Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 10 July 2014
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
Volume 6 - 2014 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00146

Differential behavioral and physiological effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy adults of younger and older age

  • 1Brain Imaging and Neurostimulation (BINS) Laboratory, Department of Neurology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Neurology, University Hospital Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland

Changes in γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) mediated synaptic transmission have been associated with age-related motor and cognitive functional decline. Since anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (atDCS) has been suggested to target cortical GABAergic inhibitory interneurons, its potential for the treatment of deficient inhibitory activity and functional decline is being increasingly discussed. Therefore, after-effects of a single session of atDCS on resting-state and event-related short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) as evaluated with double-pulse TMS and dexterous manual performance were examined using a sham-controlled cross-over design in a sample of older and younger participants. The atDCS effect on resting-state inhibition differed in direction, magnitude, and timing, i.e., late relative release of inhibition in the younger and early relative increase in inhibition in the older. More pronounced release of event-related inhibition after atDCS was exclusively seen in the older. Event-related modulation of inhibition prior to stimulation predicted the magnitude of atDCS-induced effects on resting-state inhibition. Specifically, older participants with high modulatory capacity showed a disinhibitory effect comparable to the younger. Beneficial effects on behavior were mainly seen in the older and in tasks requiring higher dexterity, no clear association with physiological changes was found. Differential effects of atDCS on SICI, discussed to reflect GABAergic inhibition at the level of the primary motor cortex, might be distinct in older and younger participants depending on the functional integrity of the underlying neural network. Older participants with preserved modulatory capacity, i.e., a physiologically “young” motor network, were more likely to show a disinhibitory effect of atDCS. These results favor individually tailored application of tDCS with respect to specific target groups.

Introduction

Changes in the excitation-inhibition balance with advancing age involving alterations of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) mediated synaptic transmission (Grachev and Apkarian, 2001; Grachev et al., 2001; Pinto et al., 2010; Gaetz et al., 2011) have been proposed as (one) underlying mechanism for age-related motor and cognitive functional decline (Gleichmann et al., 2011). Regarding the direction of alterations of the human motor system however, controversial findings exist for respective surrogate markers of GABAergic inhibition extracted from various electrophysiological methods (Hortobagyi et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2009; McGinley et al., 2010; Marneweck et al., 2011; Rossiter et al., 2014). It has previously been shown that deficient resting-state motorcortical inhibition determines poor event-related modulation, i.e., less fast and precise release of inhibition during movement preparation, a finding that was closely associated with decrements in manual dexterity (Heise et al., 2013). Moreover, insufficient induction of cortical plasticity in response to motor training has also been related to imbalanced inhibitory activity in older participants (Sawaki et al., 2003; Fujiyama et al., 2009; Rogasch et al., 2009).

In times of an average life expectancy far beyond 70 years of age in high-income countries (World Health Organization, 2012), ameliorating the negative prize that comes with advancing age seems appealing. On the basis of findings in young healthy participants, it has been suggested that modulating cortical excitability with anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (atDCS) is causally linked to behavioral improvement in the cognitive and motor domain (Nitsche et al., 2003c, 2006; Antal et al., 2004a,b; Fregni et al., 2005; for review Reis and Fritsch, 2011; Sohn et al., 2012). Anodal tDCS over the primary motor cortex (MI) led to enhanced motor performance (Boggio et al., 2006; Vines et al., 2006; Cogiamanian et al., 2007; Matsuo et al., 2011) and increased efficiency of motor learning in healthy participants in a variety of behavioral paradigms (Galea and Celnik, 2009; Hunter et al., 2009; Reis et al., 2009; Stagg et al., 2011a,c).

Since mild direct current stimulation most likely affects horizontal intracortical interneurons of which GABAergic neurons represent the largest neuronal population which exert strong inhibition upon the pyramidal cell (Lang et al., 2011), GABA-mediated inhibition has been proposed to constitute one potential target of atDCS (Nitsche et al., 2004, 2005; Stagg et al., 2009, 2011b). Hence, for conditions marked by deficient inhibitory activity as it has been shown to occur on the one hand in disease, like following a stroke (Hummel et al., 2009; Edwards et al., 2013; Honaga et al., 2013; Liuzzi et al., 2014), in movement disorders (Beck et al., 2008; Heise et al., 2010; Jackson et al., 2013), or on the other hand in the course of healthy aging (McGinley et al., 2010; Marneweck et al., 2011; Heise et al., 2013), this mechanism would be of particular interest and could offer a potential tool to target age-related functional decline. In accordance with this idea, enhanced motor behavior and augmented effects of motor training have been found after stimulation in participants of older age (Hummel et al., 2010; Goodwill et al., 2013; Zimerman et al., 2013).

Therefore, one point of interest of the present work was whether atDCS is able to perturb intracortical inhibition measured by means of double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (dpTMS) in resting-state and event-related conditions. The driving hypothesis was that level of resting-state inhibition is shifted as to open up a window and subsequently allow for more pronounced event-related modulation to occur. Furthermore, evidence for a direct association between stimulation induced changes in motorcortical excitability/inhibition and motor function is scarce. For that reason the present work addressed the question whether atDCS-induced changes in the intracortical inhibitory network within the primary motor cortex would be associated in direction and magnitude with stimulation-induced changes in dexterous motor behavior in older and younger participants.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Young (N = 16 young average age 24.27 ± 1.6 years, range 22–28, 8 female) and old (N = 16 old average age 73.4 ± 6.3 years, range 65–83, 7 female) healthy participants volunteered in the experiment. All were right-handed (Oldfield, 1971) and none reported a history of serious medical, neurological or psychiatric diseases or any contraindications for tDCS or TMS, as probed by a standardized questionnaire based on available safety recommendations (Nitsche et al., 2008; Rossi et al., 2009). In all participants, the score of Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975) was ≥29/30. Subjects were naïve to the experimental purpose and none of them were professional piano players or trained as a typist. All participants gave full written informed consent to participate in the experiment in accordance with the ethics committee of the Medical Counsel Hamburg (protocol number PV3770).

Experimental Set Up

The effect of atDCS on resting-state and event-related intracortical inhibition and behavior was tested in a double blind crossover design. Participants and all study personal involved in data acquisition and analysis were blind regarding the type of stimulation condition. All volunteers participated in two separate sessions (anodal/sham stimulation). The order of anodal vs. sham stimulation was pseudorandomized within each age group. A minimum interval of 48 h between sessions (range 2–33 days) assured complete wash-out of the single-session atDCS effect (Nitsche et al., 2008). Within each session, the measurement of electrophysiological and behavioral parameters was performed before, and at three time points—immediately, 45, and 90 min (P, P45, P90)—after stimulation (Figure 1A). The time points of measurement are based on previous findings showing physiological effects outlasting stimulation for up to 90 min (Nitsche and Paulus, 2001; Nitsche et al., 2003b). Attention and fatigue were repeatedly evaluated during each session at the beginning of every measurement time point (separate Visual Analog Scales).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental setup. (A) Volunteers participated in two separate sessions (anodal/sham stimulation), pseudorandomized within each age group. Time flow within each single (anodal/sham stimulation) session: resting-state and event-related TMS as well as behavioral measurements were performed immediately, 45, and 90 min (P, P45, P90) after 20 min of anodal or sham transcranial direct current stimulation. (B) TMS paradigm for event-related SICImove measurement. Based on the individual reaction time (iRT), determined at the beginning of each measurement time point before the TMS experiment, unconditioned and conditioned TMS was applied early (~25% of iRT) and late (~90% of iRT) during the preparation of a visually paced index finger adduction for acquisition of event-related TMS data. A total of 16 pulses were applied at every measurement time point for each stimulation condition during resting-state, and per time zone during event-related measurement. (C–E) Behavioral measurements: Dexterous manual behavior was tested with three different task involving FDI activity with graded level of complexity tested in pseudorandomized order. (C) Solitary index finger tapping (1FT) and (D) alternating index and little finger tapping (2FT) were recorded over 3 × 10 s between GO- and STOP-signal. In both tasks the subjects were instructed to tap as fast and as precise as possible on pre-defined buttons of a 4-digit keypad. (E) During the choice-reaction time task (CRT) participants were asked to respond as fast as possible to a (neutrally pre-cued) target stimulus indicating either a left or a right index finger key press starting from a standardized middle position on a standard keyboard. Inter-trial intervals jittered between 1 and 6 s. For all experiments participants were seated with forearms supported on a table. Any whole hand, wrist, or arm movements were physically restricted throughout all behavioral experiments. Hand positioning on the keyboard assured movement of respective fingers only. Please note that the graphs depict the behavioral tasks without restriction for display purposes only.

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation

Anodal trancranial direct current stimulation (atDCS) was delivered with an intensity of 1 mA for 20 min in the tDCS session using an eldith DC-Stimulator (neuroConn, Ilmenau, Germany) with two 25 cm2 saline-soaked gel-sponge electrodes (0.04 mA/cm2 current density). In accordance with established protocols (Nitsche et al., 2008), the anode was positioned over the representation of the first dorsal interosseus muscle (FDI) within the left MI, which was determined with single pulse TMS (Figure 1A). The cathode was placed on the skin overlying the contralateral supraorbital region (Nitsche and Paulus, 2000).

Anodal tDCS applied in this way results in an increase in excitability of the underlying MI that outlasts the period of stimulation (Lang et al., 2004). Sham stimulation was administered according to a well-established protocol (Gandiga et al., 2006). At the onset of both interventions (atDCS and Sham), current was increased in a ramp-like fashion eliciting a transient tingling sensation on the scalp that faded over seconds and that elicited comparable perceptions. Current remained at the 1 mA level for 20 min in the tDCS session and for up to 30 s in the Sham session. In both sessions, currents were turned off slowly over 8 s; a procedure that does not elicit perceptions and that was implemented out of the field of view of the subjects. During the application of stimulation participants were presented a video to enhance blinding and assure level of attention and alertness.

