Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 17 December 2018
Sec. Educational Psychology

A Systematic Review of Teachers’ Causal Attributions: Prevalence, Correlates, and Consequences

  • 1Department of Special Education and Counselling, The Education University of Hong Kong, Tai Po, Hong Kong
  • 2Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada

The current review provides an overview of published research on teachers’ causal attributions since 1970s in the context of theoretical assumptions outlined in Weiner’s (2010) attribution theory. Results across 79 studies are first examined with respect to the prevalence of teachers’ interpersonal causal attributions for student performance and misbehavior, as well as intrapersonal attributions for occupational stress. Second, findings showing significant relations between teachers’ attributions and their emotions and cognitions, as well as student outcomes, are discussed. Third, an overview of results showing the prevalence and implications of teachers’ causal attributions to be moderated by critical background variables is also provided. Finally, observed themes across study findings are highlighted with respect to the fundamental attribution error and the utility of Weiner’s attribution theory for understanding how teachers’ explanations for classroom stressors impact their instruction, well-being, and student development.

Introduction

Classroom instruction and learning are complex processes characterized by intensive cognition as to the causal nature of student and teacher outcomes. When a student performs poorly on an academic task, for example, he or she might ascribe the failure to lack of effort, bad luck, or teaching quality. Likewise, teachers also experience successes and failures with respect to their instructional goals concerning student performance and classroom behaviors, with studies showing teachers who fail to achieve their personal teaching goals to report a more maladaptive emotional profile (Sutton, 2007; Frenzel et al., 2009, 2015; Frenzel, 2014) as well as poorer levels of occupational stress, burnout, satisfaction, and commitment (Chang, 2009). Recent findings further suggest that teachers’ motivational beliefs may help mitigate the impact of perceived instructional failures on their emotional well-being such as intrinsic motivation (e.g., Roth et al., 2007; Roness, 2011), achievement goal orientations (e.g., Butler, 2012; Wang et al., 2017), and teaching self-efficacy (Klassen et al., 2011).

Over the past 50 years, motivation research has also consistently examined the effects of individuals’ causal explanations or attributions for failure experiences on their emotions, persistence, and achievement in educational settings (e.g., Heider, 1958; Atkinson, 1964). According to attribution theory, individuals are particularly motivated to seek specific explanations for negative educational outcomes, with these causal attributions, in turn, having important consequences for academic development (Weiner, 1985, 2010). Whereas most research applying attribution theory to the classroom has focussed on students’ causal attributions for academic setbacks (for reviews, see Weiner, 1976; Wilson and Linville, 1982; Ames and Ames, 1984; Graham, 1984b, 1991; Forsterling, 1985; Graham and Williams, 2009), the nature and effects of teachers’ causal attributions for their classroom experiences has also received consistent empirical attention (Cooper and Lowe; 1977, study 2; Tollefson and Chen, 1988; Butler, 1994; Matteucci et al., 2008b; Wang et al., 2015). However, there to date exists no comprehensive review article in education literature of empirical research on the prevalence, correlates, and consequences of causal attributions for classroom challenges made by teachers. To address this unfortunate research gap, the present article provides a systematic review of published research on the types of attributions made by practicing teachers’ when faced with student problems and teaching obstacles as well as their impact on instructional behaviors, student outcomes, and teachers’ own psychological well-being.

Conceptual Framework

Attribution Theory

According to Weiner’s (1985, 2000, 2010) attribution theory, a causal attribution is defined as an individual’s perception as to the cause of a success or failure event, with the attribution selected able to be classified according to underlying causal dimensions that correspond to specific effects on subsequent emotions, decision-making, and performance. The first of these causal dimensions, locus of causality, refers to the attribution being either internal or external to the individual in origin. For example, attributions having an internal locus of causality would prototypically implicate one’s own ability or effort, whereas attributions to external factors might instead involve luck or environmental circumstances. In contrast, the second causal dimension, stability, reflected the extent to which one attributed an event to causes that were temporally persistent, such as low ability, or causes that fluctuated over time, such as luck. In the event of failure, although one would expect it to reoccur if an attribution was made to a stable antecedent, attributions to unstable causes should be more motivating in allowing for the possibility of future success.

