Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 18 June 2021
Sec. Neonatology
Volume 9 - 2021 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2021.660393

Creative Music Therapy and Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Pre-term Infants at 2 Years: A Randomized Controlled Pilot Trial

  • 1Department of Neonatology, Newborn Research Zurich, University Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Neonatology and Pediatric Intensive Care, University Children's Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3Children's Research Center, University Children's Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 4Larsson-Rosenquist Foundation Centre for Neurodevelopment, Growth and Nutrition of the Newborn, University Hospital Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

Impaired neurodevelopment is increasingly recognized as a major health issue in children born prematurely. Creative music therapy (CMT) intends to prevent and or reduce neurobehavioral deficits in pre-term infants using musical stimulation and socio-emotional co-regulation. We conducted a randomized, clinical pilot CMT trial to test feasibility and to examine long-term neurodevelopmental outcomes in pre-term infants (NCT02434224: https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02434224). Eighty-two pre-term infants were randomized either to CMT or standard care. A specially trained music therapist provided family-integrating CMT via infant-directed singing during hospitalization. Fifty-six infants underwent follow-up at 2 years of corrected age. No significant beneficial nor adverse effects of CMT were identified in routine clinical neurodevelopmental measures (Bayley-III Scales of Infant and Toddler Development and the standardized neurological examination). Longer term follow-up (5 years) and larger future studies are recommended to elucidate possible long-term effects of music in relation to more sensitive outcomes including executive function, detailed language processing and social-emotional development.

Highlights

- First randomized study to evaluate creative music therapy in pre-term infants.

- Use of standardized, long-term, clinically relevant outcomes for neurodevelopment.

- No significant effect of CMT was identified at 2 years of age.

- Follow-up neurodevelopmental examination at 5 years is recommended.

- Future multi-center investigations with more sensitive outcome measure are needed.

R. has developed very well. He does not speak yet (except for mama, tata etc.), but he has been humming lots of songs since May. One can recognize the melody immediately when he starts. All of our children have always liked music, but we have never seen it like him. We always ask ourselves whether early music therapy has influenced it or whether it would have happened otherwise? Sometimes he wakes up at night and can then hum songs for half an hour. Then we have a great concert. Do you have experiences or feedback from parents who report the same thing? We just wonderyes

Mother's quote of a 1-year-old infant born prematurely in the 24rd week of gestation participating in the present trial.

Introduction

Impaired neurodevelopment is a major health issue in children born prematurely worldwide. The most frequently observed developmental problems in this high-risk population include cognitive deficits (1, 2), problems with executive function (3, 4) and behavioral issues (5, 6). Developmental problems may lead to academic underachievement and long-life mental health concerns (4, 7). Some children experience severe motor and sensory disabilities including cerebral palsy, hearing loss, and visual impairment (8). Neurodevelopmental follow-up programs for very pre-term infants have become a standard of post-discharge care in many western countries to support early detection and initiate timely supportive therapies.

In addition to many predictors of unfavorable neurodevelopmental outcomes in pre-term infants (e.g., brain lesions, bronchopulmonary dysplasia, infections, retinopathy of prematurity), it is plausible that the stress of the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) environment and the separation from the mother may negatively influence its development (911). Animal and human studies have shown that adequate auditory stimulation as well as socio-emotional caregiving and contact is essential for neurodevelopment (1215). Toddlers who received active music sessions beginning at 6 months of age show enhanced pre-linguistic communicative, musical, and social development (16). Further evidence indicates that music interventions may enhance neurobehavioral abilities that are often impaired in infants born prematurely (17). Notably music interventions are associated with enhanced self-regulatory abilities in full-term infants (18).

An increasing number and variety of therapies and interventions are emerging. Interventions aim to prevent and reduce neurobehavioral deficits in pre-term infants (19). Creative Music Therapy (CMT) uses musical stimulation and socio-emotional co-regulation to address patient needs. Socio-emotional co-regulation refers to caregiver-guided dyadic process aimed at regulating an infant's emotional distress with musical parameters and therapeutic responsiveness. CMT is an individualized, interactive, resource- and needs-oriented approach with a core tenet that almost every human being responds to music–no matter how premature, ill, or disabled (20). The approach aims to relax and stimulate the infant by involving both the infant and parent(s) in creating meaningful interactions through music. After assessing infant and parental needs, the music therapist hums and sings in lullaby style with entrained responsiveness (21) in parallel to the infant's breathing patterns and in harmony with facial expressions and gesticulations. CMT aims to support infant self-regulation by experienced co-regulation and mutual responsiveness. Because CMT integrates the family, parents are individually in the therapeutic process, i.e., providing music therapy during skin-to-skin contact to foster bi-directional relaxation, bonding and attachment.