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Two Magstim 200 magnetic stimulators connected via one Bistim module (Magstim Company, Whitland, Dyfed, UK) and one figure-of-eight coil with 80 mm wing diameter were used for single and double pulse application. The coil was placed over the hand motor area, with the handle in antero-medial orientation, ~45° to the interhemispheric line. Procedures to establish motor hotspot for the first dorsal interosseus muscle (FDI) and resting motor threshold followed standardized procedures (Siebner and Rothwell, 2003). Resting motor threshold (rMT) was defined as the percentage of maximum of stimulator output (%MSO) to produce MEP amplitudes of at least 50 μV in five out of ten consecutive trials (Rossini et al., 1999). Subthreshold conditioning stimulus was followed by a suprathreshold test stimulus in the paired pulse paradigm. We used an interstimulus interval of 3 ms to evaluate SICI (Kujirai et al., 1993). Conditioning stimulus was set at 80% of resting motor threshold (Ziemann et al., 1996b) and the test stimulus was adjusted to elicit unconditioned MEP amplitudes of 0.5–1 mV peak-to-peak. Since the focus of the current work was set on evaluating the specific effect of atDCS on SICI, stimulus intensities (CS and TS) were re-adjusted prior to every measurement time point (BL, P, P45, P90).

Based on the individual reaction time (iRT), determined at the beginning of each measurement time point before the TMS experiment, unconditioned and conditioned TMS was applied early (~25% of iRT) and late (~90% of iRT) during the preparation of a visually paced index finger adduction for acquisition of event-related TMS data as described previously (Hummel et al., 2009; Heise et al., 2010, 2013) (Figure 1B). A total of 16 pulses were applied at every measurement time point for each stimulation condition during resting-state, and per time zone during event-related measurement.

EMG signals were recorded with disposable surface Ag/AgCl electrodes from the FDI in a belly-tendon montage, amplified, and digitized (CED MICRO 1401, Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) and electronically stored for off-line analysis.

Behavioral Data

Dexterous manual behavior was tested with three different tasks involving FDI activity with graded level of complexity as described previously (Heise et al., 2013).

Solitary index finger tapping (1FT, Figure 1C) and alternating index and little finger tapping (2FT, Figure 1D) were recorded over 3 × 10 s between GO- and STOP-signal. In both tasks the subjects were instructed to tap as fast and as precise as possible on pre-defined buttons of a 4-digit keypad. The order of behavioral tasks was pseudorandomized across session and participant within each measurement time point (BL, P, P45, P90).

During the choice-reaction time task (CRT, Figure 1E), participants were asked to respond as fast as possible to a (neutrally pre-cued) target stimulus indicating either a left or a right index finger key press starting from a standardized middle position on a standard keyboard. Inter-trial intervals jittered between 1 and 6 s.

Visual cues were provided on a 20-inch computer screen by Presentation software (Neurobehavioral Systems, Inc., Albany, CA, USA), also used to record response parameters (number of key presses, reaction time, key-press intervals, key selection) for off-line analyzes.

For all experiments participants were seated with forearms supported on a table. Any whole hand, wrist, or arm movements were physically restricted throughout all behavioral experiments. Hand positioning on the keyboard assured movement of respective fingers only. Participants were tested on the dominant right hand only.

Control Experiment

Since we were not able to exactly reproduce the atDCS effect on SICI of previous findings (Nitsche et al., 2005; Kidgell et al., 2013), who examined SICI with a CS adjusted to 70% of active motor threshold (aMT) and different stimulation intensities, we introduced a control experiment, consisting of two additional sessions. In young healthy subjects (N = 5, presenting not the expected release of inhibition after atDCS), SICI was measured with two different paradigms (setting 1: CS adjusted to 80%rMT, setting 2: CS adjusted to 70% aMT) before and after (immediately and 45 min) either 7 or 13 min of stimulation duration in two separate sessions (≥24 h inter-session interval, order counter-balanced). For each SICI setting and time point (BL, P, P45) 20 conditioned and 20 unconditioned stimuli were applied.

Data Processing

Any EMG-data contaminated with muscle activity before TMS pulses were discarded from further analysis after visual inspection. Motor-evoked potential (MEP) amplitudes were measured peak-to-peak. As it is standard practice SICI was normalized to the corresponding unconditioned MEP (SICI = conditioned MEP/unconditioned MEP × 100) at either resting-state (SICIrest) or respective pre-move time zones for event-related modulation (SICImove) (Hummel et al., 2009; Heise et al., 2010, 2013).

Behavioral data were processed using a customized automated log file parser to calculate outcome variables (1FT: response time for inter-tap interval, 2FT: response time for valid transitions between finger V and II, CRT: response time for correct key presses).

Statistical Analyzes

A random coefficient multilevel model was used for analysis of stimulation-induced change in resting-state (SICIrest) and event-related SICI modulation (SICImove), as well as in behavioral data (1FT, 2FT, CRT). Primary outcome was the stimulation-induced change (Δ) in all dependent variables (DV), normalized to baseline (ΔSICIrest, ΔSICImove, Δ1FT, Δ2FT, ΔCRT).

Stimulation induced change and its temporal pattern among age groups was the main focus of the analysis. Therefore, separate models were fitted for each DV with STIMULATION CONDITION (atDCS, sham), GROUP (old, young), and TIME POINT of measurement within each session (BL, P, P45, P90) as fixed factors.

Change in SICI induction during movement preparation (SICImove) was fitted as a linear trend from early to late pre-move phase and added as a covariate (pTIMEST) to the model, since this has been proven to adequately estimate event-related SICI modulation (Heise et al., 2013). Improved model fit was tested including random intercept for (i) SUBJECT and (ii) linear slope (pTIMEST) as random slope for the SICImove model, since we expected a certain amount of intra-individual variance to be influenced by stimulation condition. In order to estimate variances of random effects, restricted maximum likelihood (REML) criteria were employed. Model selection was strictly hypothesis driven and dictated by the experimental design, therefore all fixed effects and interactions remained in the final model. Final model selection was based on Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) for model comparison, normalized residuals based on REML fit served for model validation (Brown and Prescott, 2006; Pinheiro and Bates, 2009).

In order to exclude any crossover effect of stimulation on behavioral outcome, which we expected to change with constant practice although participants were familiarized to the tasks prior to the experiment, factor SEQUENCE (order of stimulation conditions over sessions) was introduced. Furthermore, to be able to differentiate the latter from a pure effect of training, SESSION (1st, 2nd) was also modeled as fixed effect as recommended for the analysis of longitudinal cross-over experiments (DíAz-Uriarte, 2002).

Since previous findings revealed an association between better event-related SICI modulation and more skilful manual performance (Heise et al., 2013), here principal component analysis (PCA) was used to extract the common information of “dexterous manual performance” (1FT, 2FT, CRT pooled for BL) and entered into correlation analysis with event-related SICImove modulation (linear slope, pTIMEST) to verify this association. In a next step, the association between stimulation-induced change (net change, ΔatDCS − Δsham) in resting-state and event-related SICI, as well as respective association with net change in behavior was analyzed. Furthermore, the question was whether baseline event-related SICI modulation was associated with stimulation-induced change of resting-state SICI (SICImove modulation at BL and ΔSICIrest pooled for P and P45).

Since in the 2FT task, no stable level of performance was achieved but an obvious performance improvement (5–10% speed increase) was also observed in the sham condition, skill learning had to be assumed. Therefore, a secondary analysis of the learning curve in the 2FT task (outcome centered) was performed using a growth curve, i.e., modeling time (continuous) as linear trend, second and third order polynomial (poly[TIME 3]linear, quadratic, cubic), to estimate the curvilinear temporal pattern of performance change over time. As fixed factors STIMULATION CONDITION and AGE GROUP and their respective interactions with time were modeled. Random intercept and slope and STIMULATION CONDITION nested within subjects (~time | ID/STIMULATION CONDITION), were modeled using an autoregressive covariance structure: AR(1). Effect sizes of atDCS on individual learning curves, using random effects of the growth curve model (representing the individual deviation from the estimated subgroup mean), were correlated (partial correlation correcting for age group) with atDCS-induced (net) change of SICIrest and SICImove modulation.

Control Experiment

Stimulation-induced change in SICI (ΔSICI: conditioned/unconditioned MEP amplitude*100, expressed as difference from BL) was analyzed with fixed factors TIME POINT (P, P45), STIMULATION DURATION (7, 13 min), and SICI SETTING (CS 70%aMT, CS 80%rMT) and tested for their interaction. Modeling random intercept for SUBJECT improved model fit.

Data cleaning was kept to a minimum, as recommended for reaction time data (Baayen and Milin, 2010), only excluding physically impossible trials (e.g., CRT <100 ms). Each part of the analysis was carried out on the maximum available data set. Missing data were not replaced or imputed, neither in outcome nor in independent variable or covariates. Data preparation and statistical analyzes were performed using the software package R for Statistical Computing version 2.15.1 (2012-06-022, www.r-project.org/) for Mac OS X GUI 1.40-devel Leopard build 64-bit, package nlme version 3.1-104 for linear mixed effects modeling (Pinheiro et al., 2012), package multcomp version 1.2-12 for simultaneous post-hoc pairwise comparison and respective corrections (Hothorn et al., 2008). Partial correlations (Kendall's τ) adjusting for factor for AGE GROUP were calculated using ppcor package version 1.0 (Kim, 2012), multiple correlations were computed using percentage-bend correlations (Wilcox, 2012), adjusted correlation coefficients (rpb), percentile bootstrap 95% confidence intervals for correlation coefficients, value of test statistics (Tpb), and level of significance are given.

All graphical presentation of data is done with packages lattice version 0.20-6 (Sarkar, 2008) and ggplot2 version 0.9.1 (Wickham, 2009). Results for random coefficient models are given as Type III sums of squares for sequentially fitted fixed effects (F, df, p), Wald statistics for marginal parameter estimates (t, df, p, 95% CI), as well as variance component estimates for random effects (variance, SD). Significant results from simultaneous pairwise post-hoc comparison with Tukey Contrast are given with adjusted p-value for estimates of contrasts (estimate ± SE, z-value, adjusted p).