Although Weiner’s initial model included only two causal dimensions, a third dimension referred to as controllability – the extent to the perceived cause of an outcome is regarded as personally changeable – was later added to distinguish this framework from the “locus of control” construct proposed by Rotter (1966; Weiner, 1979). In contrast to Rotter’s model in which the concepts of internality and controllability were conflated (e.g., internal locus: attributions to effort and ability), Weiner differentiated between them given the divergent motivational consequences of internal attributions depending on their perceived controllability. For example, whereas a failure attribution to lack of effort implies the potential for improvement, attributing failure to an intractable lack of ability does not.

In addition to differentiating three dimensions underlying causal attributions, attribution theory further proposes specific cognitive and emotional consequences of each dimension that occur after causal attributions are selected, namely expectations of future performance and perceived responsibility for the events. Whereas attributions with an internal locus of causality should lead to perceptions of self-esteem and feelings of pride, expectations for future success and the emotions of hopefulness/hopelessness should be more significantly impacted by stability dimension of the attribution selected (Weiner, 1985, 2000, 2010). Likewise, the controllability dimension is proposed to most significantly correspond with perceptions of personal responsibility and the emotions of guilt and shame. This theory further proposes the occurrence of mixed emotions, for example, with failure attributions to lack of effort contributing to less pride (internal) yet higher levels of motivating emotions such as hope (unstable) and guilt (controllable), emotions that, in turn, are assumed to directly contribute to achievement-striving behaviors and performance.

Interpersonal and Intrapersonal Applications of Weiner’s Attribution Theory

Weiner’s (2001) attribution theory also differentiates between intrapersonal and interpersonal attributions for achievement outcomes. The intrapersonal perspective, as described above, refers to the attributions individuals make for their own performance, focusing on how expectations for personal success and responsibility can lead to self-directed feelings of pride, guilt, hope, or shame and, in turn, self-relevant educational outcomes (e.g., persistence). On the other hand, the interpersonal approach to Weiner’s theory concerns the attributions made for outcomes experienced by others and focuses primarily on how perceptions of another’s responsibility for an outcome contributes to other-directed emotions (e.g., sympathy, anger) and behaviors (e.g., punishment, assistance; Weiner, 2001, 2003). For example, although teachers may attribute their own instructional failures or occupational stress to specific factors (e.g., insufficient resources, lesson preparation), their attributions for students’ misbehaviors or poor performance may differ (e.g., insufficient student effort, parental support).

With respect to teacher behavior resulting from attributions for student failure to factors implicating student responsibility, Weiner (2003, 2010) further proposed two main goals underlying teacher reprimands: utilitarian goals or retributive goals. In contrast to teachers with utilitarian goals who use punishment to encourage future behavioral change, teachers with retributive goals focus instead on students’ past transgressions and administering negative deserved consequences out of fairness (Weiner, 2000). Moreover, early scenario research by Weiner and Kukla (1970) suggested that although the degree of reward vs. punishment administered by teachers depended largely on student performance, the perceived combination of prototypic attributions (student effort vs. ability) could also impact teaching behavior. In this study, poor student performance attributed to high ability combined with lack of effort was most strongly punished, whereas poor-performing students perceived as investing effort despite low ability were not punished as severely. Follow-up research by Reyna and Weiner (2001) further indicated that retributive punishment was more likely when students were perceived as responsible for their failure due to lack of effort, whereas more utilitarian punishment was reported when students were believed to lack ability and not be responsible for their performance (Weiner, 2003).

The Present Review

As outlined above, Weiner’s (1985, 2000, 2001, 2010) attribution theory thus represents a comprehensive theoretical framework for understanding how individuals in educational settings perceive the causes underlying the performance of themselves and others, as well as the effects of these causal attributions on subsequent cognitions, emotions, and behaviors in the classroom. In incorporating both interpersonal and intrapersonal perspectives, this theory is also particularly useful for understanding how teachers perceive student challenges and occupational stressors, and how their attributions for these experiences impact student–teacher interactions, teaching behaviors, and teachers’ emotional well-being. However, despite multiple publications providing in-depth reviews of research on causal attributions in students since the 1970s (e.g., Weiner, 1972, 1976; Graham, 1984b, 1991; Graham and Williams, 2009), there to date exists no published comprehensive reviews of research on causal attributions in teachers. The current review attempts to address the gap in motivation research literature by reviewing existing published research specifically exploring the types of causal attributions made by practicing K-12 teachers for both student difficulties and personal failures. More specifically, the current review aimed to answer: (1) how prevalent are certain attributional styles in teachers and (2) what are the relevant antecedents, correlates, and consequences of certain attributional styles on teachers and their students. Implications of the findings presented concerning future directions in teacher motivation and professional development practices will also be discussed. In the sections that follow, we first present our literature search and screening protocols, followed by subsequent reviews attributions of an interpersonal nature (concerning students’ academic performance vs. misbehaviors) and of an intrapersonal nature (teachers’ own occupational stress). Each type of causal attribution style is further described in each section with respect to its prevalence, relationships with teacher and/or student outcomes, and the extent of observed disagreement between teachers and others (e.g., parents, students) concerning specific attributional styles.