Several studies have demonstrated beneficial effects of music therapy on arousal, behavior and infant respiratory rate (2224). A meta-analysis (25) supports the observed beneficial effects, particularly on infant respiratory rate and maternal anxiety. However, there is little data on the effect of music therapy on short- and long-term neurodevelopment in pre-term infants. To address this gap, we conducted a prospective, randomized, controlled pilot trial examining feasibility and short- and long-term neurodevelopmental outcomes in very pre-term infants (26). The primary outcome of the trial was brain structure and function and assessed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) at term equivalent age. The lagged, resting-state MRI analysis revealed infants who have received CMT had more reliable functional networks, higher functional integration, and shorter thalamocortical processing delays in several regions (left prefrontal, supplementary motor, inferior temporal) compared to infants in the control group (27). These promising results suggest potential positive impact on long-term neurodevelopment as these brain regions are associated with discriminating sounds, language, cognition, fine motor coordination and empathy (28, 29). Herein, we report the predefined secondary long-term neurodevelopmental outcomes of the trial participants at 2 years of corrected age (26). We hypothesized that infants receiving CMT would have improved cognitive, language, and motor outcomes compared to the infants who received standard care.

Materials and Methods

Study Design

The study is a prospective, single-center, between-subject randomized, controlled pilot trial in preparation for a multi-center trial. The study was conducted in line with the principles of the declaration of Helsinki. The study was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee at the University of Zurich, Switzerland (KEK-ZH 2014-0655) and the independent Monitoring Committee of the Swiss Agency for Therapeutic Products (Swissmedic) (Audit No.: CTCQA14). All parents provided written informed consent prior to the initiation of study procedures.

Population

Eighty-two very pre-term infants were recruited from January 2015 to December 2017 from a level-3 perinatal center (Department of Neonatology, University Hospital Zurich, Switzerland). Infants were eligible if they were born prematurely (gestational age <32 weeks), reached the chronological age ≥ seven days of life and did not require invasive cardiovascular and respiratory support at recruitment. Neonates were excluded if they were diagnosed with a major congenital anomaly/ genetically defined syndrome, congenital malformation adversely affecting life expectancy/neurodevelopment, intraventricular hemorrhage (≥Volpe grade 3) (30), or when the infants were admitted for palliative care. For safety reasons, CMT would pause when infants would require invasive cardiovascular and respiratory support during the study period as recommended in the clinical practice protocol (31).

Randomization and Masking

Participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups in a 1:1 ratio using a random computer-generated list created prior to study initiation. Allocation was concealed. Blinding of participating families and personnel was not possible due to the nature of the intervention (i.e., integrating parents). Follow-up assessors and data analysts were masked to infant group allocation. Details on the study protocol including blinding for the subsequent long-term follow-up assessment have been previously published (26).

Procedures

Infants were assigned to receive either standard care according to department practice (including skin-to-skin care), or to receive standard care plus a minimum of 8 CMT sessions as previously recommended for measurable impact (32, 33). Procedures to minimize contamination have been described in the study protocol (26).

Following parental consent, a qualified music therapist (FH) provided CMT for ~20 min at the bedside (incubator or warmer) two to three times per week until hospital discharge. Interventions followed feeding time and were conducted with the infant alone or with the parents providing skin-to-skin contact.

During CMT (infant only), the music therapist started the session by touching the infants, e.g., at the head and feet, to “welcome” the infant and entrain the breathing rhythm (31). After observation, the music therapist would hum smoothly and tailor vocalizations in line with the infant‘s breathing rhythm, facial expression, and gesture. Throughout the session a melody progressively develops according to the infant‘s behavioral state. For example, the therapist would use sedating musical parameters (e.g., calm, repetitive humming) to soothe an agitated infant. Conversely, the therapist would use activating musical parameters (up-rising melodies) to stimulate a limp infant. In all cases, parental musical preferences would be integrated into the improvisation – see clinical CMT protocol for details (31). At the end of the session, the music therapist would smoothly fade out the humming smoothly and gently remove her hands from the infant.

In the presence of the parents, the music therapist uses the same method of humming and singing. During these sessions, a parent holds their infant providing skin-to-skin contact throughout the session. The music therapist places an arched NICU-monochord with 29 strings (Wooden vibro-acoustic string instrument provided by Saitenklang, Bern, Switzerland©) at the parents' elbow to transmit relaxing vibrations via bone and air conduction. The music therapist then hums/sings in harmony with the monochord sounds. At the end of the session, the music therapist slowly smoothly fades the music out. Throughout the sessions parents are encouraged and empowered to respond to the infant's cues and needs, as well as to hum and speak “motherese” (34) to facilitate bonding. Prior to hospital discharge, parents receive a consultation providing information and instruction on musical interventions during the first year of life including recommended songs to support musical development for each developmental milestone (27).

Neonatal Data

Neonatal data was extracted from the prospective national database of the Swiss Neonatal Network. Gestational age, birth weight z-scores, major brain injury, bronchopulmonary dysplasia, retinopathy of prematurity, necrotizing enterocolitis, neonatal sepsis and socioeconomic status were defined as previously published for this cohort (35).

