Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 23 December 2014
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings

Why sprint interval training is inappropriate for a largely sedentary population

  • 1Health Psychology and Behavioural Medicine Research Group, School of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin University, Perth, WA, Australia
  • 2School of Sport and Service Management, University of Brighton, Eastbourne, UK

Public health practitioners and researchers in behavioral medicine recognize the need to find effective physical activity interventions and prescriptions to curb the growth in inactivity and prevent chronic illness (Conn et al., 2009; Hagger, 2010; Hardcastle et al., 2012; Katzmarzyk and Lear, 2012). For example, researchers in exercise physiology have focused on the minimal dose of exercise needed to gain favorable physiological adaptations to cardiovascular and metabolic systems (Gibala et al., 2012). Efforts to identify a minimal dose of exercise are linked to the problem of exercise adherence with few people meeting current physical activity guidelines of 30 min per day of moderate intensity exercise. Given that time is the most commonly cited barrier to exercise (Trost et al., 2002; Sequeira et al., 2011), exercise professionals have focused attention on the development of time-efficient exercise interventions (Gibala, 2007). A recent development is the advocacy of Sprint Interval Training (SIT) as a means to attain substantial health benefits with a lower overall exercise volume. SIT is characterized by repeated, brief (4–6 × <30 s), intermittent bursts of all-out exercise, interspersed by periods (approximately 4.5 min) of active or passive recovery (Gibala et al., 2012). Research has consistently demonstrated that participation in SIT results in a host of physiological adaptations including improvements in health and fitness indicators (Burgomaster et al., 2006, 2008; Gibala et al., 2006, 2012; Rossow et al., 2010; Tong et al., 2011). In addition, these improvements have been reported to be equal or superior to traditional continuous aerobic training despite SIT involving a substantially lower total overall training volume (Rossow et al., 2010; Tong et al., 2011; Gibala et al., 2012; Cocks et al., 2013). Consequently, SIT is being advocated as a time-efficient alternative intervention for the achievement of fitness and health benefits through exercise (Gibala, 2007; Whyte et al., 2013).

In this article we contend that SIT is unlikely to be taken up by the majority of the sedentary population and caution is needed before such training is advocated to the general public. Proponents of SIT have focused almost exclusively on physiological adaptations. However, the exclusive focus fails to consider whether a largely sedentary population will feel physically capable and sufficiently motivated to take up and maintain a regime of highly intense exercise. Based on theory and research in exercise psychology, we contend that the prospect of participating in SIT for previously sedentary individuals is likely to be considered too arduous and may evoke anticipated perceived incompetence, lower self-esteem, and potential failure (Williams and Gill, 1995; Hein and Hagger, 2007; Lindwall et al., 2011). They may likely be more inclined to avoid participating as a consequence. We also contend that should previously sedentary individuals be introduced to high intensity exercise of the type proposed in SIT it will likely evoke a high degree of negative affect that may lead to an avoidant response with the prospect of future sessions. In addition, we contend that SIT is a complex and structured regime that requires high levels of self-discipline and self-regulation and is, therefore, unlikely to be adopted outside the laboratory environment (Hagger, 2013; Hagger and Luszczynska, 2014). Finally, we debate the notion that SIT is time-efficient and suggest that it does not sufficiently address “lack of time” as a commonly-cited barrier to exercise (Hardcastle and Hagger, 2011).

In a largely sedentary population, the strenuous nature of SIT is likely to be a deterrent to participation because individuals tend to avoid exercise if they find it aversive. Several theories including social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1977), achievement motivation theory (Weiner, 1985) and self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 1985; Hagger et al., 2006; Chatzisarantis et al., 2007) contend that a high level of motivation and competence are needed to participate in regular physical activity. Typically, sedentary and low-active individuals do not feel competent in the physical domain and may not, therefore, feel sufficiently confident to engage in the activity (Teixeira et al., 2012). The motivation and effort required to participate in high intensity exercise is much higher than that needed to undertake activities of a moderate intensity (e.g. walking) (Williams and Gill, 1995; Tritter et al., 2013). If individuals feel unable to demonstrate competence in SIT, they are more likely to invest little effort in a prescribed activity or avoid it all together. Low competence, self-esteem, and motivation among sedentary individuals are a considerable problem for exercise promoters presented with the task of developing means to promote increased activity to a resistant population (Williams and Gill, 1995; Hein and Hagger, 2007).

