Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 13 September 2021
Sec. Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias
Volume 13 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2021.714220

Cortical Frontoparietal Network Dysfunction in CHMP2B-Frontotemporal Dementia

Christian Sandøe Musaeus1* Jette Stokholm Pedersen1 Troels Wesenberg Kjær2,3 Peter Johannsen1† Gunhild Waldemar1,3 Maria Joy Normann Haverberg2 Theis Bacher2 Jørgen Erik Nielsen1,3 Peter Roos1 The FReJA Consortium
  • 1Danish Dementia Research Centre (DDRC), Rigshospitalet and Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark
  • 2Department of Neurology, Zealand University Hospital, Roskilde, Denmark
  • 3Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark

A rare cause of inherited frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is a mutation in the CHMP2B gene on chromosome 3 leading to the autosomal dominantly inherited FTD (CHMP2B-FTD). Since CHMP2B-FTD is clinically well-characterized, and patients show a distinct pattern of executive dysfunction, the condition offers possible insight in the early electroencephalographic (EEG) changes in the cortical networks. Specifically, EEG microstate analysis parses the EEG signals into topographies believed to represent discrete network activations. We investigated the EEG dynamics in patients with symptomatic CHMP2B-FTD (n = 5) as well as pre-symptomatic mutation carriers (n = 5) compared to non-carrier family members (n = 6). The data was parsed into four archetypal microstates and global power was calculated. A trend was found for lower occurrence in microstate D in CHMP2B-FTD (p-value = 0.177, F-value = 2.036). Patients with recent symptom onset (<1 year) showed an increased duration of microstate D, whereas patients who had been symptomatic for longer periods (>2 years) showed decreased duration. Patients with CHMP2B-FTD present with executive dysfunction, and microstate D has previously been shown to be associated with the fronto-parietal network. The biphasic pattern may represent the pathophysiological changes in brain dynamics during neurodegeneration, which may apply to other neurodegenerative diseases.

Introduction

Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) is a common cause of early-onset dementia (Ratnavalli et al., 2002), and disease-causing mutations have been identified in a number of genes (Rohrer et al., 2009; Paulson and Igo, 2011). One of these genes is CHMP2B on chromosome 3 causing an autosomal dominantly inherited FTD previously named FTD-3, now CHMP2B-FTD, which has only been described in one large family globally. It is caused by a single base change, c.532-1G > C in the CHMP2B gene (Skibinski et al., 2005). The phenotype of FTD-3 patients is behavioral-variant-FTD, most often characterized by initial impairment in executive functions (working memory, attentional control, inhibition and mental flexibility) as the first and most pronounced symptom (Stokholm et al., 2013). Looking at the structural brain changes using MRI, a longitudinal study showed increased atrophy rates in the inferior temporal cortex, superior frontal cortex and the insular cortex when comparing mutation carriers to non-carriers (Eskildsen et al., 2009). In addition, when investigating metabolic changes as measured by 18F-fluro-deoxy-glucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET), involvement of the parietal areas was shown in pre-symptomatic mutation carriers, while changes in frontal areas appeared once the subjects became symptomatic (Johannsen et al., 2016). Despite a low sample size due to the rarity of the disease, the patients were all from the same family, and no studies have so far investigated the functional brain dynamics using quantitative electroencephalography (qEEG) in patients with CHMP2B-FTD.

In a transgenic mouse model of CHMP2B, a study has found short and long-range synaptic degeneration (Ghazi-Noori et al., 2012) and studies have also applied qEEG as a way to investigate functional brain dynamics (Stothart et al., 2016; Tait, 2019). Here, one study found that the changes were most pronounced the frontal areas (Tait, 2019) and suggested a compensatory mechanism of local synchrony to compensate for the breakdown of the cortical networks.

One type of qEEG marker that has been applied is spectral power or the squared amplitude of EEG rhythmic signal. Here, in patients with behavioral-variant FTD, studies have found conflicting results with some reporting an increased slowing (Stigsby et al., 1981; Besthorn et al., 1996; Yener et al., 1996; Passant et al., 2005), while another study found an absence of increased slow-wave activity, but a decrease of fast-wave activity (Lindau et al., 2003). In support of the latter, a recent study found that the peak frequency was not as affected in patients with FTD as seen in patients with AD (Goossens et al., 2017).