Neurodevelopment Assessment

At follow-up, parents were invited to bring their child for routine follow-up examinations according to the Swiss post-discharge neurodevelopment surveillance standard for very pre-term infants (36). Experienced developmental pediatricians, who were masked to study group allocation, performed neurodevelopmental assessments (18–24 months of corrected age) at a Swiss Neonatal Network and Follow-up Group Center. The examination involved standardized neurodevelopmental assessments including the Bayley-III Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley-III) (37) and a structured neurologic examination, somatic growth measurements. Vision and hearing were assessed either by direct examination or via parental report. Cognitive, language, and motor developmental scores were obtained from the Bayley-III. The cognitive composite score includes evaluation of sensorimotor development, object relatedness, memory, exploration, manipulation, concept formation and memory. The language composite score is obtained by summing the receptive and expressive skills scores. The receptive language score includes comprehending and responding to requests as well as discriminating between environmental sounds. The expressive language score evaluates the child's ability to name objects and actions, respond to questions, communicate wants, and use multiword sentences. Similarly, the motor assessment includes both fine motor score (e.g., visual tracking, use of a “pincer” grasp and reaching objects) and a gross motor score (e.g., locomotion, balance, and jumping) (37). Receptive and expressive language scores, and fine and gross motor scores have normative values (standard deviation [SD]) of 10 and composite scores of 100. Higher values indicate better performance. Cerebral palsy was defined and graded according to Rosenbaum et al. (38) and the Gross Motor Function Classification System (GMFCS) (39). Ongoing developmental therapies (e.g., physiotherapy) were recorded according to parental report.

Outcomes

Prespecified secondary outcomes include Bayley-III cognitive, language and motor development scores as well as cerebral palsy of any grade (present/not present), severe cerebral palsy (GMFS grade 3–5), ongoing therapy, and visual or hearing problems–defined as moderately reduced vision or unilateral blindness with good vision in the contralateral eye and hearing loss (40–90 dB hearing level) corrected with aids, respectively.

Post-hoc exploratory outcomes included the composite outcome category “severe neurodevelopmental impairment” which was defined as one of the following: a score below −2SD from the norm (i.e., <70) in the Bayley-III, cerebral palsy with a GMFCS of more than two, visual or hearing problems; and as the continuous absolute values and z-scores of body weight, height, and head circumference. De-identified outcome data were extracted from the Swiss Neonatal record base and entered into the study database for blinded analysis.

Statistics

The sample size for this pilot trial was set at 60 participants per pilot trial design recommendations (4042). Since this is the first randomized study to evaluate possible neurodevelopmental effects of CMT in pre-term infants, we employed a pilot design to evaluate outcome measures and assess clinical/recruitment feasibility to determine estimate sample size for a future, appropriately powered trial (26). Independent Student's t-test or Mann-Whitney U-test was used to compare baseline characteristics between groups as appropriate. Fisher's exact test was used for to assess interval scaled and nominal variables respectively. Intention-to-treat (ITT) analysis was employed to compare secondary and exploratory outcomes according to treatment allocation. Unadjusted intervention association with outcomes was calculated using independent Student's t-test for continuous variables and Fisher's exact test for nominal variables. The assumption that continuous outcome values were normally distributed in both groups was not violated. A per-protocol analysis was conducted by comparing infants who received the treatment originally allocated with a minimum of eight CMT sessions with infants who received standard care according to department practice without CMT (compare 26). Results are reported as mean differences and odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals. Since missing data were related to the severity of prematurity (i.e., not random), no data were imputed. Two-sided tests were performed and findings were considered significant when p < 0.05. Statistical analyses were performed by using IBM SPSS, Version 26. The study is reported according to the CONSORT Guidelines (43).

Results

Population and Feasibility

Among 293 very pre-term infants, 82 were included in the study (gestational age = 28.0 ± 2.1 weeks). One hundred and twelve infants did not meet all inclusion criteria. As described in our previous paper on study feasibility (27), seventy one parents rejected participation because they were concerned that the MRI exam could be an additional stressful event for their infant and/ or their infant could be assigned to the control group without CMT. In total, 37 were randomly assigned to receive CMT and 45 to standard care (27). Of the 82 randomized infants, eight infants in the standard treatment group were transferred to another hospital. Seven infants from the CMT group did not receive the intervention because they had to be transferred to another center where the CMT intervention was not available. Six infants from the standard care group had to be excluded from the study since the parents withdrew study participation when the infants were randomized to the control group without any music stimulation (compare 27).

All CMT infants received the CMT sessions as planned (median 15; range: 8–30 sessions; total CMT sessions: 446) during hospitalization (median 5 weeks; range: 3–10 weeks). On average, CMT infants received nine CMT sessions during kangaroo-care (range: 4–22; total sessions: 262) and six sessions at the bedside with the infant alone (range 2–18; total sessions: 173). No adverse reactions occurred during and directly after the CMT [compare Haslbeck et al. (27)].