Methods

Four databases were included in the literature search including ERIC, PsycINFO, Web of Science, and Scopus. Concerning article search terms, the word “teacher” or “instructor” or “educator” have been included with the words “attribution” or “attributing” or “causal explanation” or “causal ascription” also being included in the keywords or abstract. Database searches included journal articles (e.g., empirical articles, reviews, open-access), book chapters, doctoral dissertations, or conference proceedings written in the English language (see Table 1 and Figure 1 for details on search terms and results). Snowball searches were also conducted by reviewing references of primary articles found through database searches. From an initial total of more than 3,889 search results, 373 unique and relevant articles were subsequently screened as per four exclusion criteria specific to the aim of the current review to investigate causal attributions in practicing K-12 educators concerning their teaching practice.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Key search term.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Literature searches flow diagram.

First, studies with non-practicing teachers were excluded, including studies with pre-service teachers (e.g., Goyette et al., 2000; Shields and Edens, 2009; Carter et al., 2014), teaching assistants (e.g., Morgan and Hastings, 1998; Meel, 2011), and convenience samples (e.g., psychology undergraduates; Weiner and Kukla, 1970; Reyna and Weiner, 2001, study 1). Second, as the review focuses on attributions made by K-12 teachers, studies with teachers in other educational contexts such as pre-school teachers (e.g., Dobbs and Arnold, 2009), vocational or language teachers outside K-12 settings (e.g., Dubois, 2000, study 3; Ghanizadeh and Ghonsooly, 2014, 2015; Jonasson, 2014), and post-secondary instructors were excluded (e.g., Ames, 1975; Zhou, 2006).

Third, articles examining attributions made by teachers that were not related to teaching or learning were excluded, such as attributions related to students’ physical attributes (e.g., Felson, 1980; Roth and Eisenberg, 1983), students’ unhealthy lifestyles (e.g., Power et al., 2010), chronic pain (Logan et al., 2007), poverty (e.g., Robinson, 2011), teacher selection and screening (e.g., Pounder, 1987), or principals’ behaviors (e.g., Leithwood and Jantzi, 1997). Finally, studies in which attributions made by students concerning their teachers were excluded (e.g., Wyatt and Haskett, 2001; Chedzoy and Burden, 2009), as were articles exploring students’ perceived teacher attributions (e.g., Hau and Salili, 1996; Prihadi et al., 2012) or vice versa (e.g., Bar-Tal et al., 1981), to more clearly demarcate attributions made by teachers from those made by students. With the exclusion criteria applied, the final review total included 79 empirical papers in which practicing teachers’ causal attributions for instructional issues were explicitly addressed with each article selected aligning with one of three emergent themes: teacher attributions for student performance, student behavioral/emotional problems, and teachers’ occupational stress (see Table 2 for details on study designs and paradigms for all these studies and see Table 3 for an overview of study outcomes, domains, and dimensions).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Study designs and paradigms for all 79 studies reviewed.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Overview of study outcomes, domains, and dimensions.

Results

Interpersonal Attributions: Student Performance

Most research on teachers’ causal attributions has explored teachers’ beliefs concerning the causes underlying their students’ academic performance with respect to success and failure outcomes. However, it should be noted that teachers’ perceptions of student performance are multifaceted and subjective in nature, with some studies examining teacher-perceived student success or failure as pertaining specifically to academic marks (e.g., Wiley and Eskilson, 1978; Butler, 1994) or student competition (e.g., top performers; Beckman, 1976), and others focusing instead on student improvement or learning gains as a successful outcome (Emmerich et al., 2006; Espinoza et al., 2014). In many studies, although teachers are directly asked to reflect on a particular student success or failure scenario, they are typically provided with no specific guidance by researchers as to the standard against which determinations of success and failure are to be made (e.g., Wiley and Eskilson, 1978; Cooper and Burger, 1980; Fennema et al., 1990; Clark, 1997). Accordingly, in the present review we operationalize the terms success and failure in a more general manner such that success concerns satisfactory academic work as perceived by teachers, with student failure being perceived as generally unsatisfactory academic work.