A method to study cortical brain networks using EEG is microstate analysis. This technique involves segmenting the EEG signal into a number of states (Lehmann et al., 1987) defined by spatial topographies. These functional states have been shown to be reliable over multiple recordings (Khanna et al., 2014). Furthermore, microstates have been linked to activity of resting state networks as measured with resting state fMRI (Van De Ville et al., 2010; Yuan et al., 2012). In FTD, a study investigated the changes in microstates in behavioral variant FTD and found changes in microstate C (Nishida et al., 2013), which has been linked to the insular-cingulate network (Britz et al., 2010). This finding may explain the changes in personality such as disinhibition and apathy. Since executive function is the main cognitive domain in CHMP2B-FTD, other cortical networks may be affected.

In the current exploratory study, we wanted to investigate changes in global brain dynamics using spectral power and microstates in a cohort of CHMP2B mutation carriers. This is unique since no study has investigated quantitative EEG markers in a genetically uniform cohort of FTD patients.

Materials and Methods

Study Population

The Danish CHMP2B-FTD family has been subject to extensive studies within the Frontotemporal dementia in Jutland Association (FReJA) collaboration (Gydesen et al., 2002). Clinical characteristics have been recorded in 45 cases of disease, providing information about disease course and neuropsychological changes. EEGs were obtained from a total of 16 participants: five symptomatic patients with CHMP2B-FTD, five pre-symptomatic CHMP2B mutation carriers, and six non-carrier family members (controls). As a measure of global cognitive function, Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination (ACE) (Mathuranath et al., 2000) was performed on all patients and three of the pre-symptomatic carriers. As it is impossible to define an exact time of onset, disease duration was based on the time where a close relative first noticed symptoms. All participants provided written informed consent. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Capital Region of Denmark (H-1-2012-041) and all methods were performed in accordance with the regulations.

EEG Recordings

EEG data were acquired at 200 Hz and recorded to a TrackIt Mk3TM (Lifelines Neurodiagnostic Systems, Illinois, United States) using WaveGuard caps with Ag/AgCl electrodes (ANT Neuro). This equipment was chosen since it was possible to bring along for recording at patients’ homes. The following channels were used: Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, C3, C4, T3, T5, T4, T6, P3, Pz, P4, O1, and O2. The impedance before recording was below 10 kOhm or if it was not possible to reach below 10 kOhm, the channel was noted for the subsequent pre-processing. The EEGs were recorded, alternating between 30 s periods each of eyes closed and eyes open. All participants were able to perform this task.

Pre-processing EEG

For the subsequent pre-processing and analysis, we used MATLAB (Mathworks, v2017b) and EEGLAB toolbox v13.6.5b (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). First, the data were imported, and the eyes-closed segments of EEGs were selected. To computationally locate the electrodes on the scalp, we used the DIPFIT toolbox (Oostenveld et al., 2011). The data were bandpass-filtered from 1–70 Hz using the pop_firws function in MATLAB with a filter order of two, and the Kaiser window parameter beta was estimated using a maximum passband ripple of 0.001. Afterward, the data was band-stop filtered from 45–55 Hz using the same settings. Next, the data were divided into 3-s epochs as has previously been applied, and the EEGs were visually inspected to remove excessive noise or artifacts. If one or more channels contained excessive noise, drift or a bad connection, they were interpolated using spherical interpolation. In no participants were more than three electrodes interpolated. Afterward, the EEGs were re-referenced to average reference (using the fullRankAveRef function), and independent component analysis was performed using the extended infomax algorithm (Lee et al., 1999). This was done for each file, and components which mostly contained eye movement or ECG artifacts, were removed. A maximum of two components were removed for each subject. The investigator was blinded to CHMP2B mutation and clinical status.

Spectral Power Calculations

To calculate spectral power, we calculated power across epochs using the in-built function spectopo from the EEGLAB toolbox. The window length was equal to the sampling rate of 200 Hz. The relative power was calculated in each of the following frequency bands: delta (1–3.99 Hz), theta (4–7.99 Hz), alpha (8–12.99 Hz), and beta (13–29.99 Hz) by dividing the power of each frequency bands with the total power. Afterward, we calculated global relative power by averaging across all channels for each frequency band for each subject. Absolute power was not included in the current analysis due to the large variability between subjects (Musaeus et al., 2018).

