Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Neurosci., 10 January 2020
Sec. Neuropharmacology
This article is part of the Research Topic Novel Psychoactive Drugs - The Saga Continues… View all 16 articles

Virtual Reality (VR) in Assessment and Treatment of Addictive Disorders: A Systematic Review

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Hôpital Saint-Antoine, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France
  • 2Jeanne d'Arc Hospital, INICEA Group, Saint-Mandé, France

Background: Substance Use Disorder (SUD) and behavioral addictions are common and require a multidisciplinary approach. New technologies like Virtual Reality could have the potential to improve assessment and treatment of these disorders.

Objective: In the present paper, we therefore present an overview of Virtual Reality (Head Mounted Devices) in the field of addiction medicine for craving assessment and treatment.

Method: We conducted a systematic review by querying PubMed database for the titles of articles published up to March 2019 with the terms [virtual] AND [addictive] OR [addiction] OR [substance] OR [alcohol] OR [cocaine] OR [cannabis] OR [opioid] OR [tobacco] OR [nicotine] OR [methamphetamine] OR [gaming] OR [gambling].

Results: We screened 319 abstracts and analyzed 37 articles, dividing them into two categories, the first for assessment of cue reactivity (craving, psychophysiological response and attention to cue) and the second for intervention, each drug (nicotine, cocaine, alcohol, cannabis, gambling) being detailed within each category.

Conclusions: This overview suggest that VR provide benefits in the assessment and treatment of substance use disorders and behavior addictions and achieve high levels of ecological validity. While, craving provocation in VR is effective across addiction disorders, treatments based exclusively on virtual exposure to drug related cues as shown heterogenous results.

Introduction

Substance Use Disorder (SUD) and behavioral addictions are prevalent in many countries (Degenhardt et al., 2017) and require a multidisciplinary approach. Unfortunately, only a minority (7.1%) of patients, even in high income countries, receives adequate treatment. Individuals diagnosed with SUD experience several relapses after interventions and a lower quality of life because of the chronic nature of these disorders (Degenhardt et al., 2017). Therefore, there is an urgent need to conduct more research to expand assessment and treatment approaches. Virtual reality (VR) is emerging as one of the technological keys, it is increasingly accessible, and easy-to-use, and has recently attracted attention because of its potential utility for individuals with SUD (Carter and Tiffany, 1999).

VR is a computer-generated simulation that is a set of images and sounds that represents a real place or situation, which can be interacted with, in a seemingly real or physical way by a person using special electronic equipment. It can transmit visual, auditory, and various sensations to users through a headset to make them feel as if they are in a virtual or imagined environment.

Cue reactivity, which is based on classical and instrumental conditioning theory, involves conditioned response such as craving, psychophysiological response (heart rate, skin conductance…), attention bias and drug seeking behaviors triggered by stimuli previously associated with drug use (Ferreri et al., 2018). Traditional cue reactivity studies (scripts, photographs, videos and objects related to drug use), have allowed for a better understanding of situations leading to continued use and factors triggering relapse (Conklin and Tiffany, 2002). However, many limitations exist: lack of standardization, control and generalization, lack of contextual, and complex cues variety resulting in limited ecological validity and decreasing extinction probability through cue-exposure therapy. Therefore, because VR is immersive, both the environment and the perceptual stimuli can be modulated to trigger and assess pathological behaviors or sensations (e.g., craving), as well as to evaluate behavioral responses to a given situation that can elicit distress. Patients cans learn how to cope with their problems better. Beyond the use for addictive psychopathologies it has been shown to be useful for other psychiatric disorders by Park et al. (2019).

Moreover, VR has the potential to ameliorate many issues like craving assessment; active treatment of craving using cue exposure therapy (CET) or cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT)-driven techniques applied in real time. VR may also facilitate the link between clinician and patients and improve our understanding of addictive behavior.

Until recently, VR was limited by its cost and by the quality of the multimedia content. There has been a recent democratization of these systems (Playsatation4 VR, Oculus Rift, etc.) concomitant with the video game industry's growing interest in this technology. Decreasing costs and increasing power are making it useful for performing an ecological assessment of cognition, emotions, and behavior in real-time.

The purpose of this systematic review is to evaluate the usefulness and efficacy of immersive VR in cue reactivity assessment and craving management for patients undergoing SUD or behavioral addictions. To this end, the proposed systematic review will answer the following questions:

- When compared to VR neutral stimuli, are VR cues able to elicit cue reactivity in adult patients suffering from addictive disorders? Is there an advantage of using VR cue reactivity over traditional cue reactivity methods?

- Can VR be used as an effective tool for craving reduction compared to standard therapy in this aforementioned population?

Method

Eligibility Criteria

To identify appropriate resources and search for relevant evidence. We used a PICOS framework, to form a well-focused question and facilitate the literature search (Schardt et al., 2007).

We conduct a systematic review by querying PubMed and Embase using the MeSH terms and keywords [virtual] AND [addictive] OR [addiction] OR [substance] OR [alcohol] OR [cocaine] OR [cannabis] OR [opioid] OR [tobacco] OR [nicotine] OR [methamphetamine] OR [gaming] OR [gambling]. TS and TB screened 471 articles, and 37 were then analyzed and divided into two categories, the first for assessment of cue reactivity (craving, psychophysiological response and attention to cue) and the second for treatment, each drug being detailed within each category.

See PRISMA diagram (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram.

Results

Conceptual Overview

A brief description of the concepts underlying virtual reality (VR) and cue features in the field of addiction is summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of concepts underlying Virtual Reality and type of cue.

Assessment

Studies and results described below are summarized in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Clinical trial on Virtual Reality (VR) assessment of craving in addiction.

