Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 23 December 2016
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

Social Orienting and Attention Is Influenced by the Presence of Competing Nonsocial Information in Adolescents with Autism

\r\nKathryn E. Unruh*Kathryn E. Unruh1*Noah J. SassonNoah J. Sasson2Robin L. ShaferRobin L. Shafer1Allison WhittenAllison Whitten3Stephanie J. MillerStephanie J. Miller4Lauren Turner-Brown,,Lauren Turner-Brown4,5,6James W. Bodfish,James W. Bodfish1,3
  • 1Vanderbilt Neuroscience Graduate Program, Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
  • 2School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, The University of Texas at Dallas, Richardson, TX, USA
  • 3Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
  • 4Sheps Center for Health Services Research, Carolina Institute for Developmental Disabilities, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
  • 5TEACCH Autism Program, UNC School of Medicine, The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Carrboro, NC, USA
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill School of Medicine, Chapel Hill, NC, USA

Background: Our experiences with the world play a critical role in neural and behavioral development. Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) spend a disproportionate amount of time seeking out, attending to, and engaging with aspects of their environment that are largely nonsocial in nature. In this study we adapted an established method for eliciting and quantifying aspects of visual choice behavior related to preference to test the hypothesis that preference for nonsocial sources of stimulation diminishes orientation and attention to social sources of stimulation in children with ASD.

Method: Preferential viewing tasks can serve as objective measures of preference, with a greater proportion of viewing time to one item indicative of increased preference. The current task used gaze-tracking technology to examine patterns of visual orientation and attention to stimulus pairs that varied in social (faces) and nonsocial content (high autism interest or low autism interest). Participants included both adolescents diagnosed with ASD and typically developing; groups were matched on IQ and gender.

Results: Repeated measures ANOVA revealed that individuals with ASD had a significantly greater latency to first fixate on social images when this image was paired with a high autism interest image, compared to a low autism interest image pairing. Participants with ASD showed greater total look time to objects, while typically developing participants preferred to look at faces. Groups also differed in number and average duration of fixations to social and object images. In the ASD group only, a measure of nonsocial interest was associated with reduced preference for social images when paired with high autism interest images.

Conclusions: In ASD, the presence of nonsocial sources of stimulation can significantly increase the latency of look time to social sources of information. These results suggest that atypicalities in social motivation in ASD may be context-dependent, with a greater degree of plasticity than is assumed by existing social motivation accounts of ASD.

Introduction

The Social Motivation Theory of Autism posits that autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is the result of an early and extreme lack of motivation toward social information, leading to the development of a social-specific reward deficit (Dawson and Lewy, 1989; Dawson, 1991; Dawson et al., 2005; Chevallier et al., 2012; Kohls et al., 2012) This theory provides an account of the social deficits that comprise ASD. Symptoms that are nonsocial in nature (e.g., restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, or interest) are also core features of ASD; however, the magnitude to which these nonsocial patterns of behavior occur is not accounted for within the framework of the social motivation theory.

A prominent feature of nonsocial symptoms in ASD is restricted, or circumscribed interests (CI). Commonly reported interests of individuals with ASD include vehicles, electronics, dinosaurs, particular animals, numbers, facts, cartoons, solitary games, and mechanical systems (South et al., 2005; Turner-Brown et al., 2011; Anthony et al., 2013) In contrast to the reduced reward processing associated with social motivation deficits in ASD, the excessive interest and fixation associated with CI, suggest a role for increased activity of reward circuitry in ASD. Further, it is possible that this enhanced experience of reward or pleasure associated with CI may bias attention away from social sources of stimulation. One hypothesis that can be considered from this formulation is that a nonsocial attentional bias may contribute to reduced social interest and concomitant social deficits seen in ASD.

Typical patterns of attentional bias can be demonstrated using preferential viewing paradigms. The logic of this paradigm is that when images are paired, the resulting pattern of visual orientation and attention can give insight into the relative preference or reward value of the two stimulus types. Similar paradigms have been used to assess preference across species. For example, macaques show visual preference for their own species over others as young as 2 months of age (Kim et al., 1999). Similarly, human neonates show preference for both realistic and schematic human faces over non-face stimuli (Fantz, 1964; Goren et al., 1975; Johnson et al., 1991; Valenza et al., 1996). Previous studies of ASD have shown that the presence of CI stimuli alters patterns of visual attention. By measuring visual attention within arrays containing social and nonsocial (object) images, Sasson et al. (2008), showed that individuals with ASD explored fewer social images when they were paired with CI-related objects, compared to when social images were paired with neutral (non-CI-related) objects.

The aim of the current study was to assess potential attentional biases in ASD, using a preferential viewing paradigm. Participants passively viewed arrays containing both social and object images; object images were varied between neutral, or “low autism interest” (LAI) images, and images associated with circumscribed interests, or “high autism interest” (HAI) images. We sought to measure both latency of initial choice as well as the distribution of overall preference patterns to social and nonsocial images. This paradigm allowed us to examine whether social orientation and attention could be influenced by the presence of specific nonsocial images (e.g., CI-related objects). The primary hypothesis was that the participants with ASD would demonstrate decreased indices of visual attention to social images (relative preference, latency to first fixation, average duration of fixations), when social images are paired with HAI images but not when social images are paired with LAI images.

Methods

Participants

Two groups of adolescents participated in this study: 48 with ASD (41 males, 7 females, mean age = 167.39 months, range = 116–218 months) and 39 who were typically developing (TYP; 34 males, 5 females; mean age = 165.83 range = 111–227 months). This sample includes a significantly greater number of males than females due to the disproportionate ratio of males to females with ASD. All participants met the following general inclusion criteria: Age between 9 and 18 years; intelligence quotient (IQ) greater than 70; absence of seizure disorder, acute medical, or genetic condition; and absence of any visual impairment uncorrectable with eyeglasses.