Results

Change in Resting-State SICI (ΔSICIrest)

Average SICIrest and ΔSICIrest are given in Table 1 for stimulation conditions nested within age group per time point. Absolute resting-state SICI was significantly diminished (less inhibition) in older subjects (for details in analysis of absolute SICI refer to supplemental online results).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive data for resting-state and event-related SICI for stimulation conditions within age group for separate time point.

A clear differentiation of the effect of atDCS on change in resting-state SICI (ΔSICIrest) was found between age groups [STIMULATION CONDITION × AGE GROUP, F(1, 2879) = 10.80, p < 0.001, Figure 2, Tables 2 and 3]. While the old group showed overall a significant increase of inhibition (more inhibition) under atDCS compared to the sham condition (older subgroup, sham vs. atDCS: 13.51 ± 3.82, z = 3.54, p < 0.005), there was no significant contrast between stimulation conditions in the young group, showing a trend of disinhibition after both stimulation conditions (younger subgroup, sham vs. atDCS: −4.22 ± 3.75, z = −1.13, p > 0.6). Furthermore, there was a trend for time to modulate this interaction [TIME POINT × STIMULATION CONDITION × AGE GROUP, F(2, 2879) = 2.85, p = 0.058], parameter estimates indicated that the differential atDCS effect was immediately present after stimulation cessation in the older group but increased after stimulation in the younger group. The necessity of adjusting for significant intra-individual variability in ΔSICIrest was shown by improved model fit (smaller BIC) with modeling a random intercept for subject.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Change in resting-state SICI (ΔSICIrest, conditioned MEP/unconditioned MEP amplitude*100 normalized to BL) was distinguishable among age groups. Older participants showed an increase of inhibition under atDCS compared to the sham condition (p < 0.005), while younger participants tended to show disinhibition after both stimulation conditions. The differential atDCS effect tended to be modulated by time, immediately present after stimulation cessation in the older group but rising in extent with increasing time after stimulation in the younger group (p = 0.06). ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, °p < 0.1.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Models for change in SICI resting-state and even-related SICI (DV: ΔSICIrest, ΔSICImove).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. F-tests (type III sums of squares) for change of resting-state and even-related SICI (DV: ΔSICIrest, ΔSICImove).

Change in Event-Related SICI (ΔSICImove)

Absolute event-related SICI modulation was drastically diminished in the old group irrespective of stimulation condition or time point of measurement as demonstrated previously (Heise et al., 2013) (Table 1, details are given in supplemental online results). A significant effect of stimulation on change in SICImove slope was found to diverge among age groups [STIMULATION CONDITION × AGE GROUP, F(1, 150) = 4.36, p < 0.05, ΔSICImove, Tables 2 and 3]. In the older group, a trend toward increase in modulatory capacity after atDCS was found (OLDER subgroup, sham vs. atDCS: −219.2 ± 95.6, z = −2.29, p = 0.09) while modulation remained unaffected by stimulation condition in the younger group (YOUNGER subgroup, sham vs. atDCS: 63.32 ± 95.6, z = 0.66, p > 0.9, Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Change in event-related SICI modulation (linear slope normalized to BL slope, in%, ±s.e.m.). Older participants showed a non-significant trend toward increase in modulatory capacity after atDCS (p = 0.09) while modulation remained unaffected by stimulation condition in the younger group (p > 0.9). Black indicates change in atDCS condition, white depicts sham condition. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, °p < 0.1.

Behavioral Data

Detailed results for absolute behavioral performance are given in supplemental online material. Here, only results for main outcome, i.e., change in behavior normalized to BL (Δ1FT, Δ2FT, ΔCRT), are given.

Change in 1FT (Δ1FT)

Performance in 1FT task was marked by a significant performance decrease, i.e., increase of intertap response time, over time (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Descriptive data for behavioral measures 1FT, 2FT, and CRT (absolute in ms and change Δin%) given as mean ± s.e.m. for stimulation conditions nested within age group per time point.

Analyzing change in 1FT (Δ1FT, Tables 5 and 6) showed an increasing change in intertapping time, i.e., worsening of 1FT performance over time, revealed by a main effect of time point [F(1, 19197) = 12.76, p < 0.001]. Furthermore, a main effect of stimulation condition [F(1, 19197) = 6.14, p < 0.05], which was modulated by age group [AGE GROUP × STIMULATION CONDITION, F(1, 19197) = 69.73, p < 0.0001]. Parameter estimates indicated that performance decrements were evident in younger participants under any condition and older participants under sham condition but performance tended to remain stable in older under atDCS, however, not confirmed by post-hoc pairwise testing (OLDER subgroup, atDCS vs. sham: 3.14 ± 1.51, z = 2.08, p > 0.1, atDCS subgroup, OLDER vs. YOUNGER: 4.17 ± 2.22, z = 1.88, p > 0.2; all other contrasts p > 0.7, Figure 4A).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Wald statistics for performance change in behavioral measures (DV: Δ1FT, Δ2FT, ΔCRT) model.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. F-tests (type III sums of squares) for performance change in behavioral measures (DV: Δ1FT, Δ2FT, ΔCRT).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) (upper row, left) Change in 1FT normalized to BL (Δ1FT) over time. 1FT was marked by performance decrements in younger participants under any condition and older participants under sham condition. Performance tended to remain stable in older under atDCS. (B) (bottom) Change in 2FT normalized to BL (Δ2FT) over time. Older participants showed clear advantage under atDCS early after stimulation, at P and P45, while younger participants benefitted from atDCS at late time point P90. (C) (upper row right) Change in CRT normalized to BL (ΔCRT) over time. Older participants showed stable performance under sham and overall increasing response speed under atDCS. Left column depicts old subgroup, right column shows young subgroup, black indicates change in atDCS condition, and white depicts sham condition. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, °p < 0.1.

Change in 2FT (Δ2FT)

Average transition times of absolute 2FT are given in Table 4. While young participants were faster, there was an overall increase in performance speed.

The analysis of change in 2FT performance (Δ2FT, Tables 5 and 6) revealed a differentiation of performance change between age groups over time, which was significantly modulated by stimulation condition [STIMULATION CONDITION × TIME POINT × AGE GROUP, F(2, 14078) = 9.68, p < 0.0001, Table 5]. Simultaneous pairwise post-hoc testing confirmed a significant difference in performance change between stimulation conditions showing a clear advantage under atDCS in older participants early after stimulation, at time point P (sham vs. atDCS 5.99 ± 1.70, z = 3.52, p < 0.05) and P45 (sham vs. atDCS 7.40 ± 1.67, z = 4.42, p < 0.01), but not later at time point P90 (sham vs. atDCS 4.11 ± 1.67, z = 2.47, p > 0.2). In younger participants, no difference in performance change between stimulation conditions was found early after stimulation cessation, at P (sham vs. atDCS −1.29 ± 1.24, z = −1.04, p > 0.9) or P45 (sham vs. atDCS −2.30 ± 1.23, z = −1.87, p > 0.7), but at late time point P90 performance increase was larger under atDCS (sham vs. atDCS 6.60 ± 1.22, z = 5.39, p < 0.01, Figure 4B).

Change in CRT (ΔCRT)

Overall, participants performed with an average response time of 544.6 ms in the CRT, corresponding to an intercept of 6.3 ± 0.02 on the log scale. Younger participants were on average 6% faster and a significant decrease of average CRT performance time was observable over time (Table 4).

Analyzing change in CRT (ΔCRT, Tables 5 and 6) revealed a differentiation in the stimulation effect among age group [AGE GROUP × STIMULATION CONDITION, F(1, 6276) = 10.93, p < 0.001, Table 5]. Simultaneous pairwise post-hoc testing verified that this effect was driven by the older group, who showed stable performance under sham and overall increasing response speed under atDCS (sham vs. atDCS 2.11 ± 0.71, z = 2.98, p < 0.05, Figure 4C). Furthermore, performance change over time differed among age groups [AGE GROUP × TIME POINT, F(2, 6276) = 4.18, p < 0.05], with decreasing performance improvement in older and increasing performance in younger. However, this effect was small and did not survive post-hoc testing (all pairwise contrasts p > 0.2).

Correlation Between Initial SICImove Modulation and “Dexterous Manual Performance”

Correlation analysis between “dexterous manual performance” (expressed as main component from PCA) and SICImove revealed an association between more pronounced SICImove modulation and more dexterous performance at baseline [τ = −0.56, T(30) = −6.63, p < 0.01].

Correlation Between atDCS-Induced Changes in Resting-State SICI and Behavior (Pooled Over P and P45)

Net change in SICIrest was not associated with net change in 1FT [partial correlation, τ = −0.14, p > 0.2, T(29) = −1.11]. However, in the younger group, more pronounced net disinhibition was associated with net increase (faster) in 1FT performance [rpb = −0.58, CI = −0.85 − −0.13, Tpb(14) = −2.67, p < 0.05], while no association was observed in the older group [rpb = −0.03, CI = −0.46 − 0.61, Tpb(14) = −2.67, p > 0.8]. In the case of 2FT, overall a weak trend was seen for net increase in inhibition to be associated with net decrease in performance, i.e., relative increase of performance time [partial correlation, τ = −0.19, p > 0.1, T(29) = −1.4]. This direction in the association was mainly driven by the older age group, however not significant [older: rpb = −0.42, CI = −0.77 − 0.11, Tpb(14) = −1.74, p = 0.1; younger: rpb = 0.08, CI = −0.57 − 0.62, Tpb(14) = 0.30, p > 0.7]. Stimulation-induced change in CRT performance showed overall a moderate association with change in resting-state SICI [partial correlation, τ = 0.29, p < 0.05, T(29) = 2.32], indicating net inhibition to correlate with net performance increase, i.e., faster CRT. This association tended to be more pronounced in the older [older: rpb = 0.48, CI = −0.15 − 0.80, Tpb(14) = 2.02, p = 0.09; younger: rpb = 0.44, CI = −0.09 − 0.75, Tpb(14) = 1.84, p = 0.09], however not reaching level of significance when analyzed for age groups separately.