Microstates Analysis

The microstate analysis was performed using the Microstate EEGlab Toolbox (Poulsen et al., 2018). First, the EEGs were lowpass-filtered at 40 Hz. For each subject we began by extracting the first 2,500 electric field montages at global field power (GFP) peaks with a minimum peak distance of 10 ms. GFP peaks that exceeded two times the standard deviation of the GFPs of all maps were excluded. To identify topographic clusters within these data, we submitted all n × 2,500 electric potential topographies to a modified K-means clustering algorithm (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1995) using 300 repetitions. Polarity of the EEG topography was ignored (Lehmann, 1971; Wackermann et al., 1993; Pascual-Marqui et al., 1995). We chose to predefine the number of microstates as four to remain consistent with the majority of prior studies of EEG microstates (Khanna et al., 2014), and because four has been confirmed to generate reproducible maps (Khanna et al., 2015). A set of four global maps was generated (see Supplementary Figure 2) and back fitted to the whole EEG. To reduce noise, we rejected microstate segments shorter than 30 ms. This step was performed since we assumed that shorter segments were due to noise. After back-fitting the global maps, we calculated global explained variance (GEV) as defined in the Microstate EEGlab Toolbox (Poulsen et al., 2018), duration, occurrence, and coverage for EEG files. Here, the duration was defined as the average time for each map to be present before transitioning to another map. Occurrence was defined as the average number of times a microstate occurred each second. Coverage was defined as the total percent of the EEG for which a microstate was accounted for. GEV was defined as the variance of EEG activity explained by all four microstates.

Statistics

All statistics were performed in MATLAB (vR2017b). To compare gender, we performed Fisher’s exact test due to the low sample size. The tests were performed between controls and pre-symptomatic carriers, between controls and CHMP2B-FTD, and between pre-symptomatic carriers and CHMP2B-FTD. For age and number of epochs, we performed a Kruskal–Wallis test, and for comparing ACE between pre-symptomatic carriers and patients, we used Wilcoxon rank sum test.

When comparing relative power and microstate features between subjects, we first log-transformed the data due to the non-normal distribution. Although not significantly different between the groups, gender and age have been shown to affect microstates features (Tomescu et al., 2018) and were therefore used as covariates in the ANCOVA (Gruner, 2020) analyses. In addition, we performed ANCOVA analysis between the patients and controls using the same covariates as mentioned above. Lastly, to understand the effect size, we calculated Cohen’s d for the occurrence of microstate D between patients and controls with the data not being log-transformed.

Due to low sample size, we did not perform any statistical analysis when comparing patients with short duration of symptoms (<1 year), and patients who had been symptomatic for a longer period (>2 years).

Results

Demographics

Demographics are listed in Table 1. No significant difference in age was found between controls, pre-symptomatic carriers and patients (p-value = 0.110) although pre-symptomatic carriers were generally younger than clinically affected patients. There were no significant differences in gender between the three groups. The average disease duration for CHMP2B-FTD was 3.4 years (standard deviation: 2.73). We did not find a significant difference in the number of 3-s epochs (p-value = 0.807).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of study participants.

Spectral Power

We did not find any significant differences in the electrode-to-electrode comparisons. Furthermore, we did not find any significant differences in global relative power, but there was a trend toward an increase in relative alpha power for patients and especially for pre-symptomatic mutation carriers (p-value = 0.099, F-value = 2.881) as compared to controls (see Supplementary Figure 3).

Microstates

There was no significant difference in the total GEV between controls, pre-symptomatic mutation carriers and patients (p-value = 0.271, F-value = 1.446), with an average GEV across groups of 49.67%. No significant differences were found for the microstate features between the three groups, see Table 2. However, a trend was found for lower occurrence of microstate D in pre-symptomatic carriers and patients compared to healthy controls (p-value = 0.177, F-value = 2.036). Between symptomatic and controls we also found a trend for the occurrence of microstate D (p-value = 0.064) and a large effect size as measured with Cohen’s d (d = 0.940).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Microstate features, including duration, occurrence, and coverage for microstates A-D, and the p-value and F-values comparing FTD-3, pre-symptomatic carriers, and HC.

When investigating the difference between microstate features between the two patients with short duration of symptoms (<1 year), and the three patients who had been symptomatic for a longer period (>2 years), we found an increase in features for microstate D and decreased coverage for the rest of the microstates (see Figure 1 and Supplementary Table 4).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Bar graphs showing the mean and standard deviation of duration, occurrence, and coverage for microstate D and the mean of the other microstates (A, B, and C) for the controls (n = 6), pre-symptomatic carriers (n = 5), patients with CHMP2B-FTD with recent (<1 year) onset of symptoms (n = 2), and patients who had been symptomatic for a longer (>2 years) period (n = 3).