Nicotine

Seventeen studies (n = 465) assessed cue reactivity after virtual nicotine-related cues in adult smokers. Nicotine addiction criteria were reported in 13 studies, using DSM 4 (Guze, 1995) and/or Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence criteria (Heatherton et al., 1991). Dependence severity was disparate across 8 studies, ranging from low to high (Lee et al., 2003, 2005; Traylor et al., 2008, 2009, 2011; Acker and MacKillop, 2013; Gamito et al., 2014; Thompson-Lake et al., 2015). Virtual reality exposure was controlled by a randomized healthy control group in 2 studies (Traylor et al., 2011; Gamito et al., 2014) and active 2D image comparators in 2 others (Lee et al., 2003, 2005). In most of the remaining studies, virtual nicotine cues were controlled by virtual neutral environments with random exposure protocols. Virtual environments included multisensory exposure with visual, auditory and olfactory stimuli in 8 studies (Carter et al., 2008; Traylor et al., 2008, 2009, 2011; Kaganoff et al., 2012; Acker and MacKillop, 2013; García-Rodríguez et al., 2013; Thompson-Lake et al., 2015) and visual and auditory environment for others (Ferrer-García et al., 2010; Paris et al., 2011; Pericot-Valverde et al., 2011; Acker and MacKillop, 2013; García-Rodríguez et al., 2013; Thompson-Lake et al., 2015).

Craving

All studies investigating craving (n = 445) in VR showed craving induction in adult smokers. Various cues (proximal: lighter, ashtray, pack of cigarettes, contextual: convenience store, party, complexes: smokers at a party or at a pub, having lunch at home) were associated with craving induction.

In most cases (17–24, 26, 27, 29, 32, 33), complex cues were evaluated and often compared to other types of cues (Bordnick et al., 2005; Traylor et al., 2008; Paris et al., 2011; Pericot-Valverde et al., 2011; Kaganoff et al., 2012; Gamito et al., 2014; Thompson-Lake et al., 2015). Some results suggested greater craving induction following complex cues compared to proximal (Thompson-Lake et al., 2015) or contextual cues (Gamito et al., 2014). Pericot-Valverde et al. (2011) suggested that social pressure in the form of virtual avatar could induce a faster craving increase in a complex-cue environment, although no difference in overall craving intensity was found. Conversely, García-Rodríguez et al. (2013) showed that smoking a virtual cigarette could act as a stronger proximal cue on craving induction compared to complex situations (García-Rodríguez et al., 2013). Altogether, 3 out of 7 studies failed to find differences in craving induction between types of cues and there are several possible explanations for these results. First of all, the limited use of random exposure protocol could have exposed craving measurements to a report effect, which therefore could limit cues comparability; only one study truly controlled this bias (Thompson-Lake et al., 2015). Secondly, most studies performed craving assessment using single-item visual analog scales, reducing craving to a single dimension, and being then more sensitive to the ceiling effect. Conversely, multidimensional scales can highlight interesting craving differences by assessing it on various motivational dimensions using the Questionnaire of Smoking Urges (QSU) (Thompson-Lake et al., 2015) or through a neuro-economic assessment of craving as proposed by Acker and MacKillop (2013) which found subjects more likely to spend money on cigarettes, continue to smoke even at higher cost and are less sensitive to cigarette prices. Finally, Traylor et al. showed no difference in nicotine craving between subjects who are nicotine and alcohol dependent vs. nicotine deprendent only subjects after exposure to virtual complex environments (Traylor et al., 2011). The relationship between craving induction and dependence levels in VR were mixed and understudied with only one positive result showing predictive value of dependence and withdrawal scores on craving intensity (Thompson-Lake et al., 2015), while no correlation between craving and cigarette consumption was found in Ferrer-García et al. (2010), this possibly reflects a lower nicotine dependence in the smoker group. Surprisingly, no studies evaluated the effect of sensorial diversity on craving, which was supposed to increase immersion, especially since presence and craving levels were correlated in one study (Ferrer-García et al., 2010).

Physiological response

The physiological response to virtual nicotine-related cues was assessed in 5 studies (n = 145 participants) (Bordnick et al., 2005; Lee et al., 2005; García-Rodríguez et al., 2012, 2013; Thompson-Lake et al., 2015). Results are inconsistant. On the one hand, proximal and complex cues triggered increased skin conductance (Bordnick et al., 2005) and heart rate (García-Rodríguez et al., 2013; Thompson-Lake et al., 2015) compared to neutral environments in smokers. On the other hand, complex cues were associated with decreased heart rate and no change in skin resistance in smokers compared to non-smokers (García-Rodríguez et al., 2012). Ultimately, these contradictory findings may reflect a low nicotine dependence in the smoker group. Temperature was assessed in one study only (García-Rodríguez et al., 2012) and they found that its unfrequent increase in only 2 out of the 7 environments. Finally, one functional MRI study showed a pre-frontal cortex activation in response to the VR nicotine stimuli compared to neutral stimuli (Lee et al., 2005).

Attention to cue

3 studies evaluated the impact of virtual nicotine-related cues on attentional bias by using the self-administered questionnaires (ACVAS) or the Smoking Attention Scale (SAS) (Traylor et al., 2009; Kaganoff et al., 2012; Thompson-Lake et al., 2015) (n = 102). All studies reported a significant increase of attentional bias compared to virtual neutral environments. Adding olfactory nicotine cues were surprisingly not associated with greater attention to cue in Traylor et al. (2009) study. One study showed the feasibility of using implicit measurement of attentional bias in VR through eye-tracking and found greater ocular fixations on nicotine cues in smokers compared to non-smokers (Gamito et al., 2014).