Correlation Between Stimulation-Induced Change in Event-Related SICI Modulation and Change in Behavior

No association between net change in event-related SICI modulation (ΔpTIMESTatDCS − ΔpTIMESTsham) and net change in behavior was found.

Correlation Stimulation-Induced Change in Resting-State and Event-Related SICI Modulation

No association was found between stimulation-induced net change in resting-state and event-related SICI, neither when pooled over all time points, nor when analyzed for single time point.

Correlation Between Initial SICImove Modulation and atDCS-Induced ΔSICIrest

However, initial event-related SICI modulation might serve as potential predictor for stimulation-induced change in resting-state SICI. Partial correlation (controlling for AGE GROUP) between event-related SICI modulation at BL and atDCS-induced ΔSICIrest pooled over all time points after stimulation revealed a marginally significant association between event-related SICI modulation and stimulation induced change in resting-state SICI [τ = 0.24, T(29) = 1.86, p = 0.06], indicating more pronounced atDCS-induced SICI effect with better modulatory capacity of event-related SICI. Analyzing age groups separately revealed, that this association was mainly driven by the older group [rpb = 0.53, CI = −0.04 − 0.87, Tpb(14) = 2.36, p = 0.06], while younger did not show an association [rpb = 0.10, CI = −0.43 − 0.69, Tpb(14) = 0.39, p > 0.6, Figure S1A].

Secondary Analysis of Stimulation Effects on Learning Curve in 2FT

Growth curve analysis was used to estimate the influence of STIMULATION CONDITION and AGE GROUP on performance improvement in the 2FT task over time, i.e., learning curve. The overall temporal pattern of 2FT learning curve was curvilinear and best fitted by a second and a third order polynomial [poly[TIME 3], F(1, 18557) = 429.12, p < 0.0001]. Learning curve varied between the two age groups [AGE GROUP × poly[TIME 3], F(3, 18557) = 62.08, p < 0.0001], and was differentially influenced by stimulation condition [STIMULATION CONDITION × poly[TIME 3], F(3, 18557) = 6.02, p < 0.0004]. Most important, the temporal pattern of the learning curve was differentially modulated by stimulation condition among the age groups [AGE GROUP × STIMULATION CONDITION × poly[TIME 3], F(2, 18557) = 6.09, p < 0.0004, Table 7, for Wald statistics on parameter estimates refer to supplemental material]. Parameter estimates indicate that no difference regarding the linear trend was found, i.e., no difference in the linear slope among age groups irrespective of stimulation [triple interaction with linear trend: t(18557) = −0.08, p > 0.9]. However, the temporal pattern of the learning curve varied in terms of concavity [triple interaction with quadratic trend: t(18557) = 3.19, p < 0.01], and change of curvature [triple interaction with cubic trend: t(18557) = 2.76, p < 0.01] showing that older participants showed larger improvements under atDCS early on while younger participants showed increase in learning curve steepness later on (Figure S2A).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. F-test (type III sums of squares) learning curve 2FT.

Correlation with net change in SICImove modulation indicated that steeper learning curves in terms of more pronounced improvement, i.e., reduction of 2FT performance time, to be moderately associated with net reduction of SICImove modulation [τ = 0.32, T(29) = 2.51, p < 0.05]. This association was mainly driven by the younger subgroup [younger: rpb = 0.61, CI = −0.04 − 0.91, p < 0.05, older: rpb = 0.08, CI = −0.53 − 0.76, p > 0.6]. No association was found for net change of SICIrest and learn curve effect size [τ = 0.21, T(29) = 1.63, p = 0.1].

Control Experiment

Using a weaker CS of 70%aMT induced less inhibitory effect than a CS of 80%rMT (Table 8).

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Average SICI in control experiments (conditioned MEP/unconditioned MEP *100, mean ± s.e.m.).

Analyzing the effect of atDCS on ΔSICI measured with different paradigms (setting 1: CS adjusted to 80%rMT, setting 2: CS adjusted to 70%aMT) and stimulation durations (7, 13 min.) over time (BL, P, P45) revealed a significant main effect of SETTING (p < 0.01) with setting 2 inducing on average 6.65% release of inhibition. Overall, STIMULATION DURATION was a significant main effect (p < 0.0005), with longer stimulation duration inducing more release of inhibition in contrast to shorter stimulation duration (Tables 9, 10).

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Wald statistics control experiment (DV: ΔSICI).

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. F-test (type III sums of squares) control experiment (DV: ΔSICI).

A significant SETTING × TIME POINT interaction (p < 0.05) showed that the stimulation induced release of inhibition with setting 2 as well as increase in inhibition with setting 1 weakened over time, which was confirmed by post-hoc testing (time point P, setting 1 vs. setting 2: 11.47 ± 3.39, z = 3.38, p < 0.005; time point P45, setting 1 vs. setting 2: 1.65 ± 3.40, z = 0.49, p > 0.9), i.e., no difference between SICI measured with setting 1 or setting 2 was observable at 45 min after stimulation cessation. Time after stimulation cessation did not modulate the effect of duration of stimulation (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Control experiment testing effect of different settings with CS intensity variation upper graph: 70%aMT (setting 2), bottom graph: 80%rMT (setting 1) on atDCS effect of 7 min (gray) and 13 min (black) stimulation duration over time. CS intensity significantly modulated direction of atDCS effect over time. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, °p < 0.1.

Discussion

In the present data five main results were found: firstly and most strikingly, atDCS led to differential effects on resting-state inhibition (SICIrest) in younger and older participants. While a relative release of inhibition was found in the young, the opposite, a relative increase in inhibition was found in the older. Despite its direction, the atDCS effect also tended to be distinct among age groups with regard to extent and timing. The relative increase in inhibition in the older was evident immediately after stimulation cessation and remained at this level until 90 min following stimulation offset. In the younger however, level of resting-state inhibition did not change significantly from baseline until 90 min after stimulation cessation.

Secondly, stimulation-induced change in event-related modulation of inhibition (SICImove) was merely observable in the older who typically demonstrate lost capacity to release inhibition during movement preparation. This was shown by a tendency toward increased modulatory capacity after atDCS in the older subgroup.

Thirdly, initial modulatory capacity, i.e., modulation of event-related inhibition prior to stimulation, was a predictor for the direction and magnitude of the atDCS-induced effects in resting-state inhibition. Specifically older participants with more pronounced modulation of event-related inhibition, i.e., preserved (“young”) modulatory capacity, showed larger atDCS impact in terms of a relative release of resting-state inhibition resembling the disinhibitory effect observed in the younger.

Fourthly, with regard to the behavioral findings, dexterous manual performance was in general positively influenced by atDCS. In accordance with previous findings, the extent of this effect varied with the nature of the task performed (task-specificity) and the beneficial effect was target-group specific, i.e., it was mainly seen in the older. And finally, in terms of associations between net changes in physiology and behavior, a more incongruent picture was found. For 1FT and 2FT, in general less pronounced net resting-state inhibition was associated with comparatively faster performance. In contrast, net release of resting-state inhibition tended to correlate with performance decline in CRT.

Increase of Event-Related SICI Modulation in the Older

In accordance with previous findings (Heise et al., 2013), fast and precise event-related modulation of SICI in terms of a rapid release of inhibition usually observed before movement onset (Reynolds and Ashby, 1999; Gilio et al., 2003; Sinclair and Hammond, 2008), was weakened or even absent in older participants.

Strikingly, atDCS had no effect on the modulation of event-related inhibition in the younger. But event-related modulation tended to increase after atDCS in the older participants. No association however, was found between stimulation-induced changes in resting-state and event-related inhibition. On the one hand, event-related modulation of inhibition (SICImove) might reflect at least in parts different processes than resting-state measurements of inhibition (SICIrest). On the other hand, event-related modulation of intracortical inhibition in terms of a rapid release of inhibition toward movement onset seems to follow a fairly hard-coded process as long as the underlying mechanisms are intact. Although atDCS weakened resting-state inhibition levels in the younger, event-related release of inhibition was not compromised. In the older age group on the contrary, event-related modulation tended to increase with atDCS. This finding clearly contrasts the initial expectation regarding the atDCS effect, based on the assumption that event-related SICI modulation depends on resting-state levels of inhibition (Heise et al., 2013). It might rather be that initial event-related modulation indicates and predicts overall modulatory capacity of the motor system, and hence the extent to which perturbation is generally possible.

These findings stress the hypothesis that atDCS effects are highly dependent on the state of the cortical network stimulated, as implied by findings of a differentiation of effects among tasks (Boggio et al., 2010; Hummel et al., 2010; Bullard et al., 2011; Leite et al., 2011, 2013) or target groups (Hummel et al., 2010; Tseng et al., 2012; Zimerman et al., 2013) or phase within plastic changes as for example in learning and memory consolidation (Dockery et al., 2009; Kantak et al., 2012; Saucedo Marquez et al., 2013).