Discussion

In the current exploratory study, we investigated the changes in global brain dynamics using spectral power and microstates in a cohort of patients with CHMP2B-FTD. A trend was found between the three groups for occurrence of microstate D (p-value = 0.177, F-value = 2.036) with a large effect size (d = 0.940) between the patients and controls. When dividing the patients based on disease duration, we found a biphasic pattern for microstate D with increased microstate features in patients with recent onset of symptoms (<1 year), while patients who had been symptomatic for longer (>2 years) showed decreased network activity in microstate D. This finding suggests an increased activity of the underlying network in the early stages of the disease. Looking at relative power, we found a trend in global relative power in the alpha band, which was most pronounced in pre-symptomatic carriers (p-value = 0.099, F-value = 2.881).

One study has investigated changes in microstates in patients with behavioral variant FTD and found the duration of microstate C significantly lower compared to controls (Nishida et al., 2013). Microstate C has previously been associated with the insular-cingulate network (Britz et al., 2010), which has been shown to be affected in behavioral variant FTD (Zhou et al., 2010). In our current study we found that microstate D showed a trend toward a decrease in CHMP2B-FTD as compared to controls. A possible explanation for these conflicting findings could be that patients with behavioral variant FTD usually present with changes in personality and behavior, while patients with CHMP2B-FTD initially present with executive dysfunction while behavioral symptoms appear at a later stage of the disease (Stokholm et al., 2013). To understand the connection between microstates and the spatial changes in the networks, studies have investigated the connection between microstate D and both the blood oxygen level-dependent signal and resting state networks measured with fMRI (Britz et al., 2010; Musso et al., 2010; Yuan et al., 2012) as well as using source localization (Custo et al., 2017). For all the aforementioned methods, microstate D has been associated with the fronto-parietal network, which has been shown to be related to executive function (Reineberg and Banich, 2016). Our finding that patients with CHMP2B-FTD show a distinct pattern of disruption of the microstate D is in accordance with a frontoparietal dysfunction presenting as the clinical feature of reduced executive function.

Since our participants are all from the same family, and the CHMP2B-FTD is due to a specific genetic mutation, we would have expected that all patients showed a decrease in microstate D features. However, diverging results have also been found in studies of EEG microstates for microstate A in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Here, some studies comparing Alzheimer’s disease and controls found a shorter average duration of microstates in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (Dierks et al., 1997; Strik et al., 1997; Stevens and Kircher, 1998), but this may be due to using temporal windowing instead of clustering analysis for microstates. Other studies, which for the most part use clustering analysis, report a longer duration (Ihl et al., 1993; Schumacher et al., 2019; Smailovic et al., 2019; Musaeus et al., 2020; Tait et al., 2020). Also, more recent studies using clustering analysis did not find any significant differences (Nishida et al., 2013) or an increased occurrence and coverage for microstate A (Musaeus et al., 2019). In addition, some studies have found an altered features of microstate D in patients with AD (Smailovic et al., 2019; Tait et al., 2020) or that longer duration of microstate D may be a predictor of conversion from MCI to AD (Musaeus et al., 2019). Changes in microstate D has directly been associated with the disruption of the fronto-parietal network (Musaeus et al., 2019).