Participants with ASD were recruited through an autism research registry in conjunction with regional assessment and treatment clinical service programs for persons with ASD. Inclusion of the registry required a previous Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV diagnosis of ASD made by a licensed clinician experienced in the assessment and diagnosis of autism, and based on parent interview and direct observation for the completion standardized autism diagnostic assessment instruments (Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised; ADI-R), Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ADOS). Following referral from the registry, all ASD participants were evaluated by trained study personnel using (a) the ADI-R (Lord et al., 1994) to examine lifetime criteria for ASD, (b) the ADOS (Lord et al., 2012), (c) the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) (Constantino and Gruber, 2002) to examine the current severity of autism symptoms, and (d) the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Second Edition (KBIT-2) (Kaufman and Kaufman, 2004) to examine general cognitive ability.

TYP children were recruited via an email sent university faculty and staff. TYP children were excluded if they had a history of psychiatric or developmental disorder, if they were currently taking psychotropic medication, if an immediate family member had an ASD diagnosis, or if they received a score above the ASD cutoff on the SRS. These adolescents were chosen to be matched on gender and chronological age, compared to the ASD group. Groups were matched on gender because previous studies indicate interest in social stimuli and CI-related stimuli can vary between males and females (Sasson et al., 2012).

One TYP participant was excluded for having an SRS score that fell in the ASD range. There was no significant difference between groups for nonverbal IQ [t(62) = 1.60, p = 0.116]. Independent samples t-tests were conducted between groups for each of the psychometric measures and relevant subscales. As expected, ASD participants scored significantly higher than TYP individuals on measures of social-communication and repetitive behavior. See Table 1 for group means and results of statistical analysis. Before participation, all individuals and their legal guardians supplied written informed consent for study participation. The protocol for this study was approved by the Vanderbilt University School of Medicine Biomedical Institutional Review Boards.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics and participant characterization.

Stimuli and Task

Preferential Viewing Task: The preferential viewing task was designed for this study and is comprised of 18 static, high-quality color picture arrays. Each array contained a pair of social and object images (see Figure 1). We chose to use static images to ensure greater experimental control across our stimulus categories, including accounting for category specific motion differences (e.g., biological vs. mechanical motion) as well as low-level salience properties of the stimuli, such as luminance and image complexity. Further, the use of these static images allowed us to include a contrast of low- and high- autism interest images based on previous experimental results.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Sample array and heat maps for ASD and TYP participants. Viewing time differed across participant groups for social and object images. (A) Sample preferential viewing array (SOC + HAI). (B) Aggregated viewing time for TYP participants. (C) Aggregated viewing time for ASD participants. Regions marked in red indicate the greatest amount of viewing time.

The 18 social images were taken with permission from the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Early Experience and Brain Development (Tottenham et al., 2009). Identities of the faces did not repeat, were split evenly between males and females, and consisted of Caucasian, African-American, and Asian-American. Of the 18 object stimuli, half were selected to represent items frequently occurring as topics of CI in ASD (South et al., 2005). In previous work in our lab we validated the reward value of these stimuli using standardized valence and arousal ratings. These stimuli were rated by participants with ASD as significantly higher in valence than control object images (Sasson et al., 2012). We have termed these CI-related stimuli “High Autism Interest” (HAI) objects. Examples of HAI objects include: Trains, vehicles, airplanes, clocks, and blocks. The additional nine objects included control objects, which were not related to CI and which we have found participants with ASD to rate significantly lower in valence (Sasson et al., 2012). We have termed these images “Low Autism Interest” (LAI) objects. Examples of LAI objects include: Clothing, tools, musical instruments, and plants. Each image measured approximately 8 × 10 cm, and images were separated by a gap of approximately 12 cm. Images were also matched for luminance and complexity. Equivalent areas of interest were drawn for social and nonsocial images, and each corresponded to approximately 25% of the total viewing area. Each stimulus array contained one social image paired with one object (either HAI or LAI) image. Positioning (left vs. right) of all stimulus categories was counterbalanced across arrays.

Eye-Tracking

Testing occurred in a research laboratory. Participants sat approximately 60 cm from a 1024 horizontal x 768 vertical 17-inch display and viewed stimuli subtending a visual angle of 16.1 degrees. Eye movements were recorded with a Tobii 1750 eye tracker (Tobii Technology, Stockholm, Sweden). The system uses an infrared light to produce reflection patterns on the corneas of the eye and monitors these reflections relative to the eye's position. This system samples at a rate of 50 Hz. This eye tracking system is mounted on the computer monitor, and therefore does not interfere with data collection. The system allows for head movement within a cubic space of 30 × 15 × 20 cm from a distance of 60 cm, allowing the participants to view in a naturalistic manner. The task was preceded by a 5-point calibration procedure, which was repeated until calibration was sufficient for each of the data points. Prior to the task, the participant was told to view the arrays however he/she wanted. Stimulus arrays were then displayed individually for 5 s each. Prior to each trial, a blank slide with a fixation cross appeared for 5 s to reorient attention and ensure that all scanning patterns began equidistant from each image in the stimulus pair.

Psychometric Measures

Social Responsiveness Scale

The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino and Gruber, 2002) is a parent report questionnaire intended to measure behaviors related to social impairment, including social awareness, social information processing, capacity for reciprocal social communication, and social anxiety/avoidance, in children ages 4–18 years of age. An additional section of the SRS contains questions regarding autistic preoccupations and traits.

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

The ADOS (Lord et al., 2012) is a semi-structured, play-based diagnostic measure of the core features of ASD. In addition to providing a score to measure against diagnostic thresholds, the ADOS now provides scores of ASD severity (Gotham et al., 2008). These scores can be used to compare severity across ages (ADOS modules) in individuals with ASD.

Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised

Previous studies have shown a wide variety of repetitive behaviors occur in autism (Bodfish et al., 2000; Honey et al., 2007; Lam and Aman, 2007). We chose to use the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R; Bodfish et al., 1999; Lam and Aman, 2007) to identify the presence of specific subtypes of repetitive behavior. The RBS-R is an informant rating scale that assesses five categories of repetitive behavior (motor stereotypy, repetitive self-injury, compulsions, routines/sameness, restricted interests). These subscales have high internal consistency, with Cronbach's alpha values ranging from 0.78 (restricted interests) to 0.91 (routines/sameness) (Lam and Aman, 2007).