Between April 2018 and March 2020 (last assessment), 56/82 (68%) of randomized infants returned for the follow-up visit. Of the 26 drop-outs (n = 13 in both the CMT and standard care groups) discontinuation resulted from moving abroad (n = 2) or for unknown reasons (n = 24) (Figure 1). Groups neither differed in neonatal parameters nor baseline demographic characteristics (Table 1). Compared to infants lost to follow-up, infants assessed at age 2 years had similar baseline characteristics but for lower gestational age (p < 0.001), longer supplemental oxygen therapy (p = 0.001) and longer hospital stay (p < 0.001) (Supplementary Table 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and neonatal baseline characteristics of infants randomized to music therapy (CMT) or standard care (control group).

Neurodevelopmental Outcomes at Corrected Age 2 Years

In the intention-to-treat analysis, no differences were observed between groups in Bayley-III scores, cerebral palsy rate, ongoing developmental therapy, or vision/hearing problems (Table 2). Similarly, exploratory outcomes “neurodevelopmental impairment” rate and somatic growth parameters did not differ at follow-up (Table 2). Findings were confirmed by per protocol analysis (Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Intention to treat outcomes at follow-up (2-years) for infants in music (n = 24) and standard care (n = 32) groups.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Per protocol outcomes at follow-up (2-years) for infants in music (n = 24) and standard care (n = 32).

Discussion

We report prespecified secondary neurodevelopmental outcomes of a randomized controlled pilot trial investigating the effect of CMT on brain development at term equivalent age. No treatment effects of CMT were noted on cognitive, language and motor outcomes in 2-year-old pre-term infants. These findings are in contrast with the trial primary outcome analysis of the trial. Primary outcome analysis at term equivalent age indicated evidence of beneficial CMT effects on functional brain activity and connectivity in networks underlying higher-order cognitive, socio-emotional, and motor functions (27). In line with the present study, a recent report noted no group differences in Bayley-III findings between infants exposed to recorded music in the NICU compared to controls (44). Similar to the primary outcomes of our study, investigators identified enhanced brain network development at term equivalent age in the intervention group (45).

We expected that CMT may positively influence receptive auditory and language skills in the intervention group, particularly in comprehending and responding to requests as well as in discriminating environmental sound. This assumption was based on a prior report of fMRI findings of improved superior temporal gyrus functional connectivity in infants receiving CMT–linked with language comprehension and processing of social cognition (46). Additionally, we have previously reported improved centrality in the left and right hippocampus with CMT (27)–associated with social-emotional processing, comprehension and response (47). Observed correlations are aligned with the assumption that music and language processing activate similar networks (48). Researchers have posited the capacity to receive, comprehend, and discriminate sound stimuli depends on the hearer‘s prior hearing experiences (49, 50).

One explanation for the lack of observed CMT effect observed in this pilot trial may be that CMT has no effect on the long-term development of the infant. Another possible explanation may be that the standardized neurodevelopment battery may not be sensitive enough to detect changes that are observed in the fMRI analysis. Lejeune and colleagues additionally used the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery an additional outcome measurement alongside the Bayley-III. However, no differences were observed in pre-term born infants randomized to music exposure vs. controls (no music) at 12 and 24 months. The effect of CMT on neurocognitive development may be better explored with test batteries examining executive functioning. Sensitive endpoints evaluating composite emotional processing and regulation, executive functioning and detailed language processing may be more suitable for assessing the possible impact of music on neurodevelopment. This is relevant as music has been specifically linked to non-musical faculties including socio-emotional competencies, intelligence, and language processing (51). Our group and others have reported increased functional connectivity in brain regions is associated with neonatal music exposure in pre-term infants (27, 45). Future research should explore hot (i.e., driven by emotion) and cold (i.e., driven by logic and critical analysis) executive functioning (52), working memory (53), and selective attention (54).

Alternative explanations for present findings include the fact that the standardized neurodevelopmental exam during early infancy does not assess the wide range of cognitive functions. Indeed, such cognitive functions, working memory, self-regulation and language skills appear then differentiate during development from early infancy to school age such as. Some have argued that the Bayley-III test underestimates developmental delay in very pre-term infants assessed at 2 years of age thereby reducing statistical power of randomized controlled trials (55). While the Bayley-III may be appropriate for providing evidence on the safety of early intervention, researchers have questioned the predictive validity of the Bayley-III for examining efficacy at 2 years of age -when severely disabled infants have been excluded – as was the case in the present study (41, 56, 57). Therefore, we plan long-term neurodevelopment assessment at school age as neurodevelopment can be tested in a more sensitive and specific way (57, 58).

Another possible explanation for the lack of long-term effect of CMT on neurodevelopment may be that the intervention was exclusively provided during the NICU stay. A post-discharge follow-up music therapy program comprising music group sessions and home visits by a qualified music therapist could fill this gap and bridge specialized NICU care and ambulatory pediatric care while supporting parent-to-parent (peer) in group sessions–as recommended by several authors (59, 60). Several review articles note that early intervention bridging hospital with follow-up interventions are associated with most beneficial outcomes (19).