A possible explanation is that the diverging findings in patients with AD, may reflect the changes in microstates at different stages of the neurodegenerative disease. In our study, patients with recent onset of symptoms (<1 year) had an increase in microstate D features as compared to a decrease in patients who had symptoms for longer (>2 years), see Figure 1. In support of this, a study found that patients with CHMP2B-FTD showed an increase in cerebral blood flow at the initial phase of the disease (Gydesen et al., 1987), which may be a sign of a compensatory upregulation and possibly associated with functional changes as measured with EEG. However, further studies are needed to understand the association between cerebral blood flow and microstate changes. Furthermore, the pre-symptomatic carriers did not show signs of cognitive decline as measured with ACE (see Table 1) but a small increase in duration for microstate D was found in these participants (see Table 2). In addition, studies investigating transgenic mouse models with mutant CHMP2B showed that changes were most pronounced in the frontal areas and suggested a compensatory mechanism of local synchrony to compensate for the breakdown of the cortical networks (Tait, 2019). We therefore propose a biphasic model of cortical network dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases as seen in microstates (see Figure 2). Here, in the pre-symptomatic phase the affected cortical network (i.e., microstate D) displays a compensation phase with increased activity in the affected network (Figure 2I). This continues until the network can no longer compensate, and consequently the patient becomes symptomatic (Figure 2II). This is followed by the decompensation phase (Figure 2III) where the network can no longer compensate for the neurodegeneration. In the case of CHMP2B-FTD, the frontal and parietal brain areas are affected, and the features of microstate D starts to decline. These findings are of course preliminary and larger prospective studies are needed to support this model including other types of neurodegenerative diseases. However, the CHMP2B family represents a homogenous, unique and powerful cohort to generate hypotheses which may be verified in other cohorts including both familial and sporadic cases with neurodegenerative diseases.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Proposed model of cortical network dysfunction as seen in neurodegenerative diseases. When the neurodegenerative disease starts to affect a cortical network, (I) the compensation phase shows increased activity in the affected network (microstate D). This continues until the network can no longer compensate and then the patient starts to present symptoms in (II) the symptomatic phase (time of referral). In (III) the decompensation phase, the network can no longer compensate for the damages, and the activity falls below the previous level.

For global relative power, we found a trend in the alpha band (p-value = 0.099, F-value = 2.881). In general, alpha power has been associated with attention (Klimesch, 2012; Benedek et al., 2014), and patients with CHMP2B-FTD show problems with attentional control early in the disease. However, no significant differences were found, which is in line with previous studies finding diverging results (Stigsby et al., 1981; Besthorn et al., 1996; Yener et al., 1996; Passant et al., 2005) and the relative alpha was not lower in patients with CHMP2B-FTD. Future studies should assess the spectral power changes associated with FTD over time.

We chose to extract four microstates since it is the most commonly reported number of microstates and have been shown to be reliable (Khanna et al., 2014) in spite of the low sample size. We found that the algorithm was robust, and the main findings could be replicated (see Supplementary Material). Furthermore, even though the GEV was not significantly different between the three groups, it was low compared to other studies [normally reporting a GEV of >70% (Michel and Koenig, 2018)]. This could be due to previous studies recording EEGs on younger participants, or that we back fitted the microstates based on concatenated GFP peaks for all participants and did not calculate individual maps.

We acknowledge the limitations of the small sample size, but since the disease is a very rare genetic condition only described in this unique Danish family, it was not possible to recruit more subjects. Overall, the statistical analyses should be carefully interpreted due to the small sample size, which limits the statistical power to observe changes with confidence, However, we found a large effect size (d = 0.940), which may point to a difference between the groups. Furthermore, we did not have recent MRI or FDG-PET scans for all participants, which limits our ability to investigate the connection between structural, metabolic and microstate features. In addition, the lack of neuropsychological testing limits us from understanding the relationship between the clinical presentation and microstate features.

Conclusion

In the present exploratory study, we found evidence of changes in microstate D in patient with CHMP2B-FTD. Microstate D has previously been shown to be associated with the fronto-parietal network, which is strongly associated with executive function. Furthermore, we found a pattern of increased activity in microstate D when the symptoms had been present for <1 year, whereas the patients who were symptomatic for >2 years showed decreased network activity. We propose a biphasic model for interpreting microstate changes in CHMP2B-FTD, which may be applicable to other neurodegenerative diseases and may explain the conflicting results of network activity found in other neurodegenerative diseases.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because of Danish regulations. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to christian.sandoee.musaeus@regionh.dk.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The Ethics Committee of the Capital Region of Denmark (H-1-2012-041). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

The FReJA Consortium

Gydesen Sd, Brown Je, Isaacs AMf, Collinge Jg, Gade Ah, Englund Ei, Fisher Ef, Nielsen TTa, Thusgaard Tj, and Holm Ik.

a Danish Dementia Research Centre (DDRC), Rigshospitalet and Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark

d Psychiatric Centre Ballerup, Copenhagen University Hospital, Ballerup, Denmark

e Department of Neurology, Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge, United Kingdom

f Department of Neurodegenerative Disease and UK Dementia Research Institute at UCL, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, United Kingdom

g Institute of Prion Diseases and MRC Prion Unit at UCL, Courtauld Building, London, United Kingdom

h Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark

i Department of Pathology, University Hospital, Lund, Sweden

j Parkvaenget Nursing Home, Holstebro, Denmark

k Department of Pathology, Aalborg University Hospital, Aalborg, Denmark

Author Contributions

CM, JP, PR, and JN initiated the study, recruited patients, and gathered patient data. TK, MH, and TB were responsible for equipment including recording setup. CM and PR analyzed the data. CM wrote the first draft of the article. All authors have edited and critically revised the manuscript.