Interest Scale

The Interest Scale (Turner-Brown et al., 2011) is used to collect detailed information on the presence and severity of circumscribed interests. This scale contains a checklist of interests, for which parents indicate if these are currently or have ever been an interest of their child; these are summed separately to indicate the number of past interests and number of current interests the child has endorsed. Additional questions characterize the child's strongest interest, including the degree to which this interest is shared with other people (social involvement), and the flexibility, frequency, intensity, interference, and accommodation of that specific interest, which are combined to produce a total severity score (range 0–23; higher score indicates greater severity).

Analysis of Task Performance

The nature of the paired preference task requires that each participant is looking at the slide for a sufficient amount of time to observe both images; this differs from other tasks in which total look time can more effectively control for differences in total viewing time. Therefore, we developed a method to exclude participants based on insufficient total look time per slide, as to eliminate potential bias from the data. To calculate exclusion criteria, each participant was judged based on how many slides they viewed for less than 2.5 s (half the total time each stimulus was presented). Each participant who scored higher than 10 was excluded from analyses. Applying these criteria resulted in exclusion of 15 participants with ASD and 7 TYP participants. This proportion of excluded participants is comparable to other studies that do report exclusion based on eye-tracking data in a comparable age group (e.g., Sterling et al., 2008; Chevallier et al., 2015). Analysis revealed that the excluded group did not differ from the included group on age [t(84) = 1.24, p = 0.217] or nonverbal IQ [t(82) = 0.507, p = 0.613]; however, the excluded group contained significantly more females than those included in final analyses.

Eye-Tracking Variables

Eye-tracking data was analyzed to look at a variety of gaze components. These variables were averaged across social images and object images, within array types, resulting in four dependent variable categories for each eye-tracking variable: SOC + LAI: Social, SOC + HAI: Social, SOC + LAI: Object, and SOC + HAI: Object. Eye tracking patterns were analyzed as a result of conducting fixation analyses. Fixations were classified using the Tobii Studio I-VT filter, which defines fixations as gaze moving at a velocity slower than 30° per second, for at least 60 ms. We extracted four dependent variables from the data collected: (a) Preference: the proportion of on screen fixation time devoted to each image type, relative to total time spent on the stimulus array; (b) Detail orientation: The average number of discrete fixations the participant makes on each stimulus type, relative to total time on the image, across arrays; (c) Fixation duration: The average length of fixations to each image type, across arrays; and (d) Prioritization: The latency to first fixate on each stimulus type, which measures attention capture and orienting.

Statistical Analysis

Repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) was conducted on each of the primary variables, with object type (LAI or HAI) as the within-subjects variable and group (TYP, ASD) as the between groups variable. Separate RM-ANOVA analyses were conducted for variables pertaining to social attention and object attention. Sphericity was not assumed between groups; therefore, values for main effects are reported using the Greenhouse-Geisser correction. A significant interaction for any of the dependent variables would suggest that one object type disproportionately influences attention, compared to the other. All significant interactions were followed up with post-hoc analyses to identify the direction of the effect. When two or more post-hoc comparisons were performed within a measure, the significance levels of these orthogonal comparisons was corrected using the Bonferroni method.

Bivariate (Pearson's r) correlations were used to assess relationships between eye-tracking and psychometric data. For these analyses, each variable was log-transformed to account for skewness in the distributions and to improve interpretability. Each variable was transformed by a factor of log(x+1) to preserve data points equal to zero, which were meaningful in this ratio data set. Significance of correlation analyses was assessed using Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons.

Results

Figure 1 illustrates the differences in relative look time between the two groups; red indicates more time spent looking to the region, while yellow indicates less looking. Aggregated viewing time of the TYP group indicates more time spent looking to social images (Figure 1B), while aggregated viewing time of the ASD group indicates greater looking to the object images (Figure 1C).

Group Differences: Eye-Tracking Variables

Preference–Social: A 2x2 (Group: ASD, TYP; Array: SOC + LAI, SOC + HAI) Repeated measures (RM)-ANOVA was conducted for social preference. There was no group x array interaction (p = 0.184). There was a main effect of array [F(1, 62) = 25.32, p < 0.0001]. There was a main effect of group [F(1, 62) = 21.14, p < 0.0001]. Main effect results indicate both groups showed greater preference for faces in SOC + LAI arrays, compared to SOC + HAI arrays. Additionally, the TYP group showed greater total fixation time for faces than the ASD group in both array types. Figure 2A illustrates group differences for social preference.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean (+/– standard error) proportion of total look time for social and object images in ASD and TYP participants. (A) Proportion of look time to social images and (B) proportion of look time to object images. ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; TYP, typically developing; SOC, social; LAI, low autism interest; HAI, high autism interest. **p < 0.01.

Preference–Object: A 2x2 (Group: ASD, TYP; Array: SOC + LAI, SOC + HAI) RM-ANOVA was conducted for object preference. There was no group x array interaction (p = 0.164). There was a main effect of array [F(1, 62) = 34.90, p < 0.0001]. There was a main effect of group [F(1, 62) = 7.95, p < 0.01]. Main effect results indicate both groups showed greater preference for objects in SOC + HAI arrays, compared to SOC + LAI arrays. Additionally, the ASD group showed greater total fixation time for objects than the TYP group in both array types. Figure 2B illustrates group differences for object preference.

Detail Orientation–Social: A 2x2 (Group: ASD, TYP; Array: SOC + LAI, SOC + HAI) RM-ANOVA was conducted for the number of fixations on social images. There was no group x array interaction (p = 0.30). There was a main effect of array [F(1, 62) = 8.29, p = 0.005] and group [F(1, 62) = 12.75, p = 0.001]. These results indicate that both groups made more fixations to social images when paired with HAI images than when paired with LAI images; the ASD group made significantly more fixations on social images than TYP, in both contexts.

Detail Orientation–Object: A 2x2 (Group: ASD, TYP; Array: SOC + LAI, SOC + HAI) RM-ANOVA was conducted for the number of fixations on object images. There was no group x array interaction (p = 0.24). The main effect of array was at trend-level significance [F(1, 62) = 1.40, p = 0.056]. These results indicate that both groups made more fixations to LAI images than HAI images. There was no main effect of group (p = 0.735).