Alcohol

Five studies (n = 101) assessed cue reactivity after presentation of virtual alcohol-related cues in adults suffering from alcohol use disorder (Bordnick et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2008; Ryan et al., 2010; Traylor et al., 2011; Kim and Lee, 2015). Diagnosis criteria were reported in 4 studies (Bordnick et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2008; Traylor et al., 2011; Kim and Lee, 2015) using DSM 4 (Guze, 1995) or Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Scale [AUDIT (Saunders et al., 1993)]. Dependence level was disparate and ranged from intermediate to severe dependence [AUDIT or Alcohol Dependence Scale (ADS) (Kivlahan et al., 1989)] in 3 studies (Lee et al., 2008; Traylor et al., 2011; Kim and Lee, 2015). Virtual alcohol-related cues were controlled with virtual neutral environment in each study and randomly exposed in 3 studies. Four studies included a control group (Lee et al., 2008; Ryan et al., 2010; Traylor et al., 2011; Kim and Lee, 2015) of which one was randomized (Traylor et al., 2011). Virtual environments included a multisensory exposure with visual, auditory and olfactory stimuli in 3 studies (Bordnick et al., 2008; Ryan et al., 2010; Traylor et al., 2011) and visual and auditory stimuli in 2 studies (Lee et al., 2008; Kim and Lee, 2015).

Craving

All studies investigating craving (n = 93) showed its induction after VR cue exposure (Bordnick et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2008; Ryan et al., 2010; Traylor et al., 2011). Cues usually associated with alcohol consumption (proximal: bottle of alcohol, contextual: bar, complex: party with alcohol) were those inducing craving. All studies investigated craving through single-item VAS. Three studies (Bordnick et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2008; Ryan et al., 2010) studied social interaction impact on craving. Bordnick et al. (2008) found that social pressure (such as offering a cigarette in a party) in addition to complex cues showed no difference in craving level whereas negative social experiences were associated to craving reduction in subjects with alcohol use disorders. Lee et al. (2008) found similar results regarding social pressure in alcohol dependent subjects; unlike with social drinkers for which craving was increased, suggesting a greater sensitivity to the social context in this population (Ryan et al., 2010). Finally, Traylor et al. assessed cross-cue reactivity and didn't find differences in alcohol craving after nicotine-related cue exposure among nicotine and alcohol dependent subjects compared to nicotine only dependent subjects (Traylor et al., 2011). These results may however have been limited by the duration of alcohol abstinence (12 h) which could have result in a ceiling effect, reflected by a greater baseline craving.

Attention to cue

Two studies assessed attentional bias upon virtual alcohol-related cue exposure using the self-report questionnaire Alcohol Attention Scale (AAS) (Bordnick et al., 2008; Ryan et al., 2010). Attentional bias increased every virtual complex cue compared to virtual neutral environments among alcohol dependent subjects (Bordnick et al., 2008). Ryan et al. (2010) found that the subscore “Thinking about alcohol” was higher after social pressure for the binge drinker group compared to healthy controls. Through implicit measurement (with a Virtual Approach-Avoidance Task), Kim and Lee (2015) highlighted attentional bias in heavy social drinkers (HSD) that showed increased difficulty avoiding virtual alcohol-related situations compared to light social drinkers. High depression score may however have affected response time in the HSD group, thus limiting this result.

Cocaine

One study (Saladin et al., 2006) concerning adults (n = 11) suffering from cocaine use disorder (DSM-IV) (Guze, 1995) assessed cue reactivity after virtual cocaine-related cues compared to virtual neutral environment, randomly exposed. Virtual environments included visual and auditory cues.

Craving

Craving induction through virtual cues has been demonstrated with contextual and complex environments compared to neutral virtual environments in cocaine-dependent subjects. Craving assessment was performed using single-item VAS. Situations triggering highest craving are those related to cocaine use and interactions with a dealer.

Physiological response

Physiological reactivity was also measured. Heart rate increased in a manner comparable to craving in scenes of cocaine use and interaction with a dealer compared to neutral scenes. Besides, there was no change in skin conductance measurements between virtual neutral exposure or virtual cocaine-related exposure.

Cannabis

One study (Bordnick et al., 2009) concerning adults (n = 20) suffering from cannabis use disorder (DSM-IV) (Guze, 1995) assessed cue reactivity after virtual cannabis-related cues were introduced. The study was controlled with a virtual neutral environment that was randomly exposed. Virtual environments consisted of multisensory exposure with visual, auditory and olfactory cues.

Craving

Bordnick et al. (2009) showed craving induction after virtual proximal and complex cues compared to virtual neutral stimuli in cannabis dependent subjects. Craving assessment was performed using single-item VAS.

Attention to cue

In this study (Bordnick et al., 2009), sub-scores of the 3 items of the Cannabis Attention Scale (CAS) self-questionnaire was increased after cannabis cues in VR compared to neutral stimuli in VR.

Gambling Disorder

One study (Bouchard et al., 2017) assessed cue reactivity after virtual gambling-related cues in 28 male frequent gamblers [SOGS (Lesieur and Blume, 1987)]. Virtual gambling cue exposure was controlled with 1 real complex environment (video lottery terminal) and 1 real neutral environment (board game), randomly exposed. Virtual environments consisted of multisensory exposure with visual and auditory cues.

Craving

Craving (both anticipation and desire to play dimensions) were induced after exposure to a virtual and real slot machine environments in Bouchard et al. (2017). Craving assessment was performed using multi-dimensional scale CGS. Increase in desire was higher compared to “casual gamers.” Moreover, craving measured by the total score of the GCS was significantly correlated to gambling dependence.