Direction and Latency of the atDCS Effect on Resting-State SICI

So far, the available data in younger (Nitsche et al., 2004, 2005; Kidgell et al., 2013) and one study in older participants (Goodwill et al., 2013) reported a disinhibitory effect (release of inhibition) after a single session of atDCS when applied during resting-state. The present observation of a relative increase of inhibition in the older subgroup is therefore somewhat surprising and in disagreement with existing theories about the mode of action of atDCS at first sight. On the basis of experiments using dpTMS or neurotransmitter-specific MRS, a potential involvement of GABAergic mechanisms has been suggested for atDCS effects, leading to a weakened influence of GABAergic inhibition in young healthy volunteers (Nitsche et al., 2004, 2005; Stagg et al., 2009, 2011b). Based on pharmacological evidence and electrophysiological observation of the temporal pattern, SICI at 3 ms ISI is assumed to represent phasic inhibitory mechanisms, which are synaptically mediated at the GABAA receptor (Davies et al., 1990; Di Lazzaro et al., 1998, 2006; Ilic et al., 2002) but also with certain influence of GABAB-ergic activity (Ziemann et al., 1996a; Werhahn et al., 1999; Sanger et al., 2001). Assuming an imbalance of excitatory and inhibitory neuronal activity in the older, findings of weakened short-latency intracortical facilitation (SICF, measured with ISIs of 2.5 ms) in older individuals (Clark et al., 2011) might explain the lack of disinhibition after atDCS observed in the present older group. In consideration of a potential excitatory influence of atDCS on neuronal populations mediating SICF, this effect may be diminished in the older. On the other hand, in vitro experiments and first pharmacological studies suggest a calcium-dependence of atDCS after-effects, i.e., atDCS have been shown to increase intracellular Ca2+ levels (Nitsche et al., 2003a; Khatib et al., 2004; Dube et al., 2012). In a recent study, a conversion of the after-effect of atDCS on corticospinal excitability was found with prolonged stimulation, which was not observed under the influence of a calcium channel blocker (flunarizine) (Monte-Silva et al., 2013). The question is, how atDCS influences a system, which potentially presents with already elevated intracellular Ca2+ levels as observed during the process of healthy aging (Thibault et al., 2001; Murphy et al., 2004). The “calcium-dysregulation hypothesis of brain aging” has been formulated in the late 1980s and has been supported since then, e.g., by calcium imaging of hippocampal neurons (Gibson and Peterson, 1987; Landfield, 1987; Disterhoft et al., 1994; Mattson, 2007). In line with this hypothesis, very recent work has shown increased intracellular calcium buffer in aged rat hippocampal neurons, which was interpreted to represent a focused homeostatic mechanism counteracting elevated intracellular calcium levels (Oh et al., 2013). Therefore, the observed increase of intracortical inhibition in the present older subgroup might indicate a rebound mechanism to prevent over-excitation (Monte-Silva et al., 2013).

Although the expected disinhibitory effect was found in the younger subgroup indeed, the timing of the observed effect clearly contrasts that of the published findings since release of inhibition was not observable immediately after stimulation cessation. To evaluate whether methodological differences might explain this differences, a control experiment was conducted. This analysis revealed a significant interaction of the dpTMS paradigm with the time after stimulation cessation in terms of direction and temporal pattern of SICI induction. This means lower CS intensity resulted in more pronounced release of inhibition (disinhibition) earlier after stimulation.

The clear and immediate disinhibition after atDCS observed in earlier work might well be attributed to excitability shifts within the excitatory interneuronal population rather than modulation of GABAergic inhibitory interneuronal activity. Kujirai's original work suggested most pronounced inhibition with CS intensities of 70%rMT or 90%aMT, above which less SICI induction was explained by possible facilitatory processes superimposing inhibition (Kujirai et al., 1993). Extensive analyzes of the effects of stimulus intensity variations and their interactions with interstimulus intervals have shown less induction of inhibition at SICI protocols using around 3 ms ISI and intensities as low as 70%aMT (Peurala et al., 2008), representing the parameters tested in setting 2 in the present control experiment. However, these data have shown stable inhibition at 90%aMT approximately corresponding to 80%rMT (Chen et al., 1998) as used in the present work. In line with these findings, average baseline SICI of about 40% (i.e., 60% reduction) was found in the present younger subgroup. This resembles and even exceeds 50–80% SICI (i.e., 20–50% reduction) observed in experiments using CS intensities of 70%aMT (Nitsche et al., 2004, 2005; Kidgell et al., 2013). SICI (at 3 ms ISI) measured with lower CS intensities of around 70%aMT has been suggested to represent higher contamination with facilitatory effects, such as SICF (Peurala et al., 2008), which is thought to reflect the activity of excitatory interneuronal interaction contributing to the I-wave generation (Hanajima et al., 2002; Ilic et al., 2002).

Future work needs to examine in detail the potential influence of transcranial direct current stimulation on the recruitment of SICI in participants of older age to advance the understanding of the underlying mechanisms.

Differential Effects on Behavior

Previous data has shown improvements of manual motor performance in terms of speed (Boggio et al., 2006; Vines et al., 2006, 2008; Hummel et al., 2010), endurance of muscle force (Cogiamanian et al., 2007), or precision (Matsuo et al., 2011) after atDCS over MI contralateral to the active hand. Here, the effect of atDCS on motor behavior differed among age groups in extent and timing and was dependent on the character of the task performed. While 1FT performance generally declined with continuing tapping over time, performance levels remained stable in the older group after atDCS. Also in CRT, atDCS was only beneficial for the performance in the older group and did not impact on the younger group's performance. In contrast, the complex dexterous coordination of alternating tapping of two different effectors tested with 2FT clearly improved in the atDCS condition compared to sham. While the older group showed a clear performance benefit under atDCS, performance of the younger seemed not to be influenced by the stimulation. Previous data however, support the notion that the atDCS effect is less pronounced when the motor system is optimally tuned, e.g., in the dominant in contrast to the non-dominant hand (Boggio et al., 2006; Vines et al., 2008) or in younger in contrast to older participants (Hummel et al., 2010). It might well be, that atDCS-induced performance gains would have been more pronounced in the younger group if a more challenging task would have been used or the non-dominant hand/hemisphere had been the target area in the present experiments.

Furthermore, a growing body of data shows the specificity of atDCS effects with respect to the task performed for a variety of cognitive and motor domains (Boggio et al., 2010; Hummel et al., 2010; Bullard et al., 2011; Leite et al., 2011, 2013; Meinzer et al., 2012). The present finding of performance increases under atDCS in the more complex tasks is in accordance with previous results showing a distinction of stimulation effects with respect to the nature of the respective task performed. Greater improvements were found in tasks requiring a higher level of dexterity compared to more gross motor function tasks (Hummel et al., 2010). Only few previous studies have analyzed the potential association between atDCS-related changes in parameters of corticospinal excitability and behavioral changes. These studies imply a causal relationship between better performance and increased levels of excitability (Hummel et al., 2005; Stagg et al., 2009; Zimerman et al., 2012).

In the current data however, no correlation was found between stimulation-induced changes to SICImove and behavior. The fairly disparate picture of the correlations between stimulation-induced changes in SICIrest and change in 2FT or CRT performance makes the interpretation difficult, hence speaking against a simple linear relationship of changes in GABAA-mediated motorcortical inhibition with performance changes as measured in the present experiments.

In the case of 1FT performance, slowing of isolated finger tapping has been described before and suggested to represent changes in motor control rather than muscle fatigue, i.e., indicating the transition from an “alternating flexion/extension muscle pattern to a less effective co-contraction pattern” with continuing performance (Rodrigues et al., 2009). One explanation for the observed stability in the performance level of 1FT under atDCS in the older subgroup might be the preserved selectivity of motor control with increased intracortical inhibition, which might have been hampered by elevated disinhibition as seen in the younger.

Like Goodwill and colleagues the present work also exposed improvements in the sham condition for the most difficult 2FT task, hence skill acquisition after the initial familiarization phase has to be assumed. The secondary analysis of the improvement in the 2FT task revealed an overall improved skill level, i.e., learning curve (linear slope), under atDCS compared to the sham condition in both age groups. Therefore, it is conceivable that atDCS modulates the within session 2FT learning curve (quadratic and cubic trend) leading to faster improvements early in the practice session in the older age subgroup, while later practice gains were boosted in the younger subgroup. In the older, large performance changes in the beginning of the experiment could indicate an extended time span needed to familiarize with the task or the timing-specific influence of atDCS effects early in motor practice. In younger individuals, performance might have reached already a more stable level, or ceiling, early on. As for the late performance gains in the younger, a possible explanation could be first off-line effects of consolidation after the 45 min intersection between test blocks—an effect, shown to be compromised in older individuals (Brown et al., 2009; Zimerman et al., 2013).

A moderate association between steeper learning curves and the reduction of modulatory capacity was observed in the younger under atDCS. On a speculative note, one might assume a plasticity-inducing process due to skill acquisition during repeated 2FT performance, which has been shown to coincide with a reduction of GABAergic motorcortical inhibition (Bütefisch et al., 2000; Ziemann et al., 2001; Floyer-Lea et al., 2006; Stagg et al., 2011a), adding to the disinhibitory effect of atDCS. Therefore, this might be interpreted as an indicator of immediate physiological homeostatic mechanisms, which limit the induction of plasticity to stabilize the excitation/inhibition balance and maintain network integrity.

Limitations

Inter-individual variability of (physiological and behavioral) effects of tDCS is a frequently reported observation (Datta et al., 2012; Saucedo Marquez et al., 2013; Wiethoff et al., 2014) and was also seen in the present study. Recent work using threshold-tracking techniques to quantify SICI proposed differential effects of tDCS on early and late components of SICI (Cengiz et al., 2013), i.e., reduction of late SICI and enhancement of early SICI. The authors furthermore suggested individual variability in the exact timing of early and late components and some degree of overlapping rather than fixed intervals. It might therefore be, that differences in the current findings are related to more pronounced influence of these different elements of SICI. As seen in the younger sample who participated in the control experiment, it is conceivable, that methodological adjustments of direct current stimulation and SICI measurement protocols impact differently in the older.

Recent results do not unambiguously support the notion that the effect of tDCS might partly be ascribed to the effect of simultaneous stimulation of motor association cortices covered by large electrodes and their respective input to MI (Kirimoto et al., 2011; Lindenberg et al., 2013). Utilizing a small electrode surface of the active electrode (3.5 m2 active electrode, 35 m2 reference) to focus current flow to respective intrinsic hand muscle representation, Boros and colleagues did not observe any effect of atDCS over MI on SICI in their control experiment while they observed a significant reduction of inhibition with atDCS to the premotor cortex (Boros et al., 2008). The authors argue that previous findings of their group of SICI reduction after MI stimulation with large electrodes could be attributed to the effect of simultaneous premotor stimulation covered by 35 m2 electrodes. Nonetheless, with an electrode size of 25 m2 centered at the FDI representation in MI as used in the present experiments, parallel stimulation of adjacent premotor areas cannot be completely ruled out. It might also be of relevance that electrical field strength is not necessarily highest in the cortical regions directly underlying the electrodes, as estimated from MR-derived computational head models (Miranda et al., 2013). It cannot be excluded that performance changes are to a lesser extent related to intracortical mechanisms than to stimulation-induced changes within a broader network. Recent analyzes of uni- and bilateral tDCS with resting-state fMRI have provided evidence for changes in intra- and interhemispheric functional connectivity within primary and secondary motor areas (Lindenberg et al., 2013; Sehm et al., 2013).