Fixation Duration–Social: A 2x2 (Group: ASD, TYP; Array: SOC + LAI, SOC + HAI) RM-ANOVA was conducted for the average fixation duration on social images. There was no group x array interaction (p = 0.156). There was a main effect of array [F(1, 62) = 5.77, p = 0.019]. There was also a main effect of group [F(1, 62) = 17.81, p < 0.0001]. These results indicate that both groups made longer fixations, on average, to faces in SOC + LAI arrays, compared to SOC + HAI arrays. Additionally, TYP participants made longer fixations to social images in both conditions, compared to ASD. Post-hoc paired samples t-test showed that for the ASD group only, fixations to social images were significantly shorter in duration when paired with HAI objects, compared to LAI objects [t(32) = 2805, p = 0.008]. Group differences are illustrated in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean (+/– standard error) fixation duration to social images in ASD and TYP participants. ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; TYP, typically developing; SOC, social; LAI, low autism interest; HAI, high autism interest. **p < 0.01.

Fixation Duration–Object: A 2x2 (Group: ASD, TYP; Array: SOC + LAI, SOC + HAI) RM-ANOVA was conducted for the average fixation duration on object images. There was no group x array interaction (p = 0.63). There was no main effect of array (p = 0.26) or group (p = 0.85). These results indicate that the average length of fixation did not differ based on group or object type.

Prioritization–Social: A 2x2 (Group: ASD, TYP; Array: SOC + LAI, SOC + HAI) RM-ANOVA was conducted for latency to first fixation for social images. A group x array interaction was at trend-level significance [F(1, 62) = 3.58, p = 0.063]. There was a main effect of array [F(1, 62) = 5.23, p = 0.026], and a trend-level main effect of group [F(1, 62) = 3.44, p = 0.068]. These results indicate that both groups looked at faces more quickly when faces were paired with LAI objects, compared to HAI objects. The trend-level interaction and group effects suggest this main effect of array is driven by the ASD group showing larger differences in face latency between arrays than the TYP groups. Post-hoc paired-samples t-tests show that for the ASD group only, latency to face is significantly slower when faces are paired with HAI objects, compared to LAI objects [t(32) = −2.53, p = 0.02]. Figure 4 illustrates prioritization differences between groups.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mean (+/– standard error) latency to first fixate on social images in ASD and TYP participants. ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; TYP, typically developing; SOC, social; LAI, low autism interest; HAI, high autism interest. *p < 0.05.

Prioritization–Object: A 2x2 (Group: ASD, TYP; Array: SOC + LAI, SOC + HAI) RM-ANOVA was conducted for latency to first fixation for object images. The group x array interaction was not significant (p = 0.22). There was no main effect of array (p = 0.574), but the main effect of group showed a trend toward significance [F(1, 62) = 3.58, p = 0.06). These results indicate the ASD group looked more quickly to both object types than TYP.

Correlations: Eye-Tracking Variables and Psychometric Measures

One additional ASD participant was excluded for psychometric correlational analyses due to missing data on the psychometric variables of interest. All correlation analyses were performed using log-transformed variables, as previously described. The following correlations were conducted using data from the ASD group only.

First, we calculated correlations between autism severity and specific eye-tracking variables. These variables included all those where group differences were found between ASD and TYP groups: Social and object preference, social detail orientation, social fixation duration, and social prioritization. Pearson correlation analyses revealed significant correlations between number of total current interests, as measured by the Interest Scale, and both the face and object preference variables. For SOC + HAI arrays, ASD participants who had a greater number of interests, spent significantly less time looking at face images (r = −0.60, p < 0.001; Figure 5A) and more time looking at object images (r = 0.35, p = 0.048; Figure 5B). This relationship was not seen in SOC + LAI arrays for either face (r = −0.33, p = 0.06) or object (r = 0.008, p = 0.97) images. We also examined the relationship between face and object preference in SOC + HAI arrays and the SRS, with the items relating to repetitive behavior removed. This revealed no relationship for either face preference (r = −0.07, p = 0.69; Figure 5C) or object preference (r = −0.08, p = 0.66; Figure 5D). There were no other significant correlations found between the remaining eye-tracking variables and any psychometric measures.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlation between eye-tracking variables and phenotype measures for ASD participants. (A) Correlation of social preference and total number of current interests; (B) Correlation of nonsocial preference and total number of current interests; (C) Correlation of social preference and total score on the SRS; (D) Correlation of nonsocial preference and total score on the SRS. ASD, Autism Spectrum Disorder; TYP, typically developing; SOC, social; HAI, high autism interest; IS, Interest Scale; SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale.

Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to assess visual preference and gaze dynamics to social and nonsocial stimuli in adolescents with ASD, compared to typically developing peers. Group differences were assessed using a preferential viewing task, which paired social images with either neutral or CI-related object images. As hypothesized, individuals with ASD preferred to look at object images (both LAI and CI-related), while TYP adolescents preferred to look at faces. Groups also differed in their prioritization of social information, such that TYP adolescents displayed a shorter latency to fixate on social images than those with ASD.

We hypothesized that social viewing in ASD may be specifically influenced by the presence of certain types of nonsocial images, such as those related to CI. This was true for two variables: Social latency and social fixation duration. Individuals with ASD displayed a longer latency to orient to faces when they were paired with HAI images than LAI images. Importantly, groups did not differ in latency to orient to faces when they were paired with LAI images. It is also worth noting that social preference was reduced in the presence of HAI images, compared to LAI images; however, this was true for both ASD and TYP participants. Together, these findings provide support for our hypothesis and suggest social attention in ASD may be uniquely influenced by particular pieces of nonsocial information. Our findings are in line with previous studies that have found enhanced viewing of nonsocial objects by persons with ASD (Klin et al., 2002; Sasson et al., 2008, 2011; Elison et al., 2012). A recent study of preferential viewing in young children with ASD also revealed similar patterns of attention, suggesting this enhanced nonsocial viewing may be stable throughout childhood and adolescents (Sasson and Touchstone, 2014). Together, these studies highlight the potential importance of examining how opportunities for social experience can be diminished by the presence of competing nonsocial experiences.

Unlike the previously mentioned findings, detail orientation did not seem to follow the same pattern as the other eye-tracking variables. Participants made more, but shorter, fixations to social images when these images were paired with HAI objects, compared to LAI objects and this pattern was more pronounced for participants with ASD than TYP. This pattern was not found for the number or duration of fixations to object images. These results align with a previous study of social + nonsocial visual arrays, which found increased detail orientation in ASD compared to TYP adolescent peers (Sasson et al., 2008). However, while non-significant, these data trended toward increased detail orientation for object images, rather than social.