Funding

The study was supported by the Novo Nordisk Foundation. The funder of the study had no role in its design, in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data, in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

This work has been possible only due to the continued dedication and support from CHMP2B-FTD family members. This has been facilitated through more than 20 years by the FReJA collaboration.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2021.714220/full#supplementary-material

References

Benedek, M., Schickel, R. J., Jauk, E., Fink, A., and Neubauer, A. C. (2014). Alpha power increases in right parietal cortex reflects focused internal attention. Neuropsychologia 56, 393–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besthorn, C., Sattel, H., Hentschel, F., Daniel, S., Zerfass, R., and Forstl, H. (1996). Quantitative EEG in frontal lobe dementia. J. Neural Transm. Suppl. 47, 169–181. doi: 10.1007/978-3-7091-6892-9_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britz, J., Van De Ville, D., and Michel, C. M. (2010). BOLD correlates of EEG topography reveal rapid resting-state network dynamics. Neuroimage 52, 1162–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Custo, A., Van De Ville, D., Wells, W. M., Tomescu, M. I., Brunet, D., and Michel, C. M. (2017). Electroencephalographic resting-state networks: source localization of microstates. Brain Connect. 7, 671–682. doi: 10.1089/brain.2016.0476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dierks, T., Jelic, V., Julin, P., Maurer, K., Wahlund, L. O., Almkvist, O., et al. (1997). EEG-microstates in mild memory impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: possible association with disturbed information processing. J. Neural Transm. 104, 483–495. doi: 10.1007/bf01277666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eskildsen, S. F., Ostergaard, L. R., Rodell, A. B., Ostergaard, L., Nielsen, J. E., Isaacs, A. M., et al. (2009). Cortical volumes and atrophy rates in FTD-3 CHMP2B mutation carriers and related non-carriers. Neuroimage 45, 713–721. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghazi-Noori, S., Froud, K. E., Mizielinska, S., Powell, C., Smidak, M., Fernandez De Marco, M., et al. (2012). Progressive neuronal inclusion formation and axonal degeneration in CHMP2B mutant transgenic mice. Brain 135, 819–832. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goossens, J., Laton, J., Van Schependom, J., Gielen, J., Struyfs, H., Van Mossevelde, S., et al. (2017). EEG dominant frequency peak differentiates between Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal lobar degeneration. J. Alzheimers Dis. 55, 53–58. doi: 10.3233/jad-160188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gruner, W. (2020). MANCOVAN, MATLAB Central File Exchange. Available online at: https://www.mathworks.com/matlabcentral/fileexchange/27014-mancovan (accessed November 2017).

Google Scholar

Gydesen, S., Brown, J. M., Brun, A., Chakrabarti, L., Gade, A., Johannsen, P., et al. (2002). Chromosome 3 linked frontotemporal dementia (FTD-3). Neurology 59, 1585–1594.

Google Scholar

Gydesen, S., Hagen, S., Klinken, L., Abelskov, J., and Sørensen, S. A. (1987). Neuropsychiatric studies in a family with presenile dementia different from Alzheimer and Pick disease. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 76, 276–284. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1987.tb02896.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ihl, R., Dierks, T., Froelich, L., Martin, E. M., and Maurer, K. (1993). Segmentation of the spontaneous EEG in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychobiology 27, 231–236. doi: 10.1159/000118986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johannsen, P., Stokholm, J., Roos, P., Law, I., Isaacs, A. M., Brown, J., et al. (2016). Shift in cerebral pet glucose metabolism in frontotemporal dementia linked to chromosome 3 (FTD-3) from the presymptomatic to symtomatic stage. Alzheimers Dement. 12:1090.