It has been suggested that muscle fatigue as well as cognitive strain during the stimulation might interfere with and diminish the effect of atDCS in contrast to stimulation applied in the absence of any motor or cognitive activity (Antal et al., 2007; Thirugnanasambandam et al., 2011). Different from these earlier results in the present experiment, no isometric force production but phasic muscle function was required. Moreover, MEP amplitudes were not observed to decline with task performance, which has been proposed to occur during muscular fatigue (Brasil-Neto et al., 1993; Samii et al., 1996; Sacco et al., 2000; Zijdewind et al., 2000). Nonetheless, it is possible that the cognitive load during the tasks was higher for the older individuals and hence the effect of atDCS might have been comparatively reduced in the older. It cannot be excluded on the other hand, that older individuals were more affected by attention decline or fatigue at the latest time point, although not indicated by self-reported levels on the visual analog scale (supplemental online results).

Conclusion

Taken together, these findings show that behavioral and physiological effects are age-group dependent with regard to direction, extent, and timing. Initial modulatory capacity potentially serves as a predictor for the responsiveness to an intervention with atDCS. This means that in older the physiologically “younger” the neuronal network in terms of modulatory capacity the more likely does atDCS lead to a transient disinhibitory effect and respective behavioral improvement. These findings strengthen the hypothesis that the underlying mechanisms are dependent on the specific functional state of the motor-cortical network stimulated and hence may lead to disparate effects of stimulation.

It has to be assumed that the relationship between stimulation-induced changes to the GABAAergic system and dexterous manual behavior is not linear but rather influenced by other systems, such as glutamatergic and NMDA-mediated mechanisms. Future work needs to further explore the physiologic mechanism underlying tDCS-induced changes to behavior in order to specifically tailor the application of tDCS to the requirements of the target-groups.

Author Contributions

Kirstin-Friederike Heise conceptualized the study, acquired and analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript, Martina Niehoff helped with data acquisition and data management, J.-F. Feldheim helped with data management and analysis, Gianpiero Liuzzi helped with conceptualization of the initial study; Christian Gerloff and Friedhelm C. Hummel conceptualized and supervised the study, were involved in data interpretation and writing of the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final submission.

Funding

This research was supported by the University of Hamburg (NWF-10/04 to Kirstin-Friederike Heise) and the German Research Foundation (SFB 936-C4 to Friedhelm C. Hummel and SFB 936-C1 to Christian Gerloff).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnagi.2014.00146/abstract

References

Antal, A., Nitsche, M. A., Kincses, T. Z., Kruse, W., Hoffmann, K. P., and Paulus, W. (2004a). Facilitation of visuo-motor learning by transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor and extrastriate visual areas in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 2888–2892. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03367.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Antal, A., Nitsche, M. A., Kruse, W., Kincses, T. Z., Hoffmann, K. P., and Paulus, W. (2004b). Direct current stimulation over V5 enhances visuomotor coordination by improving motion perception in humans. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 521–527. doi: 10.1162/089892904323057263

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Antal, A., Terney, D., Poreisz, C., and Paulus, W. (2007). Towards unravelling task-related modulations of neuroplastic changes induced in the human motor cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 2687–2691. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05896.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Baayen, R. H., and Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 3, 12–28.

Beck, S., Richardson, S. P., Shamim, E. A., Dang, N., Schubert, M., and Hallett, M. (2008). Short intracortical and surround inhibition are selectively reduced during movement initiation in focal hand dystonia. J. Neurosci. 28, 10363–10369. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3564-08.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Campanha, C., Valasek, C. A., Fecteau, S., Pascual-Leone, A., and Fregni, F. (2010). Modulation of decision-making in a gambling task in older adults with transcranial direct current stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 31, 593–597. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07080.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boggio, P. S., Castro, L. O., Savagim, E. A., Braite, R., Cruz, V. C., Rocha, R. R., et al. (2006). Enhancement of non-dominant hand motor function by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation. Neurosci. Lett. 404, 232–236. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.05.051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boros, K., Poreisz, C., Munchau, A., Paulus, W., and Nitsche, M. A. (2008). Premotor transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) affects primary motor excitability in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 1292–1300. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06090.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brasil-Neto, J. P., Pascual-Leone, A., Valls-Sole, J., Cammarota, A., Cohen, L. G., and Hallett, M. (1993). Postexercise depression of motor evoked potentials: a measure of central nervous system fatigue. Exp. Brain Res. 93, 181–184. doi: 10.1007/BF00227794

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brown, H., and Prescott, R. (2006). Applied Mixed Models in Medicine. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.

Brown, R. M., Robertson, E. M., and Press, D. Z. (2009). Sequence skill acquisition and off-line learning in normal aging. PLoS ONE 4:e6683. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006683

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bullard, L. M., Browning, E. S., Clark, V. P., Coffman, B. A., Garcia, C. M., Jung, R. E., et al. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation's effect on novice versus experienced learning. Exp. Brain Res. 213, 9–14. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2764-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bütefisch, C. M., Davis, B. C., Wise, S. P., Sawaki, L., Kopylev, L., Classen, J., et al. (2000). Mechanisms of use-dependent plasticity in the human motor cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 3661–3665. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.7.3661

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cengiz, B., Murase, N., and Rothwell, J. C. (2013). Opposite effects of weak transcranial direct current stimulation on different phases of short interval intracortical inhibition (SICI). Exp. Brain Res. 225, 321–331. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3369-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chen, R., Tam, A., Butefisch, C., Corwell, B., Ziemann, U., Rothwell, J. C., et al. (1998). Intracortical inhibition and facilitation in different representations of the human motor cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 2870–2881.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Clark, J., Loftus, A., and Hammond, G. (2011). Age-related changes in short-interval intracortical facilitation and dexterity. Neuroreport 22, 499–503. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283487480

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cogiamanian, F., Marceglia, S., Ardolino, G., Barbieri, S., and Priori, A. (2007). Improved isometric force endurance after transcranial direct current stimulation over the human motor cortical areas. Eur. J. Neurosci. 26, 242–249. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05633.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Datta, A., Truong, D., Minhas, P., Parra, L. C., and Bikson, M. (2012). Inter-individual variation during transcranial direct current stimulation and normalization of dose using MRI-derived computational models. Front. Psychiatry 3:91. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00091

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Davies, C. H., Davies, S. N., and Collingridge, G. L. (1990). Paired-pulse depression of monosynaptic GABA-mediated inhibitory postsynaptic responses in rat hippocampus. J. Physiol. 424, 513–531.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

DíAz-Uriarte, R. (2002). Incorrect analysis of crossover trials in animal behaviour research. Anim. Behav. 63, 815–822. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1950

CrossRef Full Text

Di Lazzaro, V., Pilato, F., Dileone, M., Ranieri, F., Ricci, V., Profice, P., et al. (2006). GABAA receptor subtype specific enhancement of inhibition in human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 575, 721–726. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.114694

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Di Lazzaro, V., Restuccia, D., Oliviero, A., Profice, P., Ferrara, L., Insola, A., et al. (1998). Magnetic transcranial stimulation at intensities below active motor threshold activates intracortical inhibitory circuits. Exp. Brain Res. 119, 265–268. doi: 10.1007/s002210050341

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Disterhoft, J. F., Moyer, J. R. Jr., and Thompson, L. T. (1994). The calcium rationale in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Evidence from an animal model of normal aging. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 747, 382–406. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1994.tb44424.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dockery, C. A., Hueckel-Weng, R., Birbaumer, N., and Plewnia, C. (2009). Enhancement of planning ability by transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Neurosci. 29, 7271–7277. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0065-09.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dube, J., Rochette-Drouin, O., Levesque, P., Gauvin, R., Roberge, C. J., Auger, F. A., et al. (2012). Human keratinocytes respond to direct current stimulation by increasing intracellular calcium: preferential response of poorly differentiated cells. J. Cell. Physiol. 227, 2660–2667. doi: 10.1002/jcp.23008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Edwards, J. D., Meehan, S. K., Linsdell, M. A., Borich, M. R., Anbarani, K., Jones, P. W., et al. (2013). Changes in thresholds for intracortical excitability in chronic stroke: more than just altered intracortical inhibition. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 31, 693–705. doi: 10.3233/RNN-120300

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Floyer-Lea, A., Wylezinska, M., Kincses, T., and Matthews, P. M. (2006). Rapid modulation of GABA concentration in human sensorimotor cortex during motor learning. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 1639–1644. doi: 10.1152/jn.00346.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state.” A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., Nitsche, M., Bermpohl, F., Antal, A., Feredoes, E., et al. (2005). Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Exp. Brain Res. 166, 23–30. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2334-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fujiyama, H., Garry, M. I., Levin, O., Swinnen, S. P., and Summers, J. J. (2009). Age-related differences in inhibitory processes during interlimb coordination. Brain Res. 1262, 38–47. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.01.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gaetz, W., Edgar, J. C., Wang, D. J., and Roberts, T. P. (2011). Relating MEG measured motor cortical oscillations to resting gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) concentration. Neuroimage 55, 616–621. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.077

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Galea, J. M., and Celnik, P. (2009). Brain polarization enhances the formation and retention of motor memories. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 294–301. doi: 10.1152/jn.00184.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gandiga, P. C., Hummel, F. C., and Cohen, L. G. (2006). Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 845–850. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gibson, G. E., and Peterson, C. (1987). Calcium and the aging nervous system. Neurobiol. Aging 8, 329–343. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(87)90072-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gilio, F., Curra, A., Inghilleri, M., Lorenzano, C., Suppa, A., Manfredi, M., et al. (2003). Abnormalities of motor cortex excitability preceding movement in patients with dystonia. Brain 126, 1745–1754. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg188