The core feature of unusual or circumscribed interests in ASD is closely linked conceptually with the kind of nonsocial preference we found in the present study. The term circumscribed or restricted interest in ASD is often assumed clinically to represent a restriction or decrease in the number of interests in ASD relative to typically developing peers. However, we found no significant difference between groups for the number of interests endorsed on the Interest Scale, in line with previous studies of CI in ASD (Turner-Brown et al., 2011; Anthony et al., 2013). Also in line with the findings of previous studies, participants in our ASD sample endorsed interests that were more nonsocial in content than their TYP peers and more frequently engaged in their primary interest in solitude, while TYP peers more frequently engaged in their primary interest socially. These findings highlight the nonsocial nature of interests in ASD and help elucidate the association we found between social viewing in the context of HAI images and CI severity (as measured by the Interest Scale) in our ASD sample. We found that a preference for viewing nonsocial over social images during the paired preference task was associated with a greater amount of nonsocial interests in our ASD sample. This correlation may represent a relationship between atypicality of interest and stimulus preference in ASD.

The current study found deficits in social orientation and attention in participants with ASD, including decreased preference, decreased duration of fixation, and increased latency to view social images, compared to TYP peers. These results are consistent with the Social Motivation Theory of Autism (Dawson et al., 2005; Chevallier et al., 2012; Kohls et al., 2012). We also found evidence of enhanced nonsocial preference in individuals with ASD, indicated by increased preference for object images and decreased latency to fixate on object images, compared to TYP peers. Enhanced nonsocial motivation has been found in individuals with ASD using behavioral measures (Damiano et al., 2012; Sasson et al., 2012; Watson et al., 2015) and other eye-tracking paradigms (Sasson et al., 2008, 2011; Elison et al., 2012; Sasson and Touchstone, 2014). These object preference findings are important to consider in light of neuroimaging studies that show enhanced activation of reward circuitry in ASD in response to nonsocial information (Dichter et al., 2012; Cascio et al., 2014). We also found evidence that decreased social attention may be related to increases in nonsocial preference in ASD. Taken together, these studies of object preference suggest that motivational differences in ASD include both nonsocial and social sources of motivation and reward. Such a pattern indicates that an expanded version of the social motivation conceptual model of ASD may be more appropriate. A broader motivational model may account for both social impairments and restricted repetitive behaviors, as well as the potential inter-relationships between these two core ASD domains. From a motivational perspective, the potential for social and nonsocial sources of stimulation to compete for attention and effort in ASD suggests a more dynamic relationship between these sources of reward.

The phenomenon of increased motivation toward one type of stimulation contributing to decreased motivation for another source of stimulation has been termed “motivational toxicity” (Bozarth, 1994). This effect has been found in other clinical contexts such as substance abuse (e.g., Esch and and Stefano, 2004), some types of disordered eating (e.g., Smith and Robbins, 2013), and non-drug form of addiction such as compulsive internet use (e.g., Young, 1998) or gambling (e.g., Petry, 2006). In these contexts, as behavior related to the focus of the compulsion or addiction increases (e.g., drug intake, compulsive eating patterns, internet use) there is a corresponding reduction in the reward value of other forms of activity such as social relationships, vocational activities, and pursuit of other hobbies. Often, it is this secondary loss of reward value of more healthy or adaptive activities that contributes to functional impairment in these conditions (Bozarth, 1994). Given this motivational toxicity framework that can account for experience-dependent changes in motivation over time, it is interesting to note previous findings in ASD of increased risk for substance abuse (Butwicka et al., 2016), restricted food preferences (e.g., Schreck et al., 2004; Schreck and Williams, 2006; Bandini et al., 2010; Cermak et al., 2010; Emond et al., 2010), and increased drive for internet use (e.g., Mazurek et al., 2011; Kuo et al., 2014; MacMullin et al., 2016; Shane-Simpson et al., 2016). One hypothesis that emerges from this dynamic motivational framework is that several seemingly disparate aspects of the autism phenotype (e.g., social deficits, restricted interests, picky eating, special abilities) may be related to underlying deficits in motivation and reward function in ASD.

One potential criticism of the current study was our choice to use static, rather than dynamic stimuli. We chose to use static images for four reasons. First, static images were chosen to increase our ability to exert control over low-level properties of the social and nonsocial stimulus pairs (e.g., visual angle, luminance, contrast, intensity, and orientation). Previous studies have found that individuals with ASD may process visual information differently from their typically developing peers, including superior performance on visual detail-oriented tasks (Plaisted et al., 1998; O'Riordan and Plaisted, 2001; O'Riordan et al., 2001; O'riordan, 2004; Mottron et al., 2006; Kemner et al., 2008) and attention that is differentially driven by low-level stimulus properties relative to typically developing peers (Amso et al., 2014). Thus, matching our social and nonsocial stimuli on these features helps ensure that any stimulus-type difference in attention between groups is not simply a function of low-level processing advantage in ASD. This degree of salience matching is not possible when using more complex visual stimuli like movies, and thus use of dynamic stimuli in an effort to increase ecological validity represents an important trade-off between potential validity and experimental control. Second, across the previously published studies of eye-tracking in ASD a uniform finding has been atypicalities in attentional parameters associated with social stimuli and this has been found for both static (Pelphrey et al., 2002; Anderson et al., 2006; Sasson et al., 2008, 2011; McPartland et al., 2011; Elison et al., 2012; Sasson and Touchstone, 2014) and dynamic stimuli (Klin et al., 2002, 2009; Klin and Jones, 2008; Pierce et al., 2011, 2016; Jones and Klin, 2013; Chevallier et al., 2015). Thus, it is clear that the kind of diminished attention to social stimuli found in the present study is consistent with similar findings in previous studies of both dynamic and static displays. Therefore, while the nature of stimulus presentation (static/dynamic) may alter the level or amount of attention obtained (e.g., more attention paid to dynamic stimuli), it does not appear to alter the relative differences in attention to social vs. nonsocial images that is the focus of this study. Third, static visual image viewing has been repeatedly shown to elicit widespread neural activation outside of the visual cortex similar to viewing of dynamic images, and this has been shown to be the case for both social and nonsocial stimuli. For example, perception of static faces has been shown to increase activity in brain regions associated with emotion, reward, spatial perception, and motion processing (Haxby et al., 2002). Similarly, viewing static images of tools can elicit increased activity in motion and motor planning areas of the brain, compared to other object categories, such as animals (e.g., Chao et al., 1999). These studies suggest that across image categories, viewing static images is sufficient to recruit activation in brain regions similar to those that would show enhanced activity during actual use of objects or social interaction. Finally, it is not necessarily the case that viewing static images is not ecologically valid, particularly in the realm of operationalizing preference or choice. There are a variety of contexts in which people do choose to view pictures (e.g., children's story books, museums). This is perhaps most notable regarding use of the internet, where social media platforms such as Instagram and Facebook largely revolve around viewing static images.