In addition to competence and motivation, enjoyment is also a predictor of exercise adherence and most people do not enjoy high intensity exercise (Parfitt and Hughes, 2009). SIT may be inappropriate for a largely sedentary population because the negative affect that such supra-threshold intensities evoke could diminish intrinsic motivation and discourage exercise adherence. There is considerable evidence that adherence to exercise is influenced by affective responses to exercise intensity. In particular, enjoyment and feelings of pleasure have been shown to decrease as exercise intensity increases (Ekkekakis et al., 2011). The American College of Sports Medicine (ACSM) exercise guidelines state that exercise-induced feelings of fatigue and negative affect can act as a deterrent to continued participation (American College of Sports Medicine, 2013). The supra-threshold intensities induce a psychobiological stress response that is felt as unpleasant. Conversely, exercise intensities below these thresholds can be effective for improving health and fitness and are generally rated as more pleasant, and more likely to be tolerated by, most individuals irrespective of age or physical condition. In a study on sedentary adults' adherence to exercise prescriptions, Perri et al. (2002) found significantly greater adherence in the moderate intensity condition compared to the high intensity condition. Prescribing a higher frequency (5–7 days vs. 3–4) of exercise sessions increased the accumulation of exercise without a decline in adherence, whereas prescribing a higher intensity decreased adherence and resulted in the completion of less exercise over the 6 months. Although some studies have found SIT to be more enjoyable than continuous exercise (e.g., Bartlett et al., 2011), they have tended to focus on “recreationally active” participants meaning that the findings cannot be directly applied to an unfit and sedentary population. In addition, recent research has focused on strategies to reduce the negative affect experienced by individuals when performing SIT such as listening to music or receiving feedback to boost self-efficacy (e.g., Tritter et al., 2013; Stork et al., 2014). However, we contend that such endeavors are futile given that such types of training are unlikely to be adopted or maintained by sedentary individuals in the first place.

SIT is also inappropriate for a largely sedentary population because it is a relatively complex and structured exercise regime that requires a high degree of self-regulation to be effective. Mostly SIT protocols have been undertaken in laboratory settings under the supervision of exercise physiologists (e.g., Gibala et al., 2006; Burgomaster et al., 2008; Cocks et al., 2013; Whyte et al., 2013) not to mention the “significant encouragement provided during the Wingate tests” by the research team (Cocks et al., 2013, p. 645). The transfer of SIT to an unsupervised setting in which the onus is placed on inexperienced sedentary individuals to self-select the appropriate intensity is likely to be problematic. Individuals will need to know the speed and effort required to work during the supra-maximal and active recovery periods. They will also require self-monitoring tools at hand such as a stopwatch to time the intervals, and will also need to have sufficient know-how to undertake an appropriate warm up and cool down to prevent injury. It is unlikely that sedentary individuals with low levels of experience with exercise are going to be able to be independently successful in a complicated activity like SIT. Further research is needed to explore whether the SIT can be successfully implemented and maintained in a real life setting with those insufficiently active and unfamiliar with vigorous intensity exercise.

Finally, we question the often-cited benefit of SIT that it is time-efficient. If the minimal number of intermittent bursts of activity is four followed by four 4.5 min breaks, then at least 20 min is needed and this does not include a warm up or cool down. Therefore, in reality individuals would still need to free up 30 min in order to participate, even if on fewer days of the week (three as opposed to five). For sedentary individuals, a time-efficient means to engage in a programme of regular physical activity and structure it around a busy lifestyle is attractive. However, coupled with other concerns about the adverse psychological effects of high-intensity exercise regimens like SIT, and their relative complexity to undertake, we contend that lower intensity bouts of exercise of a similar duration are likely to be more appealing to sedentary individuals. Exercise of this nature may, therefore, be optimally effective in promoting adherence to exercise in this population.