Internet Gaming Disorder

One study (Shin et al., 2018) concerning adolescents and young adults (n = 34) suffering from internet gaming disorder [DSM-5 (Petry and O'Brien, 2013)] controlled with a group of 30 healthy subjects assessed cue reactivity after virtual internet gaming-related cues. Virtual environments consisted of multisensory exposure with visual and auditory cues.

Craving

Shin et al. (2018) found induced craving after randomized exposure to 4 virtual complex internet gaming-related cues (cyber-cafes). Craving assessment was performed using a single-item VAS. Craving was higher in the 2 active environments (coffee entrance and gaming invitation task) compared to a passive observation of a gaming conversation. In the environment associated with the task of refusal skills practice, the use of coping with gaming invitation strategies reduced craving compared to other environments in the IGD group. In addition, craving level and severity of dependence (modified-Internet Addiction Test) were correlated in the “coffee entrance” situation within the IGD group and a trend was found in the observation and gaming invitation situation. Although cybersickness score was higher in the IGD group compared to the control group, scores reported in both groups were low by SSQ (Simulator Sickness Questionnaire) standards.

Treatment

Studies and results described below are summarized in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Clinical trial on Virtual Reality (VR) treatment in addiction.

Nicotine

Virtual Exposure Therapy (VET)

5 VET trials (Lee et al., 2004; Park et al., 2014; Pericot-Valverde et al., 2014, 2015, 2019) assessed craving and nicotine addiction reduction in nicotine dependent adults (n = 241). One VET trial was effective on craving and cigarette consumption reduction (controlled on exhaled CO concentration) after 5 weekly sessions on 48 subjects with low to moderate nicotine dependence in association with a tobacco education group (Pericot-Valverde et al., 2014). In a second trial, the team showed an association between craving reduction and younger age, higher cigarette consumption, impulsivity and depressive symptoms with the same exposure protocol (Pericot-Valverde et al., 2015). However, 1 CT and 1 RCT (Park et al., 2014; Pericot-Valverde et al., 2019) found mixed results (Pericot-Valverde et al., 2019). In the first study, similar changes were observed in both VET and CBT groups on cigarette consumption reduction, exhaled CO concentration and dependence score post-treatment and at follow-up in moderate dependent subjects (Park et al., 2014). However, craving decrease wasn't significant in either group and higher baseline Fagerström score in the CBT group may have limited comparability. The second study, a RCT combination of VET plus CBT for smoking cessation, showed no benefit on retention and cessation rate compared with CBT alone after randomization in 102 nicotine moderate dependent subjects (Pericot-Valverde et al., 2019). While VET was associated with a reduction in nicotine craving, the 1-year relapse rate was higher in the VET + CBT group. Explanation concerning this last surprising result could be that cue induced craving responses extinguished after CET protocol re-emerged, a phenomenon known as spontaneous recovery. Otherwise, individuals assigned to CBT only protocol may have over time developed and rehearsed strategies for coping with both craving and withdrawal symptoms, reducing their susceptibility to relapse.

Virtual Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (VCBT)

Two randomized controlled trials (n = 137) studied VCBT in nicotine addiction reduction (Girard et al., 2009; Bordnick et al., 2012).

The first VBT program consisted of finding and crushing 60 cigarettes in a virtual environment without therapist assistance. Implemented 4 weekly in conjunction with a psychosocial support program, the VBT was effective on nicotine addiction reduction and abstinence rate in 91 moderate to high dependent smokers after treatment and during follow-up compared to a virtual task control (Girard et al., 2009).

The second VCBT consisted of 10 weekly sessions including a gradual and personalized exposure to craving with training in coping skills guided by a therapist. Implemented in combination with a nicotine replacement therapy, the VCBT was effective on cigarette consumption and craving reduction compared to nicotine replacement therapy alone in 46 moderates to high dependent smokers (Bordnick et al., 2012). In addition, the retention rate and self-confidence at the end of the intervention and coping skills during follow-up were better in this group. However, these results should be interpreted with caution because of low overall adherence to treatment.

Alcohol

Three controlled studies (n = 70) in Korea evaluated virtual exposure therapy, including aversive exposure situations on alcohol craving reduction (Lee et al., 2009; Choi and Lee, 2015; Son et al., 2015).

VET was more effective than a standardized CBT on reducing alcohol craving after 10 biweekly sessions in 38 males who were abstinent for a week. The virtual exposure included relaxation time, craving and aversive exposure guided by a therapist (Lee et al., 2009). Using the same protocol, this team found no correlation between craving reduction and brain metabolism in FDG-PET in 12 patients abstinent for 1 week (Son et al., 2015). On the other hand, VET was effective on craving reduction in heavy social drinkers after one session compared to light consumers (Choi and Lee, 2015). The exposure consisted of two aversive social situations including visual and auditory stimuli not supervised by a therapist. Moreover, HSD's ranking of alcohol markers in positive categories decreased (alcohol-IAT behavioral task) after treatment and VET was associated with decreased ocular fixation times for alcohol-related markers and improved alcohol-Stroop test scores for both groups.

Despite these encouraging results, evidence of VET effectiveness on alcohol dependence reduction had still not been studied.

Gambling Disorder

One study found no reduction on urge to gamble after a single VET on 10 lottery players (Giroux et al., 2013). More recently, pilot data from Bouchard et al. showed promising results (Bouchard et al., 2017): after demonstrating the effectiveness of a VET protocol on reducing desire to gamble similarly to a real-life exposure in 28 frequent players, they found no difference in craving reduction between randomized VET and imagined exposure on 34 pathological gamblers, both group receiving standardized CBT. Finally, 4 VET sessions were associated with stronger sense of control over gambling-related disorders after the third exposure compared to an exposure in imagination after randomization on 25 pathological players.