Attribution Prevalence

Findings from Burger et al. (1982) suggest that teachers tend to attribute students’ academic performance to factors that are internal to the student and stable over time, particularly when the outcome matches teachers’ original expectations toward the students (ability, consistent effort, student personality). Conversely, teachers’ instead tend to implicate unstable factors when explaining student performance that was not consistent with teachers’ initial expectations (instructional methods, temporary effort). It is important to note in the study by Burger et al. (1982) that teachers’ expectations were not assessed as a cognitive consequences of attributions to stable/unstable factors as proposed in Weiner’s theory (expectations for future success), but rather as in indicator of teachers’ satisfaction with their students’ performance (having satisfied past expectations for student success). Teachers have also been found to be more likely to explain student failure as due to factors internal to the student (e.g., student ability, effort) or family influences than teacher- and school-related issues (Burger et al., 1982; for similar findings see Rolison and Medway, 1985; Hall et al., 1989; Tollefson et al., 1990; Jager and Denessen, 2015).

More specifically, approximately half of the teachers in Tollefson et al.’s (1990) study attributed student failure to student effort, with a significant proportion of attributions also involving external family variables (30%) and other internal student characteristics (77%; e.g., previous experiences, emotional maturity, interest, attention), However, only 2% of teachers in this study attributed student failure to teacher characteristics, and no attributions were reported to task difficulty. Results from Jager and Denessen (2015) similarly showed teachers to primarily explain students’ failure as due to internal student factors (e.g., attention, motivation, effort) than lesson difficulty or instructional quality (least common), reporting greater blame when failure was perceived as internal to and controllable by the student and lower criticism following external and uncontrollable attributions. Although other study findings suggest that teachers’ may focus especially on students’ external and uncontrollable antecedents to explain student performance (e.g., influence of prior teachers, learning experiences, or other students; Bar-Tal and Guttmann, 1981; Rolison and Medway, 1985; Hall et al., 1989; Bertrand and Marsh, 2015), existing findings generally suggest that teachers are more likely to attribute student failure to factors internal to the students themselves.

In addition to studies exploring teacher attributions for student failures, studies comparing their attributions for student success have also been conducted. Overall, findings suggest that whereas teachers tend to attribute student failure to factors that are internal to students, and external to themselves, they are most likely to attribute student success to themselves (Guskey, 1982; Yehudah, 2002). For example, whereas teachers might tend to attribute student failures to students’ lack of effort, they are instead found to attribute students’ success to their instructional strategies (Gosling, 1994; Kulinna, 2007). However, other studies suggest that although teachers may show a tendency to take responsibility for student success (e.g., instructional quality), they nevertheless give credit to students for their success (e.g., student ability, effort; Ross et al., 1974) as opposed to external factors such as luck (Natale et al., 2009).

In addition to the types of attributions made by teachers for student performance in general, teachers’ attributions nonetheless differ depending on the student. As outlined in early research on self-fulfilling prophecies (Jussim and Harber, 2005; Jussim et al., 2009), teachers often assign “labels” to their students to differentiate their potential for success (e.g., perceived ability level), with their attributions for subsequent student performance tending to correspond with those pre-existing labels (Rejeski and McCook, 1980; Reyna, 2000, 2008). For example, although a “high-ability” students’ success would likely be attributed to ability, a success outcome for a “low-ability” student would instead be more often attributed to the effort or luck. Conversely, teachers have been found to report more external or unstable attributions when failure is experienced by a “high-ability” student (e.g., task difficulty, temporary low effort), with failure by “low-ability” students more commonly attributed to low aptitude (Cooper and Burger, 1980; Rejeski and McCook, 1980; Fennema et al., 1990).

Research specifically exploring variability in teachers’ attributions for student performance further demonstrates both consistency, in terms of predictable attributional tendencies, as well as variability from one student to the next (e.g., Jager and Denessen, 2015). More specifically, findings indicate that causal attributions to factors such as student ability, family, class difficulty, and instructional quality are consistently reported by teachers as responsible for students’ poor academic performance. Teachers have also been found to consistently attribute student failure as due to low motivation, low ability, and family difficulties as opposed to teaching-related factors (e.g., lesson difficulty, instructional quality; Jager and Denessen, 2015). However, some attributions have been found to vary significantly depending on the student, such as attributions for student failure to student attention and effort (Jager and Denessen, 2015). Nevertheless, as related findings suggest that teachers tend to perceive student effort as a stable internal trait (e.g., laziness; Tollefson et al., 1990), failing students believed to be investing insufficient effort may nevertheless not be expected to improve.