In general, the question remains if our improved MRI results at term equivalent age (TEA) in the CMT infants may indicate improved long-term outcomes in this population. Indeed, abnormalities detected in MRI exams at TEA are claimed to be associated with adverse long-term outcomes in pre-term infants (61). Woodward et al. (8) have demonstrated significant links between cerebral white-matter and gray-matter abnormalities in MRI results at TAE and adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes, e.g., severe psychomotor and cognitive delay, at 2 years of age in very pre-term infants. However, the authors emphasize that impairments at TAE must not result in adverse long-term outcomes since further environmental, therapeutic, and educational factors may influence the infant‘s neurodevelopment. Besides, Van't Hooft et al. (61) conclude in their meta-analysis that MRI results at TEA may not predict other neurocognitive and behavioral long-term outcomes in pre-term infants. Further research may be needed to shed more light on strong associations between MRI results at TEA and long-term outcomes among pre-term infants in general and in particular concerning music interventions (8, 44, 61).

In terms of study feasibility, our findings show that CMT is safe and readily implemented. Overall study participation and compliance were moderate (27). It is worthwhile to note that the single-center study setting did not provide optimal conditions for conducting a randomized treatment intervention. Study participants were hospitalized in the same NICU and some parents of in the standard care group indirectly experienced the nature of the CMT. For this reason, some parents decided to withdraw participation and some parents may have indirectly acquired some CMT input. To mitigate this potential risk in a future trial, cluster randomization may be considered as an alternative approach to reduce the risk of cross-contamination between groups and improve compliance rates.

The drop-out rate at follow-up exam was high (32%) despite the fact that the Swiss neonatal network assures invitations to all eligible very pre-term infants with continuously updated address lists (36). The high attrition rate is in line with national (35) and international (36) attendance rates that vary from 50 to 100% (62). Like in the study by Schlapbach et al. (35), a number of infants with milder prematurity symptoms missed the follow-up exam resulting in a slight underestimation of these participants. To improve parental follow-up compliance, e.g., for the planned multi-center study, (63) recommend that neonatologists should earlier inform parents about the value of the follow-up exams, to invite the parents via phone and letter, and to invite twins/ triplets to the same appointment. When parents do not attend the exam the responsible physician should call the parents and declare the value of the exam again (36).

There are some study limitations that decrease the generalizability of our findings. First, this a pilot study had a small sample size thereby reducing the power of secondary outcome analyses. Second, although drop-outs were evenly distributed between groups, differences in attrition rates at follow-up may have introduced bias in evaluating outcomes. Nevertheless, feasibility and pilot testing results provide key information informing a planned multi-center study.

A relative strength of this study is that CMT is non-invasive, low-cost, and family-integrating approach and we used standardized routine long-term clinically relevant measures of neurodevelopment to evaluate the intervention. Future trials could investigate parental psychological adjustment and bonding as preliminary evidence suggests that CMT contributes to parental empowerment and activates “communicative musicality” (64) thereby benefiting parent-infant-attachment (21, 65, 66). Interventions that support parent-infant attachment in early life may enhance infant socioemotional development, mental health and future relationships (67, 68). This is particularly salient for pre-term infants who may have suboptimal attachment circumstances due to NICU stays (9). Moreover, successful parenting may be one of the most valuable resource for the child health and psychosocial development (19).

Conclusion

This randomized pilot trial examining the effect of CMT on neurodevelopment in pre-term infants at 2 years shows no evidence of treatment effect. These observations are in contrast with results of the primary outcome analysis of brain MRI imaging data at term equivalent age. We plan further follow-up examination at 5 years, when the neurodevelopment is more differentiated. Future studies with larger samples with sensitive outcome measures of executive functioning, language processing, social-emotional development, quality of life and parent-infant-bonding may uncover possible mid- and long-term effects of CMT on infants and their parents.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics committee Zurich. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

FH: conceptualization, data curation, statistical analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, project administration, resources, software, visualization, and writing–original draft. DB and HB: conceptualization, supervision, validation, and writing–review. CH: writing–review. GN: conceptualization, main supervision, visualization, writing–main review, and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Foundation of Anna Müller Grocholski, the Stiftung für Neonatologie, and the Vontobel Foundation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We want to thank all participating infants and families. We thank the Department of Neonatology at the University Hospital Zurich and the Child Developmental Center at the University Children's Hospital Zurich, Switzerland for all their support.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2021.660393/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

Bayley-III, Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, third edition; CCS, Cognitive composite score; CMT, Creative music therapy; GMFCS, Gross motor function classification system; ITT, Intention-to-treat analysis; NICU, Neonatal intensive care unit; MCS, Motor composite score; MRI, Magnetic resonance imaging; PP, Per-Protocol analysis; TEA, Term equivalent age.

