Hooper P., et al., Junior doctors' views on reporting concerns about patient safety: a qualitative study. Postgrad Med J, 2015. 91(1075): p. 251–6. pmid:25898840
  40. 40. Reynolds M., et al., Improving feedback on junior doctors' prescribing errors: mixed-methods evaluation of a quality improvement project. BMJ Qual Saf, 2017. 26(3): p. 240–247. pmid:27044881
  41. 41. Ahmed M., et al., Operation debrief: a SHARP improvement in performance feedback in the operating room. Ann Surg, 2013. 258(6): p. 958–63. pmid:23478533
  42. 42. Norcini J., The power of feedback. Med Educ, 2010. 44(1): p. 16–7. pmid:20078751
  43. 43. Ramani S. and Krackov S.K., Twelve tips for giving feedback effectively in the clinical environment. Med Teach, 2012. 34(10): p. 787–91. pmid:22730899
  44. 44. Harris M.M. and Yuan J., "Oh, no, not another hand-washing in-service!". Gastroenterol Nurs, 1994. 16(6): p. 269–72. pmid:8075163
  45. 45. Morrell B.L.M. and Ball H.M., Can You Escape Nursing School? Educational Escape Room in Nursing Education. Nurs Educ Perspect, 2020. 41(3): p. 197–198. pmid:30614962
  46. 46. Offner D., et al., Evaluation of an ethical method aimed at improving hygiene rules compliance in dental practice. Am J Infect Control, 2016. 44(6): p. 666–70. pmid:26905791
  47. 47. Dyrbye L.N., Thomas M.R., and Shanafelt T.D., Systematic review of depression, anxiety, and other indicators of psychological distress among U.S. and Canadian medical students. Acad Med, 2006. 81(4): p. 354–73. pmid:16565188
  48. 48. Statistica, Anzahl der Studenten im Fach Allgemeinmedizin in Deutschland im Zeitraum von 1998 bis 2018. 2019.
  49. 49. Khazaal Y., et al., Does self-selection affect samples' representativeness in online surveys? An investigation in online video game research. J Med Internet Res, 2014. 16(7): p. e164. pmid:25001007
  50. 50. Keeble C.L., G; Barber S, Baxter P, Choosing a Method to Reduce Selection Bias: A tool for Researchers. Open Journal of Epidemiology, 2015. 5: p. 155–162.
  51. 51. Vincent C., Patient Safety. monography 2015. 2.