Relations With Teacher Emotions and Behavior

According to Weiner’s (1985, 2000, 2010) attribution theory, the way in which teachers perceive the causes of their students’ performance can affect teachers’ emotions that, in turn, predict their teaching behaviors. Conversely, this theory thus also suggests that the behaviors and emotions conveyed by teachers can be interpreted by students as conveying teachers’ underlying attributional beliefs concerning their students’ performance (Frenzel and Stephens, 2013). Studies suggest that teachers tend to show more anger and disappointment when attributing student failure to low effort, and more empathy, pity, and helplessness to students perceived as low in ability (Butler, 1994; Georgiou et al., 2002a; Woolfson and Brady, 2009). Findings further indicate that teachers’ expression of sympathy following student failure can negatively impact student motivation and performance by implying attributions to low ability, with teachers’ expression of anger instead indirectly contributing to performance improvements by fostering students’ beliefs in their ability to succeed (Graham, 1984a).

In addition to emotions publically expressed by teachers, findings also show teachers’ attributions concerning student achievement to impact their personal emotional experiences. For example, a study by Prawat et al. (1983) with United States primary school teachers found teachers to experience more pride and gratitude when a student perceived as high in effort succeeded as well as higher levels of guilt when a high-ability student failed. In addition, study findings suggested that teachers experience a range of emotions when they perceive a significant change in student effort. More specifically, whereas teachers reported higher levels of pride and happiness when a low-ability student substantially increased their study efforts, they reported feeling more guilt and less gratitude following a perceived drop in effort in high-ability students. Results similarly showed teachers to experience surprise following unanticipated changes in student performance, such as when a high-ability student failed or low-ability student succeeded. These results suggest that teacher emotions are indeed linked with their causal attributions for their students’ performance, likely due to teachers perceiving personal responsibility for the change, and particularly so when changes in students’ study habits or achievement levels are unexpected.

Teachers’ attributions for student performance have also been found to impact teachers’ instructional behaviors. Findings show attributions for poor student performance to low ability to correspond with encouragement of students and willingness to provide assistance, with attributions for student failure to low effort generating more criticism toward students, negative feedback, and a lower tendency to offer help (e.g., suggestions; Cooper and Lowe, 1977; Tollefson and Chen, 1988; Butler, 1994; Matteucci et al., 2008b; Woodcock and Jiang, 2013). Teachers also tended to call on other students to assist and offer expedited help (e.g., providing correct answers) to students perceived as low in ability and responded more strictly to students perceived as low in effort in requiring them to invest more time on future tasks (Butler, 1994). However, a study by Rodriquez and Tollefson (1987) with teachers in Costa Rica found opposite results, with teachers who perceived students as failing due to insufficient effort reporting a greater willingness to help and more positive perceptions of the student (e.g., liking, expected success). Nevertheless, the reasons for these contradictory findings were unclear due to the underlying perceived dimensionality of low effort in Rodriquez and Tollefson’s (1987) study being unknown and the possibility of cultural differences concerning the perceived controllability of low effort.

Studies have also examined the effects of causal attributions on instructional responses to students’ failures with respect to their underlying causal dimensions. Research by Reyna and Weiner (2001) showed that teachers who made attributions for students’ failures to factors they perceived as personally controllable by the student (e.g., laziness, temporary lack of effort) provided not only negative feedback but endorsed retributive punishment goals. In contrast, teachers who believed student failure to result from uncontrollable factors (e.g., transfer student, low ability) or unstable factors (e.g., a temporary decrease in student effort) were found to report more utilitarian punishment goals. Moreover, teachers who believed student failure to be due to factors that were both controllable and stable in nature, such as persistent student laziness, were those most likely to use retributive punishment goals (Reyna and Weiner, 2001, study 2).