References

1. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM, Anand KJ. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis. J Am Med Assoc. (2002) 288:728–37. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.6.728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Delobel-Ayoub M, Arnaud C, White-Koning M, Casper C, Pierrat V, Garel M, et al. Behavioral problems and cognitive performance at 5 years of age after very preterm birth: the EPIPAGE study. Pediatrics. (2009) 123:1485–92. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Nosarti C, Giouroukou E, Micali N, Rifkin L, Morris RG, Murray RM. Impaired executive functioning in young adults born very preterm. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2007) 13:571–81. doi: 10.1017/S1355617707070725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Twilhaar ES, De Kieviet JF, Aarnoudse-Moens CSH, Van Elburg RM, Oosterlaan J. Academic performance of children born preterm: a meta-analysis and meta-regression. Arch Dis Child. (2018) 103:322–30. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2017-312916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Aarnoudse-Moens CSH, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics. (2009) 124:717–28. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Bröring T, Oostrom KJ, van Dijk-Lokkart EM, Lafeber HN, Brugman A, Oosterlaan J. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder symptoms in school-age children born very preterm. Res Dev Disabilities. (2018) 74:103–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Mendonça M, Bilgin A, Wolke D. Association of preterm birth and low birth weight with romantic partnership, sexual intercourse, and parenthood in adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Netw Open. (2019) 2:1–14. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.6961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Woodward LJ, Anderson PJ, Austin NC, Howard K, Inder TE. Neonatal MRI to predict neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm infants. New Engl J Med. (2006) 355:685–94. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa053792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kommers D, Oei G, Chen W, Feijs L, Bambang Oetomo. S. Suboptimal bonding impairs hormonal, epigenetic and neuronal development in preterm infants, but these impairments can be reversed. Acta Paediatr Int J Paediatr. (2016) 105:738–51. doi: 10.1111/apa.13254

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Perlman JM. Neurobehavioral deficits in premature graduates of intensive care–potential medical and neonatal environmental risk factors. Pediatrics. (2001) 108:1339–48. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.6.1339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Wachman EM, Lahav A. The effects of noise on preterm infants in the NICU. Arch Dis Child. (2011) 96:F305–9. doi: 10.1136/adc.2009.182014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Champagne FA, Curley JP. Epigenetic mechanisms mediating the long-term effects of maternal care on development. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2009) 33:593–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Chang EF, Merzenich MM. Environmental noise retards auditory cortical development. Science. (2003) 300:498–502. doi: 10.1126/science.1082163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Dahmen JC, King AJ. Learning to hear: plasticity of auditory cortical processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2007) 17:456–64. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. de Villers-Sidani E, Simpson KL, Lu YF, Lin RC, Merzenich MM. Manipulating critical period closure across different sectors of the primary auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci. (2008) 11:957–65. doi: 10.1038/nn.2144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Gerry D, Unrau A, Trainor LJ. Active music classes in infancy enhance musical, communicative and social development. Dev Sci. (2012) 15:398–407. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01142.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Hille ET, den Ouden AL, Saigal S, Wolke D, Lambert M, Whitaker A, et al. Behavioural problems in children who weigh 1000 g or less at birth in four countries. Lancet. (2001) 357:1641–3. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)04818-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Rossi A, Molinaro A, Savi E, Micheletti S, Galli J, Chirico G, et al. Music reduces pain perception in healthy newborns: a comparison between different music tracks and recoded heartbeat. Early Hum Dev. (2018) 124:7–10. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2018.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Puthussery S, Chutiyami M, Tseng PC, Kilby L, Kapadia J. Effectiveness of early intervention programs for parents of preterm infants: a meta-review of systematic reviews. BMC Pediatrics. (2018) 18:1–18. doi: 10.1186/s12887-018-1205-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Nordoff P, Robbins C. Creative Music Therapy: Individualized Treatment for the Handicapped Child. New York: John Day Company (1977).

Google Scholar

21. Haslbeck FB. The interactive potential of creative music therapy with premature infants and their parents: a qualitative analysis. Nord J Music Ther. (2014) 23:36–70. doi: 10.1080/08098131.2013.790918

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hartling L, Shaik MS, Tjosvold L, Leicht R, Liang Y, Kumar M. Music for medical indications in the neonatal period: a systematic review of randomised controlled trials. Arch Dis Child. (2009) 94:F349–54. doi: 10.1136/adc.2008.148411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Haslbeck FB. Music therapy for premature infants and their parents: an integrative review. Nord J Music Ther. (2012) 21:203–26. doi: 10.1080/08098131.2011.648653

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Standley J. Music therapy research in NICU, an updates meta-analysis. Neonatal Network. (2012) 31:311–6. doi: 10.1891/0730-0832.31.5.311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Bieleninik Ł, Ghetti C, Gold C. Music therapy for preterm infants and their parents: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. (2016) 138:e20160971. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-0971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Haslbeck FB, Bucher H-U, Bassler D, Hagmann C. Creative music therapy to promote brain structure, function, and neurobehavioral outcomes in preterm infants: a randomized controlled pilot trial protocol. Pilot Feasibility Stud. (2017) 3:36. doi: 10.1186/s40814-017-0180-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Haslbeck FB, Jakab A, Held U, Bassler D, Bucher H-U, et al. Creative music therapy to promote brain function and brain structure in preterm infants: a randomized controlled pilot study. NeuroImage. (2020) 25:102171. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Fachner J, Gold C, Erkkila J. Music therapy modulates fronto-temporal activity in rest-EEG in depressed clients. Brain Topogr. (2013) 26:338–54. doi: 10.1007/s10548-012-0254-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Koelsch S. A neuroscientific perspective on music therapy. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2009) 1169:374–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04592.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Volpe JJ. (2008) Neurology of the Newborn. 5th Edn. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders Elsevier