In summary, we contend that although SIT appears to be an effective exercise modality for physiological benefit, it is unlikely to be effective as a means to promote regular participation in physical activity in a largely sedentary population. We have argued that SIT is inappropriate for sedentary individuals because engaging in such training requires high levels of motivation and confidence. In addition, high intensity exercise is likely to evoke to negative affect which may lead to subsequent avoidance of further exercise. SIT programmes of exercise are also relatively complex and involve a high degree of self-regulation, which may also be a barrier to continuation in those who are uninitiated. Finally, we contend that SIT should not necessarily be considered time efficient as a session would likely last at least 30 min. We would like to see further research that addresses the motivational factors and responses of sedentary people to SIT. Specifically, we propose the following research agenda to improve knowledge of SIT in sedentary populations: (1) the acceptability of, and affective responses to, SIT programmes; (2) the social cognitive and motivational factors that may be related to participation in, and adherence to, SIT programmes; (3) the degree of adherence to SIT programmes compared to programmes of continuous aerobic exercise of moderate intensity; and (4) whether SIT programmes can be transferred to natural settings outside of the supervised laboratory environment.

Author Contributions

Sarah J. Hardcastle conceived the ideas presented in the article and took the lead role in drafting the article. Hannah Ray, Louisa Beale, and Martin S. Hagger assisted in drafting the article.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

American College of Sports Medicine, (2013). ACSM's Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription, 9th Edn. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: Freeman.

Bartlett, J. D., Close, G. L., MacLaren, D. P. M., Gregson, W., Drust, B., and Morton, J. P. (2011). High intensity interval running is perceived to be more enjoyable than moderate-intensity continuous exercise: implications for exercise adherence. J. Sport Sci. 29, 547–559. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2010.545427

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgomaster, K., Heigenhauser, G., and Gibala, M. (2006). Effect of short-term interval training on human skeletal muscle carbohydrate metabolism during exercise and time trial performance. J. Appl. Physiol. 100, 2041–2047. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.01220.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgomaster, K., Howarth, K., Phillips, S., Rakobowchuk, M., MacDonald, M., McGee, S., et al. (2008). Similar metabolic adaptations during exercise after low volume sprint interval training and traditional endurance training in humans. J. Physiol. 586, 151–160. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.142109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., and Smith, B. (2007). Influences of perceived autonomy support on physical activity within the theory of planned behavior. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 37, 934–954. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cocks, M., Shaw, C. S., Shepherd, S. O., Fisher, J. P., Ranasinghe, A. M., Barker, T. A. et al. (2013). Sprint interval training and endurance training are equally effective in increasing muscle microvascular density and eNOS content in sedentary males. J. Physiol. 591, 641–656. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.239566

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conn, V. S., Hafdahl, A. R., Cooper, P. S., Brown, L. M., and Lusk, S. L. (2009). Meta-analysis of workplace physical activity interventions. Am. J. Prev. Med. 37, 330–339. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.06.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation amd Self-determination in Human Behaviour. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Ekkekakis, P., Parfitt, G., and Petruzello, S. J. (2011). The pleasure and displeasure people feel when they exercise at different intensities: decennial update and progress towards a tripartite rationale for exercise intensity prescription. Sports Med. 41, 641–671. doi: 10.2165/11590680-000000000-00000

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibala, M. (2007). High-intensity interval training: a time-efficient strategy for health promotion? Curr. Sports Med. Rep. 6, 211–213. doi: 10.1097/01.CSMR.0000306472.95337.e9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibala, M. J., Little, P. J., vanEssan, M., Wilkin, G. P., Burgomaster, K. A., Safdar, A., et al. (2006). Short-term sprint interval versus traditional endurance training: similar initial adaptations in human skeletal muscle and exercise performance. J. Physiol. 575, 901–911. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.112094

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibala, M., Little, J. P., MacDonald, M. J., and Hawley, J. A. (2012). Physiological adaptations to low-volume, high-intensity interval training in health and disease. J. Physiol. 590, 1077–1084. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2011.224725

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S. (2010). Current issues and new directions in psychology and health: physical activity research showcasing theory into practice. Psychol. Health 25, 1–5. doi: 10.1080/08870440903268637

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S. (2013). The multiple pathways by which self-control predicts behavior. Front. Psychol. 4:849. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00849