Internet Gaming Disorder

One VET study showed dependence reduction after 8 biweekly sessions compared to standard CBT on 24 randomized subjects (Park et al., 2016). The VET consisted of relaxation time followed by personalized craving and aversive cue exposure assisted by a therapist, with positive reinforcement. However, no craving measure was performed.

Discussion

In the present review, we discuss major results related to VR that may provide solutions in the field of addiction especially for assessment and treatment.

Several results suggest that immersing subjects in virtual environments related to an addiction can combat many issues regarding craving assessment (Hone-Blanchet et al., 2014; Pericot-Valverde et al., 2016). VR have proved to be effective in triggering craving in both substance use disorder and behavioral addiction. Specifically designed to be immersive, the VR multisensorial cue exposition is able to drive cue attentional bias in order to report cognitive distortion related to addiction and trigger interoceptive reaction such as heart rate variation.

Although lack of statistical power makes it difficult to expose differences in craving response between types of cues, some results from nicotine craving studies (cf table) suggest that greater craving can be triggered by complex environments. Moreover, recent interest in proximal stimuli has emerged (virtual cigarettes) and new studies on other substance consumption behavior in virtual reality could further enrich the exposition realism.

Social interaction with avatars can efficiently induce cravings for multiple substances, mainly assessed in cigarette smoking and alcohol studies. However, difficulties persist in separating social craving from context craving. Indeed, several questions remain regarding stimuli specification assessment in VR craving induction. A significant number of studies were exposed to carry over effect while a better understanding of craving stimuli characteristics could be obtained by using a two by two comparison protocols or by an effective carry over effect control (for example, by controlling a return to baseline of craving level between each active stimulus exposure).

The lack of stimuli comparability resulting from the craving ceiling effect may have been increased by VAS widespread use by limiting the measurement to its intensity level, while a dimensional analysis of scales such as QSU could have revealed interesting qualitative differences (Rosenberg, 2009). More ecological assessments (eye tracking or virtual approach-avoidance task) could be proposed in order to provide an objective craving assessment by the use of real time behavioral measures (Cox et al., 2014). Its implementation in future research would help craving assessment but also offer insight and motivation to treatment via a feedback use. Feeling of presence and cybersickness should be assessed more rigorously to improve the level of ecological validity while increasing the acceptability of VR, especially since these two components are negatively correlated (Weech et al., 2019).

Traditional Cue Exposure Therapy studies' designs are mainly on substance-related cues detached from social environmental or in vivo scenarios. However, metanalyses report disappointing results (Mellentin et al., 2017; Ferreri et al., 2018), highlighting methodological biases preventing clear conclusions on its effectiveness in addictive disorders. VR has the potential to exceed those limits by providing a virtual immersion in environments closely related to everyday life and typical drug administration scenarios in order to better trigger craving in an individualized and progressive level. Indeed, Virtual Exposure Therapy (VET) has shown to be effective on craving reduction in nicotine and gambling disorders. Korean studies Lee et al. (2009), Son et al. (2015), and Choi and Lee (2015) find interesting results by adding aversive expositions to VET in alcohol use disorder. The combination of positive and negative affective states could lead to a restructuring of the conditioned stimulus response relation in addiction and should eventually result in avoidance of the administered drug.

In the future, attention to potential limits should be considered:

• Cost-effective studies are needed.

• Access to VR and therapists training need to be evaluated.

• Ethical considerations remain regarding the use of aversive exposures.

• Cybersickness could represent a threat to virtual reality usability and effectiveness and would also require integration in future studies (Gallagher and Ferrè, 2018).

• Patient's adherence should be accurately assessed before implementing aversion therapy in routine practice.

Both positive and negative studies show numerous methodological biases: confusion bias due to the absence of control groups, selection bias due to the absence of randomization, attrition bias due to little intent to treat analysis, altogether indicating low internal validity and limiting the possibility of drawing clear conclusions.

Future studies appear necessary and should evaluate dependence and abstinence maintenance as primary endpoint, offer more long-term follow-up, and include more control treatment groups to limit confusion bias.

Efficacy of treatment based exclusively on virtual exposure to drug related cues appears to be limited, but interesting results point the great potential of implementing VR in CBT. Pericot-Valverde et al. (2019) shows how VR could be disruptive by pointing out how an unsupervised behavior task in VR, such as finding and crushing cigarettes, can achieve positive results by making it enjoyable. Besides, it suggests that integrating several concepts such as patient empowerment to increase self-efficacy and self-regulation with more advances models on addiction associating action cue-exposure to positive mood state could achieve a full embodied experience for addiction behavior change in VR (Riva et al., 2017).

Despite these limitations, VR has the potential to offer new opportunities for treatment through its ability to provide a more ecological environment with more control and safety over exposition. The ability to fine-tune environments composition according to patient's preferences and most relevant data makes it a future tool for personalized medicine.

Because VR has the potential to be disruptive [for review (Freeman et al., 2017)] effort should be made to ensure good acceptability among therapist by providing reliable information, developing interest and allowing them to contribute to the development of these new technologies (Bourla et al., 2018). Furthermore, there is an urgent need to specifically assess the user experience. Virtual reality tends to develop in psychiatric practice (Freeman et al., 2017). Although its acceptability seems good in patients with mental disorders (Navarro-Haro et al., 2017), it has yet to be evaluated among health professionals. Its diffusion can challenge our professional heritage, the younger generations being potentially more sensitized to it. Efforts should be made through greater theoretical and practical information, especially since its application for health professionals could allow them to improve their skills in the assessment and treatment of addictive disorders (Fleming et al., 2009).

Besides its acceptability, its accessibility also raises questions. It could be limited due to the lack of financial support. The populations suffering from substance disorders being frequently in situation of social precariousness. The question of the cost of VR will also have to be evaluated in order to consider its wide implementation in health centers.