Google Scholar

31. Haslbeck FB, Bassler D. Clinical practice protocol of creative music therapy for preterm infants and their parents in the neonatal intensive care unit. JOVE. (2020) 25:1–10. doi: 10.3791/60412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hanson-Abromeit D, Shoemark H, Loewy J. Music therapy with pediatric units: newborn intensive care unit (NICU). In: Hanson-Abromeit D, Colwell C, editors. Medical Music Therapy for Pediatrics in Hospital Settings. Using Music to Support Medical Interventions. Maryland: Silver Spring, MD: AMTA (2008). p. 15–69.

Google Scholar

33. Haslbeck FB, Bassler D. Music from the very beginning—a neuroscience-based framework for music as therapy for preterm infants and their parents. Front Behav Neurosci. (2018) 12:1–7. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Papousek M, Papousek H. The meanings of melodies in motherese in tone and stress languages. J Infant Behav Dev. (1991) 14:415–40. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(91)90031-M

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Schlapbach LJ, Adams M, Proietti E, Aebischer M, Grunt S, Borradori-Tolsa C, et al. Outcome at two years of age in a Swiss national cohort of extremely preterm infants born between 2000 and 2008. BMC Pediatr. (2012) 12:198. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-12-198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Stoll BJ, Hansen NI, Bell EF, Walsh MC, Carlo WA, Shankaran S, et al. Trends in care practices, morbidity, and mortality of extremely preterm neonates, 1993–2012 barbara. Jama. (2015) 314:1039–51. doi: 10.1001/jama.2015.10244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infants and Toffler Development, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (2006). doi: 10.1037/t14978-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Rosenbaum P, Paneth N, Leviton A, Goldstein M, Bax M, Damiano D, et al. A report: the definition and classification of cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2007) 109:8–14.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

39. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell D, Wood E, Galuppi B. (1997). Development and reliability of a system to classify gross motor function in children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol. 39:214–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1997.tb07414.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Browne RH. On the use of a pilot sample for sample size determination. Stat Med. (1995) 14:1933–40. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780141709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Natalucci G, Latal B, Koller B, Rüegger C, Sick B, Held L, et al. Effect of early prophylactic high-dose recombinant human erythropoietin in very preterm infants on neurodevelopmental outcome at 2 years a randomized clinical trial. JAMA. (2016) 315:2079–85. doi: 10.1001/jama.2016.5504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Shih WJ, Ohman-Strickland PA, Lin Y. Analysis of pilot and early phase studies with small sample sizes. Stat Med. (2004) 23:1827–42. doi: 10.1002/sim.1807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Eldridge SM, Chan CL, Campbell MJ, Bond CM, Hopewell S, Thabane L, et al. CONSORT 2010 statement: extension to randomised pilot and feasibility trials. BMJ. (2016) 355:i5239. doi: 10.1136/bmj.i5239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Lejeune F, Lordier L, Pittet MP, Schoenhals L, Grandjean D, Hüppi PS, et al. Effects of an early postnatal music intervention on cognitive and emotional development in preterm children at 12 and 24 months: preliminary findings. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:494. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Lordier L, Meskaldji D.-E., Grouiller F, Pittet MP, Vollenweider A, et al. Music in premature infants enhances high-level cognitive brain networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2019) 116:12103–12108. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1817536116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Koelsch S.tefan, Kasper E, Sammler D, Schulze K, Gunter T, et al. Music, language and meaning: brain signatures of semantic processing. Nat Neurosci. (2004) 7:302–7. doi: 10.1038/nn1197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Immordino-Yang MH, Singh V. Hippocampal contributions to the processing of social emotions. Hum Brain Mapp. (2013) 34:945–55. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Besson M, Schön D, Moreno S, Santos A, Magne C. Influence of musical expertise and musical training on pitch processing in music and language. Restorative Neurol Neurosci. (2007) 25:399–410.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