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gleichmann, M., Chow, V. W., and Mattson, M. P. (2011). Homeostatic disinhibition in the aging brain and Alzheimer's disease. J. Alzheimers. Dis. 24, 15–24. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-101674

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Goodwill, A. M., Reynolds, J., Daly, R. M., and Kidgell, D. J. (2013). Formation of cortical plasticity in older adults following tDCS and motor training. Front. Aging Neurosci. 5:87. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grachev, I. D., and Apkarian, A. V. (2001). Aging alters regional multichemical profile of the human brain: an in vivo 1H-MRS study of young versus middle-aged subjects. J. Neurochem. 76, 582–593. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.00026.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grachev, I. D., Swarnkar, A., Szeverenyi, N. M., Ramachandran, T. S., and Apkarian, A. V. (2001). Aging alters the multichemical networking profile of the human brain: an in vivo (1)H-MRS study of young versus middle-aged subjects. J. Neurochem. 77, 292–303. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.t01-1-00238.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hanajima, R., Ugawa, Y., Terao, Y., Enomoto, H., Shiio, Y., Mochizuki, H., et al. (2002). Mechanisms of intracortical I-wave facilitation elicited with paired-pulse magnetic stimulation in humans. J. Physiol. 538, 253–261. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.013094

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heise, K. F., Steven, B., Liuzzi, G., Thomalla, G., Jonas, M., Muller-Vahl, K., et al. (2010). Altered modulation of intracortical excitability during movement preparation in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Brain 133, 580–590. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp299

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Heise, K. F., Zimerman, M., Hoppe, J., Gerloff, C., Wegscheider, K., and Hummel, F. C. (2013). The aging motor system as a model for plastic changes of GABA-mediated intracortical inhibition and their behavioral relevance. J. Neurosci. 33, 9039–9049. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4094-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Honaga, K., Fujiwara, T., Tsuji, T., Hase, K., Ushiba, J., and Liu, M. (2013). State of intracortical inhibitory interneuron activity in patients with chronic stroke. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 364–370. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.08.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hortobagyi, T., Del Olmo, M. F., and Rothwell, J. C. (2006). Age reduces cortical reciprocal inhibition in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 171, 322–329. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-0274-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., and Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biom. J. 50, 346–363. doi: 10.1002/bimj.200810425

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hummel, F., Celnik, P., Giraux, P., Floel, A., Wu, W. H., Gerloff, C., et al. (2005). Effects of non-invasive cortical stimulation on skilled motor function in chronic stroke. Brain 128, 490–499. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh369

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hummel, F. C., Heise, K., Celnik, P., Floel, A., Gerloff, C., and Cohen, L. G. (2010). Facilitating skilled right hand motor function in older subjects by anodal polarization over the left primary motor cortex. Neurobiol. Aging 31, 2160–2168. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.12.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hummel, F. C., Steven, B., Hoppe, J., Heise, K., Thomalla, G., Cohen, L. G., et al. (2009). Deficient intracortical inhibition (SICI) during movement preparation after chronic stroke. Neurology 72, 1766–1772. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a609c5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hunter, T., Sacco, P., Nitsche, M. A., and Turner, D. L. (2009). Modulation of internal model formation during force field-induced motor learning by anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of primary motor cortex. J. Physiol. 587, 2949–2961. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.169284

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ilic, T. V., Meintzschel, F., Cleff, U., Ruge, D., Kessler, K. R., and Ziemann, U. (2002). Short-interval paired-pulse inhibition and facilitation of human motor cortex: the dimension of stimulus intensity. J. Physiol. 545, 153–167. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.030122

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jackson, S. R., Parkinson, A., Manfredi, V., Millon, G., Hollis, C., and Jackson, G. M. (2013). Motor excitability is reduced prior to voluntary movements in children and adolescents with Tourette syndrome. J. Neuropsychol. 7, 29–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-6653.2012.02033.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kantak, S. S., Mummidisetty, C. K., and Stinear, J. W. (2012). Primary motor and premotor cortex in implicit sequence learning–evidence for competition between implicit and explicit human motor memory systems. Eur. J. Neurosci. 36, 2710–2715. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08175.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Khatib, L., Golan, D. E., and Cho, M. (2004). Physiologic electrical stimulation provokes intracellular calcium increase mediated by phospholipase C activation in human osteoblasts. FASEB J. 18, 1903–1905. doi: 10.1096/fj.04-1814fje

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kidgell, D. J., Daly, R. M., Young, K., Lum, J., Tooley, G., Jaberzadeh, S., et al. (2013). Different current intensities of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation do not differentially modulate motor cortex plasticity. Neural Plast. 2013:603502. doi: 10.1155/2013/603502

CrossRef Full Text

Kim, S. (2012). ppcor: Partial and Semi-partial (Part) Correlation. R package, 1.0 Edn.

Kirimoto, H., Ogata, K., Onishi, H., Oyama, M., Goto, Y., and Tobimatsu, S. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation over the motor association cortex induces plastic changes in ipsilateral primary motor and somatosensory cortices. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 777–783. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.09.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kujirai, T., Caramia, M. D., Rothwell, J. C., Day, B. L., Thompson, P. D., Ferbert, A., et al. (1993). Corticocortical inhibition in human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 471, 501–519.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Landfield, P. W. (1987). ‘Increased calcium-current’ hypothesis of brain aging. Neurobiol. Aging 8, 346–347. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(87)90074-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lang, N., Nitsche, M. A., Dileone, M., Mazzone, P., De Andres-Ares, J., Diaz-Jara, L., et al. (2011). Transcranial direct current stimulation effects on I-wave activity in humans. J. Neurophysiol. 105, 2802–2810. doi: 10.1152/jn.00617.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lang, N., Nitsche, M. A., Paulus, W., Rothwell, J. C., and Lemon, R. N. (2004). Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation over the human motor cortex on corticospinal and transcallosal excitability. Exp. Brain Res. 156, 439–443 doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1800-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leite, J., Carvalho, S., Fregni, F., Boggio, P. S., and Goncalves, O. F. (2013). The effects of cross-hemispheric dorsolateral prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on task switching. Brain Stimul. 6, 660–667. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.10.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leite, J., Carvalho, S., Fregni, F., and Goncalves, O. F. (2011). Task-specific effects of tDCS-induced cortical excitability changes on cognitive and motor sequence set shifting performance. PLoS ONE 6:e24140. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024140

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lindenberg, R., Nachtigall, L., Meinzer, M., Sieg, M. M., and Floel, A. (2013). Differential effects of dual and unihemispheric motor cortex stimulation in older adults. J. Neurosci. 33, 9176–9183. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0055-13.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liuzzi, G., Horniss, V., Lechner, P., Hoppe, J., Heise, K., Zimerman, M., et al. (2014). Development of movement-related intracortical inhibition in acute to chronic subcortical stroke. Neurology 82, 198–205. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marneweck, M., Loftus, A., and Hammond, G. (2011). Short-interval intracortical inhibition and manual dexterity in healthy aging. Neurosci. Res. 70, 408–414. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2011.04.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Matsuo, A., Maeoka, H., Hiyamizu, M., Shomoto, K., Morioka, S., and Seki, K. (2011). Enhancement of precise hand movement by transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroreport 22, 78–82. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834298b3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mattson, M. P. (2007). Calcium and neurodegeneration. Aging Cell 6, 337–350. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-9726.2007.00275.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

McGinley, M., Hoffman, R. L., Russ, D. W., Thomas, J. S., and Clark, B. C. (2010). Older adults exhibit more intracortical inhibition and less intracortical facilitation than young adults. Exp. Gerontol. 45, 671–678. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2010.04.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meinzer, M., Antonenko, D., Lindenberg, R., Hetzer, S., Ulm, L., Avirame, K., et al. (2012). Electrical brain stimulation improves cognitive performance by modulating functional connectivity and task-specific activation. J. Neurosci. 32, 1859–1866. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4812-11.2012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miranda, P. C., Mekonnen, A., Salvador, R., and Ruffini, G. (2013). The electric field in the cortex during transcranial current stimulation. Neuroimage 70, 48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.034

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Monte-Silva, K., Kuo, M. F., Hessenthaler, S., Fresnoza, S., Liebetanz, D., Paulus, W., et al. (2013). Induction of late LTP-like plasticity in the human motor cortex by repeated non-invasive brain stimulation. Brain Stimul. 6, 424–432. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.04.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Murphy, G. G., Fedorov, N. B., Giese, K. P., Ohno, M., Friedman, E., Chen, R., et al. (2004). Increased neuronal excitability, synaptic plasticity, and learning in aged Kvbeta1.1 knockout mice. Curr. Biol. 14, 1907–1915. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.10.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 1, 206–223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Fricke, K., Henschke, U., Schlitterlau, A., Liebetanz, D., Lang, N., et al. (2003a). Pharmacological modulation of cortical excitability shifts induced by transcranial direct current stimulation in humans. J. Physiol. 553, 293–301. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.049916

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Lampe, C., Antal, A., Liebetanz, D., Lang, N., Tergau, F., et al. (2006). Dopaminergic modulation of long-lasting direct current-induced cortical excitability changes in the human motor cortex. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 1651–1657. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04676.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Liebetanz, D., Antal, A., Lang, N., Tergau, F., and Paulus, W. (2003b). Modulation of cortical excitability by weak direct current stimulation–technical, safety and functional aspects. Suppl. Clin. Neurophysiol. 56, 255–276. doi: 10.1016/S1567-424X(09)70230-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Liebetanz, D., Schlitterlau, A., Henschke, U., Fricke, K., Frommann, K., et al. (2004). GABAergic modulation of DC stimulation-induced motor cortex excitability shifts in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 2720–2726. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03398.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 527(Pt 3), 633–639. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2001). Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 57, 1899–1901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Schauenburg, A., Lang, N., Liebetanz, D., Exner, C., Paulus, W., et al. (2003c). Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 619–626. doi: 10.1162/089892903321662994

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nitsche, M. A., Seeber, A., Frommann, K., Klein, C. C., Rochford, C., Nitsche, M. S., et al. (2005). Modulating parameters of excitability during and after transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 568, 291–303. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.092429

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oh, M. M., Oliveira, F. A., Waters, J., and Disterhoft, J. F. (2013). Altered calcium metabolism in aging CA1 hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J. Neurosci. 33, 7905–7911. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5457-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peurala, S. H., Muller-Dahlhaus, J. F., Arai, N., and Ziemann, U. (2008). Interference of short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) and short-interval intracortical facilitation (SICF). Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 2291–2297. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.05.031

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., Debroy, S., Sarkar, D., and R Core Team. (2012). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. 3.1-104 Edn.