Google Scholar

Khanna, A., Pascual-Leone, A., and Farzan, F. (2014). Reliability of resting-state microstate features in electroencephalography. PLoS One 9:e114163. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khanna, A., Pascual-Leone, A., Michel, C. M., and Farzan, F. (2015). Microstates in resting-state EEG: current status and future directions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 49, 105–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (2012). Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 606–617. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, T. W., Girolami, M., and Sejnowski, T. J. (1999). Independent component analysis using an extended infomax algorithm for mixed subgaussian and supergaussian sources. Neural Comput. 11, 417–441. doi: 10.1162/089976699300016719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, D. (1971). Multichannel topography of human alpha EEG fields. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 31, 439–449. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(71)90165-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, D., Ozaki, H., and Pal, I. (1987). EEG alpha map series: brain micro-states by space-oriented adaptive segmentation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 67, 271–288. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90025-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindau, M., Jelic, V., Johansson, S. E., Andersen, C., Wahlund, L. O., and Almkvist, O. (2003). Quantitative EEG abnormalities and cognitive dysfunctions in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 15, 106–114. doi: 10.1159/000067973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathuranath, P. S., Nestor, P. J., Berrios, G. E., Rakowicz, W., and Hodges, J. R. (2000). A brief cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 55, 1613–1620. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000434309.85312.19

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, C. M., and Koenig, T. (2018). EEG microstates as a tool for studying the temporal dynamics of whole-brain neuronal networks: a review. Neuroimage 180, 577–593. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musaeus, C. S., Engedal, K., Høgh, P., Jelic, V., Khanna, A. R., Kjær, T. W., et al. (2020). Changes in the left temporal microstate are a sign of cognitive decline in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Behav. 10:e01630.

Google Scholar

Musaeus, C. S., Engedal, K., Hogh, P., Jelic, V., Morup, M., Naik, M., et al. (2018). EEG theta power is an early marker of cognitive decline in dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 64, 1359–1371. doi: 10.3233/jad-180300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musaeus, C. S., Nielsen, M. S., and Høgh, P. (2019). Microstates as disease and progression markers in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Front. Neurosci. 13:563. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Musso, F., Brinkmeyer, J., Mobascher, A., Warbrick, T., and Winterer, G. (2010). Spontaneous brain activity and EEG microstates. a novel EEG/fMRI analysis approach to explore resting-state networks. Neuroimage 52, 1149–1161. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishida, K., Morishima, Y., Yoshimura, M., Isotani, T., Irisawa, S., Jann, K., et al. (2013). EEG microstates associated with salience and frontoparietal networks in frontotemporal dementia, schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 1106–1114. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., Maris, E., and Schoffelen, J. M. (2011). FieldTrip: open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2011:156869.

Google Scholar

Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Michel, C. M., and Lehmann, D. (1995). Segmentation of brain electrical activity into microstates: model estimation and validation. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 42, 658–665. doi: 10.1109/10.391164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passant, U., Rosen, I., Gustafson, L., and Englund, E. (2005). The heterogeneity of frontotemporal dementia with regard to initial symptoms, qEEG and neuropathology. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 20, 983–988. doi: 10.1002/gps.1388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulson, H. L., and Igo, I. (2011). Genetics of dementia. Semin. Neurol. 31, 449–460.

Google Scholar

Poulsen, A. T., Pedroni, A., Langer, N., and Hansen, L. K. (2018). Microstate EEGlab toolbox: an introductory guide. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/289850

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratnavalli, E., Brayne, C., Dawson, K., and Hodges, J. R. (2002). The prevalence of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 58, 1615–1621.

Google Scholar

Reineberg, A. E., and Banich, M. T. (2016). Functional connectivity at rest is sensitive to individual differences in executive function: a network analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 2959–2975. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohrer, J. D., Guerreiro, R., Vandrovcova, J., Uphill, J., Reiman, D., Beck, J., et al. (2009). The heritability and genetics of frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology 73, 1451–1456.

Google Scholar

Schumacher, J., Peraza, L. R., Firbank, M., Thomas, A. J., Kaiser, M., Gallagher, P., et al. (2019). Dysfunctional brain dynamics and their origin in lewy body dementia. Brain 142, 1767–1782. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skibinski, G., Parkinson, N. J., Brown, J. M., Chakrabarti, L., Lloyd, S. L., Hummerich, H., et al. (2005). Mutations in the endosomal ESCRTIII-complex subunit CHMP2B in frontotemporal dementia. Nat. Genet. 37, 806–808. doi: 10.1038/ng1609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smailovic, U., Koenig, T., Laukka, E. J., Kalpouzos, G., Andersson, T., Winblad, B., et al. (2019). EEG time signature in Alzheimer’s disease: functional brain networks falling apart. NeuroImage 24:102046.