49. Engel AC, Bueno CD, Sleifer P. Treinamento musical e habilidades do processamento auditivo em crianças: revisão sistemática. Audiol Commun Res. (2019) 24:e2116. doi: 10.1590/2317-6431-2018-2116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Tervaniemi M, Janhunen L, Kruck S, Putkinen V, Huotilainen M. Auditory profiles of classical, jazz, and rock musicians: genre-specific sensitivity to musical sound features. Front Psychol. (2016) 6:1900. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Law LNC, Zentner M. Assessing musical abilities objectively: construction and validation of the profile of music perception skills. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e52508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Pozzetti T, Ometto A, Gangi S, Picciolini O, Presezzi G, Gardon L, et al. Emerging executive skills in very preterm children at 2 years corrected age: a composite assessment. Child Neuropsychol. (2014) 20:145–61. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2012.762759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Hughes C, Ensor R. Executive function and theory of mind in 2 year olds: a family affair? Dev Neuropsychol. (2005) 28:645–68. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2802_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Mulder H, Hoofs H, Verhagen J, van der Veen I, Leseman PPM. Psychometric properties and convergent and predictive validity of an executive function test battery for two-year-olds. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:733. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Johnson S, Moore T, Marlow N. Using the bayley-III to assess neurodevelopmental delay: which cut-off should be used? Pediatr Res. (2014) 75:670–4. doi: 10.1038/pr.2014.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gould JF, Hunt E, Roberts RM, Louise J, Collins CT, Makrides M. Can the bayley scales of infant development at 18 months predict child behaviour at 7 years? J Paediatr Child Health. (2019) 55:74–81. doi: 10.1111/jpc.14163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Marlow N. Is survival and neurodevelopmental impairment at 2 years of age the gold standard outcome for neonatal studies? Arch Dis Child. (2015) 100:F82–4. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2014-306191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Wehrle FM, Kaufmann L, Benz LD, Huber R, O'Gorman RL, Latal B, et al. Very preterm adolescents show impaired performance with increasing demands in executive function tasks. Early Hum Dev. (2016) 92:37–43. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2015.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Ghetti C, Bieleninik Ł, Hysing M, Kvestad I, Assmus J, Romeo R, et al. Longitudinal study of music therapy' s effectiveness for premature infants and their caregivers (LongSTEP): protocol for an international randomised trial. BMJ Open. (2019) 9:e025062. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-025062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Walworth D, Standley JM, Robertson A, Smith A, Swedberg O, Peyton JJ. Effects of neurodevelopmental stimulation on premature infants in neonatal intensive care: randomized controlled trial. J Neonatal Nurs. (2012) 18:210–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jnn.2012.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Van't Hooft J, van der Lee JH, Opmeer BC, Aarnoudse-Moens CSH, Leenders AGE, Mol BWJ, et al. Predicting developmental outcomes in premature infants by term equivalent mRI: systematic review and meta-analysis. Syst Rev. (2015) 4:71. doi: 10.1186/s13643-015-0058-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Mas C, Gérardin P, Chirpaz E, Carbonnier M, Mussard C, Samperiz S, et al. Follow-up at two years of age and early predictors of non-compliance in a cohort of very preterm infants. Early Hum Dev. (2017) 108:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Adams M, Borradori-Tolsa C, Bickle-Graz M, Grunt S, Weber P, Capone Mori A, et al. Follow-up assessment of high-risk newborns in Switzerland. Paediatrica. (2014) 25:6–10. doi: 10.5167/uzh-104948

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Malloch S, Trevarthen C. Communicative musicality. Exploring the basis of human campionship. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C, editors. Communicative Musicality. Exploring the Basics of Human Campionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2009).

Google Scholar

65. Ettenberger M, Odell-Miller H, Cárdenas CR, Parker M. Family-centred music therapy with preterm infants and their parents in the neonatal-intensive-care-unit (NICU) in Colombia: a mixed-methods study. Nord J Music Ther. (2016) 25:21–2. doi: 10.1080/08098131.2016.1179903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Kehl SM, Marca-ghaemmaghami P, La H.aller M, Pichler-stachl E, Bucher HU, et al. Creative music therapy with premature infants and their parents : a mixed-method pilot study on parents' anxiety, stress and depressive symptoms and parent – infant attachment. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:18. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18010265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Bowlby J. The Making and Breaking of Affectional Bonds. London: Tavistock (1979).

Google Scholar

68. Bruschweiler-Stern N. The neonatal moment of meeting–building the dialogue, strengthening the bond. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. (2009) 18:533–44. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2009.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pre-maturity, creative music therapy, randomized controlled trial, outcome, neurodevelopment, Bayley-III, neurological exam

Citation: Haslbeck FB, Bucher HU, Bassler D, Hagmann C and Natalucci G (2021) Creative Music Therapy and Neurodevelopmental Outcomes in Pre-term Infants at 2 Years: A Randomized Controlled Pilot Trial. Front. Pediatr. 9:660393. doi: 10.3389/fped.2021.660393

Received: 02 February 2021; Accepted: 17 May 2021;
Published: 18 June 2021.

Edited by:

Bernhard Resch, Medical University of Graz, Austria

Reviewed by:

Wes Onland, Amsterdam University Medical Center, Netherlands
Helmut Dietmar Hummler, Tübingen University Hospital, Germany

Copyright © 2021 Haslbeck, Bucher, Bassler, Hagmann and Natalucci. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Friederike Barbara Haslbeck, friederike.haslbeck@usz.ch

Download