Conclusion

The studies presented in this review suggest that VR provide benefits in the assessment and treatment of substance use disorders, behavior addictions and achieve high levels of ecological validity. While, craving provocation in VR is effective across addiction disorders, treatments based exclusively on virtual exposure to drug related cues as shown heterogenous result. The addition of learning coping strategies in VRCBT studies are promising, however more rigorous methodological studies are warranted.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

TS and TB screening and abstract analysis, both authors contributed equally to the writing. AB, SM, and FF conceptual framework, supervised development of work, helped in data interpretation and manuscript evaluation. C-SP and J-VB helped to evaluate and edit the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

A sincere thank you to Samantha Johnson for her comments and proofreading of this paper.

References

Acker, J., and MacKillop, J. (2013). Behavioral economic analysis of cue-elicited craving for tobacco: a virtual reality study. Nicotine Tob. Res. 15, 1409–1416. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nts341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, P. L., Rothbaum, B. O., and Hodges, L. (2001). Virtual reality: using the virtual world to improve quality of life in the real world. Bull. Menninger. Clin. 65, 78–91. doi: 10.1521/bumc.65.1.78.18713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bordnick, P. S., Copp, H. L., Traylor, A., Graap, K. M., Carter, B. L., Walton, A., et al. (2009). Reactivity to cannabis cues in virtual reality environments. J. Psychoact. Drugs. 41, 105–112. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2009.10399903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bordnick, P. S., Graap, K. M., Copp, H., Brooks, J., Ferrer, M., and Logue, B. (2004). Utilizing virtual reality to standardize nicotine craving research: a pilot study. Addict. Behav. 29, 1889–1894. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bordnick, P. S., Graap, K. M., Copp, H. L., Brooks, J., and Ferrer, M. (2005). Virtual reality cue reactivity assessment in cigarette smokers. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 8, 487–492. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2005.8.487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bordnick, P. S., Traylor, A., Copp, H. L., Graap, K. M., Carter, B., Ferrer, M., et al. (2008). Assessing reactivity to virtual reality alcohol based cues. Addict. Behav. 33, 743–756. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bordnick, P. S., Traylor, A. C., Carter, B. L., and Graap, K. M. (2012). A feasibility study of virtual reality-based coping skills training for nicotine dependence. Res. Soc. Work Pract. 22, 293–300. doi: 10.1177/1049731511426880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouchard, S., Robillard, G., Giroux, I., Jacques, C., Loranger, C., St.-Pierre, M., et al. (2017). Using virtual reality in the treatment of gambling disorder: the development of a new tool for cognitive behavior therapy. Front. Psychiatr. 8:27. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bourla, A., Ferreri, F., Ogorzelec, L., Peretti, C.-S., Guinchard, C., and Mouchabac, S. (2018). Psychiatrists' attitudes toward disruptive new technologies: mixed-methods study. JMIR Ment. Health 5:e10240. doi: 10.2196/10240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, B. L., Bordnick, P., Traylor, A., Day, S. X., and Paris, M. (2008). Location and longing: the nicotine craving experience in virtual reality. Drug. Alcohol. Depend. 95, 73–80. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, B. L., and Tiffany, S. T. (1999). Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction 94, 327–340. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9433273.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, Y. J., and Lee, J-H. (2015). The effect of virtual covert sensitization on reducing alcohol craving in heavy social drinkers. Virtual Real. 19, 111–117. doi: 10.1007/s10055-015-0264-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conklin, C. A., Robin, N., Perkins, K. A., Salkeld, R. P., and McClernon, F. J. (2008). Proximal versus distal cues to smoke: the effects of environments on smokers' cue-reactivity. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 16, 207–214. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.16.3.207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conklin, C. A., and Tiffany, S. T. (2002). Applying extinction research and theory to cue-exposure addiction treatments. Addiction 97, 155–167. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00014.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, W. M., Fadardi, J. S., Intriligator, J. M., and Klinger, E. (2014). Attentional bias modification for addictive behaviors: clinical implications. CNS Spectr. 19, 215–224. doi: 10.1017/S1092852914000091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Degenhardt, L., Glantz, M., Evans-Lacko, S., Sadikova, E., Sampson, N., Thornicroft, G., et al. (2017). Estimating treatment coverage for people with substance use disorders: an analysis of data from the World Mental Health Surveys. World Psychiatry. 16, 299–307. doi: 10.1002/wps.20457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrer-García, M., García-Rodríguez, O., Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J., Pericot-Valverde, I., and Secades-Villa, R. (2010). Efficacy of virtual reality in triggering the craving to smoke: its relation to level of presence and nicotine dependence. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 154, 123–127. doi: 10.3233/978-1-60750-561-7-123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreri, F., Bourla, A., Mouchabac, S., and Karila, L. (2018). e-Addictology: an overview of new technologies for assessing and intervening in addictive behaviors. Front Psychiatry. 9:51. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, M., Marhe, R., and Franken, I. H. A. (2014). The clinical relevance of attentional bias in substance use disorders. CNS Spectr. 19, 225–230. doi: 10.1017/S1092852913000321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, M., Olsen, D., Stathes, H., Boteler, L., Grossberg, P., Pfeifer, J., et al. (2009). Virtual reality skills training for health care professionals in alcohol screening and brief intervention. J. Am. Board Fam. Med. 22, 387–398. doi: 10.3122/jabfm.2009.04.080208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, D., Reeve, S., Robinson, A., Ehlers, A., Clark, D., Spanlang, B., et al. (2017). Virtual reality in the assessment, understanding, and treatment of mental health disorders. Psychol. Med. 47, 2393–2400. doi: 10.1017/S003329171700040X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, M., and Ferrè, E. R. (2018). Cybersickness: a multisensory integration perspective. Multisens. Res. 31, 645–674. doi: 10.1163/22134808-20181293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamito, P., Oliveira, J., Baptista, A., Morais, D., Lopes, P., Rosa, P., et al. (2014). Eliciting nicotine craving with virtual smoking cues. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 17, 556–561. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Rodríguez, O., Pericot-Valverde, I., Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J., Ferrer-García, M., and Secades-Villa, R. (2012). Validation of smoking-related virtual environments for cue exposure therapy. Addict. Behav. 37, 703–708. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García-Rodríguez, O., Weidberg, S., Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J., and Secades-Villa, R. (2013). Smoking a virtual cigarette increases craving among smokers. Addict. Behav. 38, 2551–2554. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girard, B., Turcotte, V., Bouchard, S., and Girard, B. (2009). Crushing virtual cigarettes reduces tobacco addiction and treatment discontinuation. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12, 477–483. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2009.0118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giroux, I., Faucher-Gravel, A., St.-Hilaire, A., Boudreault, C., Jacques, C., and Bouchard, S. (2013). Gambling exposure in virtual reality and modification of urge to gamble. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 16, 224–231. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.1573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guze, S. B. (1995). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th ed. (DSM-IV). Am. J. Psychiatr. 152, 1228–1228. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.8.1228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., and Fagerström, K. O. (1991). The fagerström test for nicotine dependence: a revision of the fagerström tolerance questionnaire. Br. J. Addict. 86, 1119–1127. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hone-Blanchet, A., Wensing, T., and Fecteau, S. (2014). The use of virtual reality in craving assessment and cue-exposure therapy in substance use disorders. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:844. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaganoff, E., Bordnick, P. S., and Carter, B. L. (2012). Feasibility of using virtual reality to assess nicotine cue reactivity during treatment. Res. Soc. Work Pract. 22, 159–165. doi: 10.1177/1049731511428617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, D.-Y., and Lee, J-H. (2015). Development of a virtual approach-avoidance task to assess alcohol cravings. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 18, 763–766. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kivlahan, D. R., Sher, K. J., and Donovan, D. M. (1989). The alcohol dependence scale: a validation study among inpatient alcoholics. J. Stud. Alcohol. 50, 170–175. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1989.50.170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Lim, Y., Graham, S. J., Kim, G., Wiederhold, B. K., Wiederhold, M. D., et al. (2004). Nicotine craving and cue exposure therapy by using virtual environments. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 7, 705–713. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J.-H., Lim, Y., Wiederhold, B. K., and Graham, S. J. (2005). A functional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI) study of cue-induced smoking craving in virtual environments. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback. 30, 195–204. doi: 10.1007/s10484-005-6377-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. H., Ku, J., Kim, K., Kim, B., Kim, I. Y., Yang, B.-H., et al. (2003). Experimental application of virtual reality for nicotine craving through cue exposure. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 6, 275–280. doi: 10.1089/109493103322011560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. S., Namkoong, K., Ku, J., Cho, S., Park, J. Y., Choi, Y. K., et al. (2008). Social pressure-induced craving in patients with alcohol dependence: application of virtual reality to coping skill training. Psychiatr. Investig. 5, 239–243. doi: 10.4306/pi.2008.5.4.239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. H., Han, D. H., Oh, S., Lyoo, I. K., Lee, Y. S., Renshaw, P. F., et al. (2009). Quantitative electroencephalographic (qEEG) correlates of craving during virtual reality therapy in alcohol-dependent patients. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 91, 393–397. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesieur, H. R., and Blume, S. B. (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): a new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. Am. J. Psychiatr. 144, 1184–1188. doi: 10.1176/ajp.144.9.1184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mellentin, A. I., Skøt, L., Nielsen, B., Schippers, G. M., Nielsen, A. S., Stenager, E., et al. (2017). Cue exposure therapy for the treatment of alcohol use disorders: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 57, 195–207. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro-Haro, M. V., López-Del-Hoyo, Y., Campos, D., Linehan, M. M., Hoffman, H. G., et al. (2017). Meditation experts try virtual reality mindfulness: a pilot study evaluation of the feasibility and acceptability of virtual reality to facilitate mindfulness practice in people attending a Mindfulness conference. PLoS ONE 12:e0187777. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0187777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ouramdane, N., Otmane, S., and Mallem, M. (2009). Interaction 3D en réalité virtuelle. Etat de l'art. Tech. Sci. Inform. 28, 1017–1049. doi: 10.3166/tsi.28.1017-1049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paris, M. M., Carter, B. L., Traylor, A. C., Bordnick, P. S., Day, S. X., Armsworth, M. W., et al. (2011). Cue reactivity in virtual reality: the role of context. Addict. Behav. 36, 696–699. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C.-B., Choi, J.-S., Park, S. M., Lee, J.-Y., Jung, H. Y., Seol, J.-M., et al. (2014). Comparison of the effectiveness of virtual cue exposure therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy for nicotine dependence. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 17, 262–267. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2013.0253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, M. J., Kim, D. J., Lee, U., Na, E. J., and Jeon, H. J. (2019). A literature overview of virtual reality (VR) in treatment of psychiatric disorders: recent advances and limitations. Front Psychiatry. 10:505. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S. Y., Kim, S. M., Roh, S., Soh, M.-A., Lee, S. H., Kim, H., et al. (2016). The effects of a virtual reality treatment program for online gaming addiction. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed. 129, 99–108. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2016.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pericot-Valverde, I., García-Rodríguez, O., Gutierrez-Maldonado, J., Ferrer-García, M., and Secades-Villa, R. (2011). Evolution of smoking urge during exposure through virtual reality. Stud. Health Technol. Inform. 167, 74–79. doi: 10.3233/978-1-60750-766-6-74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pericot-Valverde, I., García-Rodríguez, O., Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J., and Secades-Villa, R. (2015). Individual variables related to craving reduction in cue exposure treatment. Addict. Behav. 49, 59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pericot-Valverde, I., Germeroth, L. J., and Tiffany, S. T. (2016). The use of virtual reality in the production of cue-specific craving for cigarettes: a meta-analysis. Nicotine Tob. Res. 18, 538–546. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntv216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pericot-Valverde, I., Secades-Villa, R., and Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J. (2019). A randomized clinical trial of cue exposure treatment through virtual reality for smoking cessation. J. Subst. Abuse. Treat. 96, 26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2018.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pericot-Valverde, I., Secades-Villa, R., Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J., and García-Rodríguez, O. (2014). Effects of systematic cue exposure through virtual reality on cigarette craving. Nicotine Tob. Res. 16, 1470–1477. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petry, N. M., and O'Brien, C. P. (2013). Internet gaming disorder and the DSM-5. Addiction 108, 1186–1187. doi: 10.1111/add.12162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riva, G., Serino, S., Di Lernia, D., Pavone, E. F., and Dakanalis, A. (2017). Embodied medicine: mens sana in corpore virtuale sano. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:120. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, H. (2009). Clinical and laboratory assessment of the subjective experience of drug craving. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 29, 519–534. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, J. J., Kreiner, D. S., Chapman, M. D., and Stark-Wroblewski, K. (2010). Virtual reality cues for binge drinking in college students. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 13, 159–162. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saladin, M. E., Brady, K. T., Graap, K., and Rothbaum, B. O. (2006). A preliminary report on the use of virtual reality technology to elicit craving and cue reactivity in cocaine dependent individuals. Addict. Behav. 31, 1881–1894. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., de la Fuente, J. R., and Grant, M. (1993). Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption–II. Addiction 88, 791–804. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sayette, M. A. (2016). The Role of Craving in substance use disorders: theoretical and methodological issues. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 12, 407–433. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schardt, C., Adam, M. B., Owens, T., Keitz, S., and Fontelo, P. (2007). Utilization of the PICO framework to improve searching PubMed for clinical questions. BMC Med. Inform. Decis. Mak. 7:16. doi: 10.1186/1472-6947-7-16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shin, Y.-B., Kim, J.-J., Kim, M.-K., Kyeong, S., Jung, Y. H., Eom, H., et al. (2018). Development of an effective virtual environment in eliciting craving in adolescents and young adults with internet gaming disorder. PLoS ONE. 13:e0195677. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slater, M. (2009). Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 364, 3549–57. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slater, M., Linakis, V., Usoh, M., and Kooper, R. (1999). “Immersion, presence, and performance in virtual environments: an experiment with tri-dimensional chess” in ACM Virtual Reality Software and Technology (VRST).