Pinheiro, J. C., and Bates, D. M. (2009). Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-PLUS. New York, NY: Springer.

Pinto, J. G., Hornby, K. R., Jones, D. G., and Murphy, K. M. (2010). Developmental changes in GABAergic mechanisms in human visual cortex across the lifespan. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 4:16. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2010.00016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reis, J., and Fritsch, B. (2011). Modulation of motor performance and motor learning by transcranial direct current stimulation. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 24, 590–596. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32834c3db0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reis, J., Schambra, H. M., Cohen, L. G., Buch, E. R., Fritsch, B., Zarahn, E., et al. (2009). Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through an effect on consolidation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 1590–1595. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805413106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reynolds, C., and Ashby, P. (1999). Inhibition in the human motor cortex is reduced just before a voluntary contraction. Neurology 53, 730–735. doi: 10.1212/WNL.53.4.730

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rodrigues, J. P., Mastaglia, F. L., and Thickbroom, G. W. (2009). Rapid slowing of maximal finger movement rate: fatigue of central motor control? Exp. Brain Res. 196, 557–563. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1886-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rogasch, N. C., Dartnall, T. J., Cirillo, J., Nordstrom, M. A., and Semmler, J. G. (2009). Corticomotor plasticity and learning of a ballistic thumb training task are diminished in older adults. J. Appl. Physiol. (1985) 107, 1874–1883. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00443.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rossini, P. M., Berardelli, A., Deuschl, G., Hallett, M., Maertens de Noordhout, A. M., Paulus, W., et al. (1999). Applications of magnetic cortical stimulation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl. 52, 171–185.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Rossi, S., Hallett, M., Rossini, P. M., Pascual-Leone, A., and Safety Of, T. M. S. C. G. (2009). Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 2008–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rossiter, H. E., Davis, E. M., Clark, E. V., Boudrias, M. H., and Ward, N. S. (2014). Beta oscillations reflect changes in motor cortex inhibition in healthy ageing. Neuroimage 91, 360–365. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sacco, P., Thickbroom, G. W., Byrnes, M. L., and Mastaglia, F. L. (2000). Changes in corticomotor excitability after fatiguing muscle contractions. Muscle Nerve 23, 1840–1846. doi: 10.1002/1097-4598(200012)23:12<1840::AID-MUS7>3.0.CO;2-H

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Samii, A., Wassermann, E. M., Ikoma, K., Mercuri, B., and Hallett, M. (1996). Characterization of postexercise facilitation and depression of motor evoked potentials to transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurology 46, 1376–1382. doi: 10.1212/WNL.46.5.1376

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sanger, T. D., Garg, R. R., and Chen, R. (2001). Interactions between two different inhibitory systems in the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 530, 307–317. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0307l.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sarkar, D. (2008). Lattice: Multivariate Data Visualization with R. New York, NY: Springer.

Saucedo Marquez, C. M., Zhang, X., Swinnen, S. P., Meesen, R., and Wenderoth, N. (2013). Task-specific effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:333. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00333

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sawaki, L., Yaseen, Z., Kopylev, L., and Cohen, L. G. (2003). Age-dependent changes in the ability to encode a novel elementary motor memory. Ann. Neurol. 53, 521–524. doi: 10.1002/ana.10529

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sehm, B., Kipping, J., Schafer, A., Villringer, A., and Ragert, P. (2013). A comparison between uni- and bilateral tDCS effects on functional connectivity of the human motor cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:183. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00183

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Siebner, H. R., and Rothwell, J. (2003). Transcranial magnetic stimulation: new insights into representational cortical plasticity. Exp. Brain Res. 148, 1–16. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1234-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sinclair, C., and Hammond, G. R. (2008). Reduced intracortical inhibition during the foreperiod of a warned reaction time task. Exp. Brain Res. 186, 385–392. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1241-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Smith, A. E., Ridding, M. C., Higgins, R. D., Wittert, G. A., and Pitcher, J. B. (2009). Age-related changes in short-latency motor cortex inhibition. Exp. Brain Res. 198, 489–500. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1945-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sohn, M. K., Kim, B. O., and Song, H. T. (2012). Effect of stimulation polarity of transcranial direct current stimulation on non-dominant hand function. Ann. Rehabil. Med. 36, 1–7. doi: 10.5535/arm.2012.36.1.1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stagg, C. J., Bachtiar, V., and Johansen-Berg, H. (2011a). The role of GABA in human motor learning. Curr. Biol. 21, 480–484. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.069

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stagg, C. J., Best, J. G., Stephenson, M. C., O'Shea, J., Wylezinska, M., Kincses, Z. T., et al. (2009). Polarity-sensitive modulation of cortical neurotransmitters by transcranial stimulation. J. Neurosci. 29, 5202–5206. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4432-08.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stagg, C. J., Bestmann, S., Constantinescu, A. O., Moreno, L. M., Allman, C., Mekle, R., et al. (2011b). Relationship between physiological measures of excitability and levels of glutamate and GABA in the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 589, 5845–5855. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.216978

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stagg, C. J., Jayaram, G., Pastor, D., Kincses, Z. T., Matthews, P. M., and Johansen-Berg, H. (2011c). Polarity and timing-dependent effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in explicit motor learning. Neuropsychologia 49, 800–804. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Thibault, O., Hadley, R., and Landfield, P. W. (2001). Elevated postsynaptic [Ca2+]i and L-type calcium channel activity in aged hippocampal neurons: relationship to impaired synaptic plasticity. J. Neurosci. 21, 9744–9756.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Thirugnanasambandam, N., Sparing, R., Dafotakis, M., Meister, I. G., Paulus, W., Nitsche, M. A., et al. (2011). Isometric contraction interferes with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) induced plasticity: evidence of state-dependent neuromodulation in human motor cortex. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 29, 311–320. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2011-0601

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tseng, P., Hsu, T. Y., Chang, C. F., Tzeng, O. J., Hung, D. L., Muggleton, N. G., et al. (2012). Unleashing potential: transcranial direct current stimulation over the right posterior parietal cortex improves change detection in low-performing individuals. J. Neurosci. 32, 10554–10561. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0362-12.2012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vines, B. W., Nair, D., and Schlaug, G. (2008). Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. Eur. J. Neurosci. 28, 1667–1673. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06459.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vines, B. W., Nair, D. G., and Schlaug, G. (2006). Contralateral and ipsilateral motor effects after transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroreport 17, 671–674. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200604240-00023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Werhahn, K. J., Kunesch, E., Noachtar, S., Benecke, R., and Classen, J. (1999). Differential effects on motorcortical inhibition induced by blockade of GABA uptake in humans. J. Physiol. 517(Pt 2), 591–597. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0591t.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wickham, H. (2009). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer.

Wiethoff, S., Hamada, M., and Rothwell, J. C. (2014). Variability in response to transcranial direct current stimulation of the motor cortex. Brain Stimul 7, 468–475. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilcox, R. R. (2012). Introduction to Robust Estimation and Hypothesis Testing San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

World Health Organization, W. (2012). “Mortality and global health estimates: life expectancy,” in Global Health Observatory Data Repository. Available online at: http://apps.who.int/gho/data/node.main.688?lang=en

Ziemann, U., Lonnecker, S., Steinhoff, B. J., and Paulus, W. (1996a). Effects of antiepileptic drugs on motor cortex excitability in humans: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Ann. Neurol. 40, 367–378. doi: 10.1002/ana.410400306

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ziemann, U., Muellbacher, W., Hallett, M., and Cohen, L. G. (2001). Modulation of practice-dependent plasticity in human motor cortex. Brain 124, 1171–1181. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.6.1171

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ziemann, U., Rothwell, J. C., and Ridding, M. C. (1996b). Interaction between intracortical inhibition and facilitation in human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 496(Pt 3), 873–881.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Zijdewind, I., Zwarts, M. J., and Kernell, D. (2000). Potentiating and fatiguing cortical reactions in a voluntary fatigue test of a human hand muscle. Exp. Brain Res. 130, 529–532. doi: 10.1007/s002210050055

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zimerman, M., Heise, K. F., Hoppe, J., Cohen, L. G., Gerloff, C., and Hummel, F. C. (2012). Modulation of training by single-session transcranial direct current stimulation to the intact motor cortex enhances motor skill acquisition of the paretic hand. Stroke 43, 2185–2191. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.645382

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zimerman, M., Nitsch, M., Giraux, P., Gerloff, C., Cohen, L. G., and Hummel, F. C. (2013). Neuroenhancement of the aging brain: restoring skill acquisition in old subjects. Ann. Neurol. 73, 10–15. doi: 10.1002/ana.23761

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: aging, anodal tDCS, manual dexterity, short-interval intracortical inhibition, TMS

Citation: Heise K-F, Niehoff M, Feldheim J-F, Liuzzi G, Gerloff C and Hummel FC (2014) Differential behavioral and physiological effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation in healthy adults of younger and older age. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:146. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00146

Received: 21 April 2014; Accepted: 16 June 2014;
Published online: 10 July 2014.

Edited by:

Philip P. Foster, The University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, USA

Reviewed by:

Oury Monchi, University of Montreal, Canada
Masashi Hamada, University College London, UK

Copyright © 2014 Heise, Niehoff, Feldheim, Liuzzi, Gerloff and Hummel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Friedhelm C. Hummel, Department of Neurology, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Martinistr. 52, 20246 Hamburg, Germany e-mail: f.hummel@uke.de

Download