Google Scholar

Stevens, A., and Kircher, T. (1998). Cognitive decline unlike normal aging is associated with alterations of EEG temporo-spatial characteristics. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 248, 259–266. doi: 10.1007/s004060050047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stigsby, B., Johannesson, G., and Ingvar, D. H. (1981). Regional EEG analysis and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer’s and Pick’s diseases. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 51, 537–547. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(81)90230-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stokholm, J., Teasdale, T. W., Johannsen, P., Nielsen, J. E., Nielsen, T. T., Isaacs, A., et al. (2013). Cognitive impairment in the preclinical stage of dementia in FTD-3 CHMP2B mutation carriers: a longitudinal prospective study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 84, 170–176. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-303813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stothart, G., Petkov, G., Kazanina, N., Goodfellow, M., Tait, L., and Brown, J. (2016). Graph-theoretical measures provide translational markers of large-scale brain network disruption in human dementia patients and animal models of dementia. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 108:71. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.07.232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strik, W. K., Chiaramonti, R., Muscas, G. C., Paganini, M., Mueller, T. J., Fallgatter, A. J., et al. (1997). Decreased EEG microstate duration and anteriorisation of the brain electrical fields in mild and moderate dementia of the Alzheimer type. Psychiatry Res 75, 183–191. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(97)00054-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tait, L. (2019). Multi-Scale Mathematical Modelling of Brain Networks in Alzheimer’s Disease (Doctoral Thesis). Exeter: University of Exeter.

Google Scholar

Tait, L., Tamagnini, F., Stothart, G., Barvas, E., Monaldini, C., Frusciante, R., et al. (2020). EEG microstate complexity for aiding early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Sci. Rep. 10:17627.

Google Scholar

Tomescu, M. I., Rihs, T. A., Rochas, V., Hardmeier, M., Britz, J., Allali, G., et al. (2018). From swing to cane: sex differences of EEG resting-state temporal patterns during maturation and aging. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 31, 58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van De Ville, D., Britz, J., and Michel, C. M. (2010). EEG microstate sequences in healthy humans at rest reveal scale-free dynamics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 18179–18184. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007841107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wackermann, J., Lehmann, D., Michel, C. M., and Strik, W. K. (1993). Adaptive segmentation of spontaneous EEG map series into spatially defined microstates. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 14, 269–283. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(93)90041-m

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yener, G. G., Leuchter, A. F., Jenden, D., Read, S. L., Cummings, J. L., and Miller, B. L. (1996). Quantitative EEG in frontotemporal dementia. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 27, 61–68. doi: 10.1177/155005949602700204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, H., Zotev, V., Phillips, R., Drevets, W. C., and Bodurka, J. (2012). Spatiotemporal dynamics of the brain at rest–exploring EEG microstates as electrophysiological signatures of BOLD resting state networks. Neuroimage 60, 2062–2072. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, J., Greicius, M. D., Gennatas, E. D., Growdon, M. E., Jang, J. Y., Rabinovici, G. D., et al. (2010). Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 133, 1352–1367. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: EEG, microstates, spectral power, FTD, Frontotemporal dementia, CHMP2B, microstates analysis

Citation: Musaeus CS, Pedersen JS, Kjær TW, Johannsen P, Waldemar G, Haverberg MJN, Bacher T, Nielsen JE, Roos P and The FReJA Consortium (2021) Cortical Frontoparietal Network Dysfunction in CHMP2B-Frontotemporal Dementia. Front. Aging Neurosci. 13:714220. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.714220

Received: 24 May 2021; Accepted: 09 August 2021;
Published: 13 September 2021.

Edited by:

Claudia Jacova, Pacific University, United States

Reviewed by:

Jose Felix Moruno-Manchon, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, United States
Enrico Premi, University of Brescia, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Musaeus, Pedersen, Kjær, Johannsen, Waldemar, Haverberg, Bacher, Nielsen, Roos and The FReJA Consortium. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christian Sandøe Musaeus, christian.sandoee.musaeus@regionh.dk

Present address: Peter Johannsen, Medical and Science, Global Development, Novo Nordisk, Søborg, Denmark

Download