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., and Harris, J. (2006). The process by which relative autonomous motivation affects intentional behavior: comparing effects across dieting and exercise behaviors. Motiv. Emot. 30, 306–320. doi: 10.1007/s11031-006-9046-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., and Luszczynska, A. (2014). Implementation intention and action planning interventions in health contexts: state of the research and proposals for the way forward. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 6, 1–47. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardcastle, S., Blake, N., and Hagger, M. S. (2012). The effectiveness of a motivational interviewing primary-care based intervention on physical activity and predictors of change in a disadvantaged community. J. Behav. Med. 35, 318–333. doi: 10.1007/s10865-012-9417-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardcastle, S., and Hagger, M. S. (2011). “You can't do it on your own”: experiences of a motivational interviewing intervention on physical activity and dietary behaviour. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 12, 314–323. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hein, V., and Hagger, M. S. (2007). Global self-esteem, goal achievement orientations and self-determined behavioural regulations in physical education setting. J. Sports Sci. 25, 149–259. doi: 10.1080/02640410600598315

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzmarzyk, P. T., and Lear, S. A. (2012). Physical activity for obese individuals: a systematic review of effects on chronic disease risk factors. Obes. Rev. 13, 95–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00933.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindwall, M., Larsmann, P., and Hagger, M. S. (2011). The reciprocal relationship between physical activity and depression in older European adults: a prospective cross-lagged panel design using SHARE data. Health Psychol. 30, 453–462. doi: 10.1037/a0023268

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parfitt, G., and Hughes, S. (2009). The exercise intensity-affect relationship: evidence and implications for exercise behaviour. J. Exerc. Sci. Fit. 7, S34–S41. doi: 10.1016/S1728-869X(09)60021-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perri, M. G., Anton, S. D., Durning, P. E., and Ketterson, T. U. (2002). Adherence to exercise prescriptions: effects of prescribing moderate versus high levels of intensity and frequency. Health Psychol. 21, 452–458. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.21.5.452

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossow, L., Yan, H., Fahs, C. A., Ranadive, S. M., Agiovlasitis, S., Wilund, K. R., et al. (2010). Post exercise hypotension in an endurance-trained population of men and women following high-intensity interval training and steady state cycling. Am. J. Hypertens. 23, 358–367. doi: 10.1038/ajh.2009.269

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sequeira, S., Cruz, C., Pinto, D., Santos, L., and Marques, A. (2011). Prevalence of barriers for physical activity in adults according to gender and socioeconomic status. Br. J. Sports Med. 45, A18–A19. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090606.59

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stork, M. J., Kwan, M., Gibala, M. J., and Martin Ginis, K. A. (2014). Music enhances performance and sprint interval exercise. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000494. [Epub ahead of print].

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Teixeira, P., Carraca, E., Markland, D., Silva, M., and Ryan, R. (2012). Exercise, physical activity, and self-determination theory: a systematic review. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 9:78. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-9-78

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, T., Chung, P., Leung, R., Nie, J., Lin, H., and Zheng, J (2011). Effects of non-wingate-based high-intensity interval training on cardiorespiratory fitness and aerobic-based exercise capacity in sedentary subjects: a preliminary study. J. Exerc. Sci. Fit. 9, 75–81. doi: 10.1016/S1728-869X(12)60001-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tritter, A., Fitzgeorge, L., Cramp, A., Valiulis, P., and Prapavessis, H. (2013). Self-efficacy and affect responses to sprint interval training. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 14, 886–890. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trost, S. G., Owen, N., Bauman, A. E., Sallis, J. F., and Brown, W. (2002). Correlates of adults' participation in physical activity: review and update. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 34, 1996–2001. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000038974.76900.92

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychol. Rev. 92, 548–573. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X92.4.548

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whyte, L. J., Ferguson, C., Wilson, J., Scott, R. A., and Gill, J. M. R. (2013). Effects of a single bout of very high-intensity exercise on metabolic health biomarkers in overweight/obese sedentary men. Metab. Clin. Exp. 62, 212–219. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2012.07.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L., and Gill, D. L. (1995). The role of perceived competence in the motivation of physical activity. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 17, 363–378.

Keywords: exercise psychology, sprint interval training, exercise intensity, behavior-change, feeling states, exercise adherence

Citation: Hardcastle SJ, Ray H, Beale L and Hagger MS (2014) Why sprint interval training is inappropriate for a largely sedentary population. Front. Psychol. 5:1505. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01505

Received: 04 November 2014; Accepted: 06 December 2014;
Published online: 23 December 2014.

Edited by:

Angelo Compare, University of Bergamo, Italy

Reviewed by:

Agostino Brugnera, University of Bergamo, Italy

Copyright © 2014 Hardcastle, Ray, Beale and Hagger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: sarah.hardcastle@curtin.edu.au

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.