Another limitation of our image set is that they were not matched in familiarity between social and nonsocial; faces were of strangers, but objects were items with which participants may have had regular interactions. However, it is important to consider that this relative difference in familiarity of faces and objects would be true for both groups: Faces were novel for both ASD and typically developing participants, just as objects were likely familiar. Thus, it is unlikely that familiarization alone could account for the clear differences observed between groups. It is possible that inclusion of familiar social images (e.g., faces of family members) may have elicited enhanced social attention in participants with ASD, although this has not been observed in previous studies (Dalton et al., 2005; Sterling et al., 2008; Gillespie-Smith et al., 2014). Likewise, it is important to note that our HAI stimuli were also not individualized to be the most salient or familiar object for each ASD participant in relation to his or her own idiosyncratic circumscribed interest. Thus, although the unfamiliar faces may have contributed to some degree of overestimation of deficits in social attention in the ASD group, the use of non-individualized CI images also likely underestimated the degree to which nonsocial attention was biased in the ASD group. Previous studies of ASD have used nonsocial stimuli that are specific to an individual's circumscribed interest (e.g., Cascio et al., 2014; Foss-Feig et al., 2016). In contrast, our method allows us to examine the effect of general stimulus categories (social vs. nonsocial). Indeed, it is remarkable that even nonsocial images outside of a person with autism's very idiosyncratic circumscribed interest were still capable of biasing his or her attention. The presence of this more general nonsocial preference points out that object bias in ASD may extend beyond just individualized areas of interest. Further, it suggests that a generalized bias to attend to and engage with nonsocial, rather than social, sources of stimulation may set the stage for the later development and refinement of a more idiosyncratic nonsocial circumscribed interest.

The metrics derived from the paired preference approach presented here may be relevant for use in the clinic in the context of both early screening and the development of outcome measures. One advantage to this approach is that it is passive and brief in nature, requiring little to no instructional control; therefore, it may be applicable across a wide range of cognitive and language impairments associated with ASD and at very early points in development. The eye-tracking methods presented here are feasible and reliable for use in infants (e.g., Colombo et al., 1988; Gredebäck et al., 2009; Libertus and Needham, 2014) and thus may be particularly amenable to the cognitive or attentional profiles observed in infancy that are associated with increased risk of ASD development in later childhood. There is also a need for objective measures of ASD-related impairments that are sensitive to change for the purpose of measuring intervention outcomes (Scahill et al., 2013). Eye-tracking metrics have been extensively used to detect maturational changes in typically developing infants (e.g., Hunnius and Geuze, 2004; Karatekin, 2007; Oakes and Ellis, 2013) and in a limited number of studies in ASD (e.g., Nakano et al., 2010; Elison et al., 2012). If nonsocial attentional bias occurs early in ASD, then identification of this feature during infancy could help guide future research on early identification and intervention.

Our results add to previous literature that has found enhanced nonsocial preference in ASD) and extends this body of evidence by showing that the presence of nonsocial information can alter social orientation and attention in adolescents with ASD. These findings suggest a more complex pattern of motivational influences in autism than is suggested by the social motivation hypothesis: Both diminished social motivation and increased nonsocial motivation may contribute to the development of ASD in general and to ASD-associated atypicalities in attention and subsequent information processing in particular.

Ethics Statement

The protocol for this study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Vanderbilt University Medical Center and the University of North Carolina, Chapel-Hill. All participants completed an informed consent procedure prior to the beginning of the study. Written parental consent was required for all participants who were minors. All minor participants were also required to give assent to the study tasks.

Author Contributions

Each author (KU, NS, RS, AW, SM, LT, and JB) was involved in revising the work critically for important intellectual content, gave his or her final approval of this version of the paper, and agrees to be accountable for all aspects of the work. JB conceived of the idea and with NS, designed the work. KU drafted the manuscript. KU, NS, RS, AW, and JB contributed to the analysis and interpretation of data. SM, LT contributed to data acquisition.

Funding

NIMH R01 MH073402; NICHD R01 HD 082127. Autism Speaks 9557.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Assistance for this study was provided by Division TEACCH and also by the Subject Registry and Behavioral Measurement Research Cores of the UNC Neurodevelopmental Disorders Research Center (P30 HD03110). We thank Dr. Gabriel Dichter, Megan Kovac, Cara Damiano, and Anna Sabatino-DiCrisco for their help with data collection, and the participants and their families for their participation in this study.

References

Amso, D., Haas, S., Tenenbaum, E., Markant, J., and Sheinkopf, S. J. (2014). Bottom-up attention orienting in young children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 664–673. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1925-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, C. J., Colombo, J., and Jill Shaddy, D. (2006). Visual scanning and pupillary responses in young children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 28, 1238–1256. doi: 10.1080/13803390500376790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anthony, L. G., Kenworthy, L., Yerys, B. E., Jankowski, K. F., James, J. D., Harms, M. B., et al. (2013). Interests in high-functioning autism are more intense, interfering, and idiosyncratic than those in neurotypical development. Dev. Psychopathol. 25, 643–652. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandini, L. G., Anderson, S. E., Curtin, C., Cermak, S., Evans, E. W., Scampini, R., et al. (2010). Food selectivity in children with autism spectrum disorders and typically developing children. J. Pediatr. 157, 259–264. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodfish, J., Symons, F., and Lewis, M. (1999). The repetitive behavior scale. West. Carol. Cent. Res. Rep.