Google Scholar

Son, J. H., Lee, S. H., Seok, J. W., Kee, B. S., Lee, H. W., Kim, H. J., et al. (2015). Virtual reality therapy for the treatment of alcohol dependence: a preliminary investigation with positron emission tomography/computerized tomography. J. Stud. Alcohol. Drugs. 76, 620–627. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2015.76.620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanney, K. M., Kennedy, R. S., and Drexler, J. M. (1997). Cybersickness is Not Simulator Sickness. Available online at: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/107118139704100292?journalCode=proe. (accessed Aug 18, 2019).

Google Scholar

Thompson-Lake, D. G. Y., Cooper, K. N., Mahoney, J. J., Bordnick, P. S., Salas, R., Kosten, T. R., et al. (2015). Withdrawal symptoms and nicotine dependence severity predict virtual reality craving in cigarette-deprived smokers. Nicotine Tob. Res. 17, 796–802. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntu245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Traylor, A. C., Bordnick, P. S., and Carter, B. L. (2008). Assessing craving in young adult smokers using virtual reality. Am. J. Addict. 17, 436–440. doi: 10.1080/10550490802268876

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Traylor, A. C., Bordnick, P. S., and Carter, B. L. (2009). Using virtual reality to assess young adult smokers' attention to cues. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 12, 373–378. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2009.0070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Traylor, A. C., Parrish, D. E., Copp, H. L., and Bordnick, P. S. (2011). Using virtual reality to investigate complex and contextual cue reactivity in nicotine dependent problem drinkers. Addict. Behav. 36, 1068–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weech, S., Kenny, S., and Barnett-Cowan, M. (2019). Presence and cybersickness in virtual reality are negatively related: a review. Front. Psychol. 10:158. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Witmer, B. G., and Singer, M. J. (1998). Measuring presence in virtual environments: a presence questionnaire. Presence. 7, 225–240. doi: 10.1162/105474698565686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: virtual reality, addictive behavior, alcohol, nicotine, cocain

Citation: Segawa T, Baudry T, Bourla A, Blanc J-V, Peretti C-S, Mouchabac S and Ferreri F (2020) Virtual Reality (VR) in Assessment and Treatment of Addictive Disorders: A Systematic Review. Front. Neurosci. 13:1409. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01409

Received: 16 September 2019; Accepted: 12 December 2019;
Published: 10 January 2020.

Edited by:

Liana Fattore, Italian National Research Council (CNR), Italy

Reviewed by:

Vijay Arjun Ramchandani, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA), United States
Marcello Solinas, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), France

Copyright © 2020 Segawa, Baudry, Bourla, Blanc, Peretti, Mouchabac and Ferreri. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexis Bourla, alexis.bourla@aphp.fr

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.