PubMed Abstract

Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., and Lewis, M. H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: comparisons to mental retardation. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 30, 237–243. doi: 10.1023/A:1005596502855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bozarth, M. A. (1994). “Pleasure systems in the brain,” in Pleasure: The Politics and the Reality, ed D. M. Warburton (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons), 5–14.

Google Scholar

Butwicka, A., Långström, N., Larsson, H., Lundström, S., Serlachius, E., Almqvist, C., et al. (2016). Increased risk for substance use-related problems in autism spectrum disorders: a population-based cohort study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2914-2. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cascio, C. J., Foss-Feig, J. H., Heacock, J., Schauder, K. B., Loring, W. A., Rogers, B. P., et al. (2014). Affective neural response to restricted interests in autism spectrum disorders. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 162–171. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cermak, S. A., Curtin, C., and Bandini, L. G. (2010). Food selectivity and sensory sensitivity in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 110, 238–246. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2009.10.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chao, L. L., Haxby, J. V., and Martin, A. (1999). Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 913–919. doi: 10.1038/13217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., and Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chevallier, C., Parish-Morris, J., McVey, A., Rump, K. M., Sasson, N. J., Herrington, J. D., et al. (2015). Measuring social attention and motivation in autism spectrum disorder using eye-tracking: stimulus type matters. Autism Res. 8, 620–628. doi: 10.1002/aur.1479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colombo, J., Mitchell, D. W., and Horowitz, F. D. (1988). Infant visual attention in the paired-comparison paradigm: test-retest and attention-performance relations. Child Dev. 59, 1198–1210. doi: 10.2307/1130483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantino, J. N., and Gruber, C. P. (2002). The Social Responsiveness Scale. Los Angel, CA: West. Psychol. Serv.

Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., et al. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 519–526. doi: 10.1038/nn1421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damiano, C. R., Aloi, J., Treadway, M., Bodfish, J. W., and Dichter, G. S. (2012). Adults with autism spectrum disorders exhibit decreased sensitivity to reward parameters when making effort-based decisions. J. Neurodev. Disord. 4, 13. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, G. (1991). VIII A psychobiological perspective on the early socio-emotional development of children with autism. Models Integr. 3, 207.

Google Scholar

Dawson, G., and Lewy, A. (1989). Arousal, Attention, and the Socioemotional Impairments of Individuals with Autism. Available online at: http://psycnet.apa.org.proxy.library.vanderbilt.edu/psycinfo/1989-97258-003 (Accessed July 26, 2016).

Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Wijsman, E., Schellenberg, G., Estes, A., Munson, J., et al. (2005). Neurocognitive and electrophysiological evidence of altered face processing in parents of children with autism: implications for a model of abnormal development of social brain circuitry in autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 17, 679–697. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dichter, G. S., Felder, J. N., Green, S. R., Rittenberg, A. M., Sasson, N. J., and Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Reward circuitry function in autism spectrum disorders. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 160–172. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elison, J. T., Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Dichter, G., and Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Age trends in visual exploration of social and nonsocial information in children with autism. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 6, 842–851. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emond, A., Emmett, P., Steer, C., and Golding, J. (2010). Feeding symptoms, dietary patterns, and growth in young children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 126, e337–e342. doi: 10.1542/peds.2009-2391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esch, T., and Stefano, G. B. (2004). The neurobiology of pleasure, reward processes, addiction and their health implications. Neuroendocrinol. Lett. 25, 235–251.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science 146, 668–670. doi: 10.1126/science.146.3644.668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foss-Feig, J. H., McGugin, R. W., Gauthier, I., Mash, L. E., Ventola, P., and Cascio, C. J. (2016). A functional neuroimaging study of fusiform response to restricted interests in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J. Neurodev. Disord. 8:1. doi: 10.1186/s11689-016-9149-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillespie-Smith, K., Doherty-Sneddon, G., Hancock, P. J., and Riby, D. M. (2014). That looks familiar: attention allocation to familiar and unfamiliar faces in children with autism spectrum disorder. Cogn. Neuropsychiatry 19, 554–569. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2014.943365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goren, C. C., Sarty, M., and Wu, P. Y. (1975). Visual following and pattern discrimination of face-like stimuli by newborn infants. Pediatrics 56, 544–549.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gotham, K., Risi, S., Dawson, G., Tager-Flusberg, H., Joseph, R., Carter, A., et al. (2008). A replication of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) revised algorithms. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 47, 642–651. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816bffb7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gredebäck, G., Johnson, S., and von Hofsten, C. (2009). Eye tracking in infancy research. Dev. Neuropsychol. 35, 1–19. doi: 10.1080/87565640903325758

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., and Gobbini, M. I. (2002). Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 59–67. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01330-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Honey, E., Leekam, S., Turner, M., and McConachie, H. (2007). Repetitive behaviour and play in typically developing children and children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 1107–1115. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0253-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunnius, S., and Geuze, R. H. (2004). Developmental changes in visual scanning of dynamic faces and abstract stimuli in infants: a longitudinal study. Infancy 6, 231–255. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0602_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., and Morton, J. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition 40, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, W., and Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature 504, 427–431. doi: 10.1038/nature12715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karatekin, C. (2007). Eye tracking studies of normative and atypical development. Dev. Rev. 27, 283–348. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, A. S., and Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. Wiley Online Library Available online at: http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/ (Accessed September 23, 2015).

Kemner, C., van Ewijk, L., van Engeland, H., and Hooge, I. (2008). Brief report: eye movements during visual search tasks indicate enhanced stimulus discriminability in subjects with PDD. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 553–557. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0406-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J. H., Gunderson, V. M., and Swartz, K. S. (1999). Humans All Look Alike: Cross-Species Face Recognition in Infant Pigtailed Macaque Monkeys. Biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. (Albuquerque, NM).

Google Scholar

Klin, A., and Jones, W. (2008). Altered face scanning and impaired recognition of biological motion in a 15-month-old infant with autism. Dev. Sci. 11, 40–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00608.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., and Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 809–816. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., and Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature 459, 257–261. doi: 10.1038/nature07868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohls, G., Chevallier, C., Troiani, V., and Schultz, R. T. (2012). Social “wanting” dysfunction in autism: neurobiological underpinnings and treatment implications. J. Neurodev. Disord. 4, 1–20. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuo, M. H., Orsmond, G. I., Coster, W. J., and Cohn, E. S. (2014). Media use among adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 18, 914–923. doi: 10.1177/1362361313497832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lam, K. S., and Aman, M. G. (2007). The repetitive behavior scale-revised: independent validation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 855–866. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0213-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Libertus, K., and Needham, A. (2014). Face preference in infancy and its relation to motor activity. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 38, 529–538. doi: 10.1177/0165025414535122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., Risi, S., Gotham, K., and Bishop, S. L. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: ADOS-2. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Lord, C., Rutter, M., and Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 659–685. doi: 10.1007/BF02172145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacMullin, J. A., Lunsky, Y., and Weiss, J. A. (2016). Plugged in: electronics use in youth and young adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 20, 45–54. doi: 10.1177/1362361314566047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazurek, M. O., Shattuck, P. T., Wagner, M., and Cooper, B. P. (2011). Prevalence and correlates of screen-based media use among youths with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 42, 1757–1767. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1413-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPartland, J. C., Webb, S. J., Keehn, B., and Dawson, G. (2011). Patterns of visual attention to faces and objects in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 148–157. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1033-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., and Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 27–43. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakano, T., Tanaka, K., Endo, Y., Yamane, Y., Yamamoto, T., Nakano, Y., et al. (2010). Atypical gaze patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated from developmental changes in gaze behaviour. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., rspb20100587. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Riordan, M. A. (2004). Superior visual search in adults with autism. Autism Int. J. Res. Pract. 8, 229–248. doi: 10.1177/1362361304045219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 27, 719. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.27.3.719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Riordan, M., and Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 54, 961–979. doi: 10.1080/713756000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oakes, L. M., and Ellis, A. E. (2013). An eye-tracking investigation of developmental changes in infants' exploration of upright and inverted human faces. Infancy Off. J. Int. Soc. Infant Stud. 18, 134. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00107.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., and Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 32, 249–261. doi: 10.1023/A:1016374617369

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petry, N. M. (2006). Should the scope of addictive behaviors be broadened to include pathological gambling? Addiction 101, 152–160. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01593.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R., and Desmond, J. (2011). Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 101–109. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, K., Marinero, S., Hazin, R., McKenna, B., Barnes, C. C., and Malige, A. (2016). Eye tracking reveals abnormal visual preference for geometric images as an early biomarker of an autism spectrum disorder subtype associated with increased symptom severity. Biol. Psychiatry 79, 657–666. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plaisted, K., O'Riordan, M., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 39, 765–775.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sasson, N. J., Dichter, G. S., and Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Affective responses by adults with autism are reduced to social images but elevated to images related to circumscribed interests. PLoS ONE 7:e42457. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasson, N. J., Elison, J. T., Turner-Brown, L. M., Dichter, G. S., and Bodfish, J. W. (2011). Brief report: circumscribed attention in young children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 242–247. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1038-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasson, N. J., and Touchstone, E. W. (2014). Visual attention to competing social and object images by preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 584–592. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1910-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S., and Bodfish, J. W. (2008). Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Res. 1, 31–42. doi: 10.1002/aur.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scahill, L., Aman, M. G., Lecavalier, L., Halladay, A. K., Bishop, S. L., Bodfish, J. W., et al. (2013). Measuring repetitive behaviors as a treatment endpoint in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 19, 38–52. doi: 10.1177/1362361313510069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreck, K. A., and Williams, K. (2006). Food preferences and factors influencing food selectivity for children with autism spectrum disorders. Res. Dev. Disabil. 27, 353–363. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2005.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreck, K. A., Williams, K., and Smith, A. F. (2004). A comparison of eating behaviors between children with and without autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 433–438. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000037419.78531.86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shane-Simpson, C., Brooks, P. J., Obeid, R., Denton, E., and Gillespie-Lynch, K. (2016). Associations between compulsive internet use and the autism spectrum. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 23, 152–165. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2015.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D. G., and Robbins, T. W. (2013). The neurobiological underpinnings of obesity and binge eating: a rationale for adopting the food addiction model. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 804–810. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

South, M., Ozonoff, S., and McMahon, W. M. (2005). Repetitive behavior profiles in asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35, 145–158. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1992-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterling, L., Dawson, G., Webb, S., Murias, M., Munson, J., Panagiotides, H., et al. (2008). The role of face familiarity in eye tracking of faces by individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 1666–1675. doi: 10.1007/s10803-004-1992-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., et al. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 168, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner-Brown, L. M., Lam, K. S., Holtzclaw, T. N., Dichter, G. S., and Bodfish, J. W. (2011). Phenomenology and measurement of circumscribed interests in autism spectrum disorders. Autism 15, 437–456. doi: 10.1177/1362361310386507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenza, E., Simion, F., Cassia, V. M., and Umiltà, C. (1996). Face preference at birth. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 22, 892–903.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Watson, K. K., Miller, S., Hannah, E., Kovac, M., Damiano, C. R., Sabatino-DiCrisco, A., et al. (2015). Increased reward value of non-social stimuli in children and adolescents with autism. Front. Psychol. 6:1026. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, K. S. (1998). Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 1, 237–244.

Google Scholar

Keywords: social motivation theory, circumscribed interests, eye-tracking, preferential viewing, reward

Citation: Unruh KE, Sasson NJ, Shafer RL, Whitten A, Miller SJ, Turner-Brown L and Bodfish JW (2016) Social Orienting and Attention Is Influenced by the Presence of Competing Nonsocial Information in Adolescents with Autism. Front. Neurosci. 10:586. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00586

Received: 26 September 2016; Accepted: 07 December 2016;
Published: 23 December 2016.

Edited by:

Ahmet O. Caglayan, Istanbul Bilim University, Turkey

Reviewed by:

Stephanie Ameis, Hospital for Sick Children, Canada
Om Sai Ramesh Vallamkonda, Lady Hardinge Medical College, India

Copyright © 2016 Unruh, Sasson, Shafer, Whitten, Miller, Turner-Brown and Bodfish. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kathryn E. Unruh, kathryn.e.unruh@vanderbilt.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.