Perceptions of Responsibility

An additional important component of the interpersonal aspect of attribution theory is the extent to which individuals perceive others as personally responsible for their experiences. Although perceptions of responsibility are not proposed in Weiner’s model as a subcomponent of, or replacement for, causal attributions, they are nevertheless clearly identified as a proximal critical cognitive consequence of the types of causal attributions teachers that, in turn, influence subsequent emotions and behaviors. As such, whereas the concept of perceived responsibility is not identical to, nor interchangeable with, the preceding causal attributions in Weiner’s theory, they nevertheless are assumed to strongly reflect the types of attributions being made (concerning perceived controllability) and have thus been repeatedly examined as a proxy for teachers’ causal attributions in previous research. Existing research in educational settings has indeed long suggested that when students are held responsible for their failure experiences, they are more likely to be perceived as deserving of punishment as opposed to rewards (Weiner, 2000, 2001). Findings with teachers have since expanded on these findings in showing that when students’ failures are perceived as due to lack of effort rather than lack of ability, teachers are not only more likely to experience greater anger but also believe students to be more responsible for their failure outcome (Matteucci, 2007; also see Matteucci et al., 2008a for a review).

Additionally, findings from Matteucci (2007) show higher levels of perceived student responsibility for failure to lead to retribution-motivated punishment behaviors such as assigning lower marks or more homework. Results further suggested that when student failure was interpreted by teachers as due to lack of ability, teachers who perceived the student to be less personally responsible were more likely to adopt a utilitarian instructional response (e.g., assistance, encouragement). With respect to mediation effects, Reyna and Weiner (2001) further observed that teachers’ attributions to factors controllable by the student on teacher emotions (anger, sympathy) were mediated by perceived student responsibility, with the positive relationship between teachers’ perceptions of student responsibility and retributional punishment goals further mediated by teachers’ emotions (i.e., lower sympathy). Concerning teachers taking personal responsibility for student failures, findings are mixed. Whereas some showing teachers to perceive themselves as less responsible than students for student failures (e.g., regardless of how it is attributed by teachers; Tollefson et al., 1990, 1991; Matteucci, 2007) and others showing teachers to take more responsibility for students’ failures than their successes (e.g., Ross et al., 1974; Cooper and Lowe, 1977; Cooper and Burger, 1980; Burger et al., 1982; Tom and Cooper, 1986).

Relations With Student Outcomes

In addition to research examining empirical links between teachers’ causal attributions for student performance and their own emotions and instructional behaviors, studies have further explored how teachers’ attributions impact their students’ motivation and performance. For example, Natale et al. (2009) found that when teachers made more effort and ability attributions for student success, their students reported higher motivation levels and better performance on reading-related tasks. More interestingly, students’ academic motivation and performance was found, in turn, to influence their teachers’ attributions: The higher a student’s motivation and performance, the stronger the tendency for teachers to attribute the student’s success to high effort and ability. However, when teachers attributed the student’s success to low task difficulty or having provided assistance, lower levels of reading-related motivation and poorer performance were subsequently observed.

Results from a study with kindergarten students and their teachers in Finland further suggested that teachers’ causal attributions can influence student interest in mathematics (Upadyaya et al., 2012). More specifically, this study showed that the greater students’ intrinsic value in mathematics, the more likely their teachers were to attribute math success to factors internal to the student, such as ability and effort, as opposed to external factors such as task easiness. Such internal attributions in turn, contributed to even higher subsequent levels of student intrinsic value and academic performance in mathematics. Similar results were observed in a study with teachers in Estonia (Tõeväli and Kikas, 2016) in which higher levels of student math performance corresponded with a lower likelihood of teachers attributing students’ success in math to teacher assistance and a greater likelihood of attributing it instead to student ability. Nevertheless, study findings also showed that students tended to perform poorly in math when their teachers attributed students’ math successes as due to teacher assistance, with students also found to perform more poorly when teachers attributed students’ math failures to low ability. In other words, poor performance tended to result in teacher attributions to low student ability that, in turn, contributed to even poorer grades thus perpetuating a detrimental attribution-achievement cycle (Tõeväli and Kikas, 2016).

Finally, teachers’ causal attributions for student performance have been found to impact students’ perceptions of their peers by way of teachers’ attributional cues (e.g., verbal communication, emotional expression, body gestures). In one experimental study, students were asked to watch videotapes in which a teacher acted so as to convey attributional cues suggesting either low or high levels of student effort to two students, each of whom had scored 80% correct on a recent exam. Whereas the teacher praised the first student and encouraged them to continue their successful efforts, the second student was criticized for not having invested sufficient effort to perform better. Study findings showed students to not only perceive the first student as smarter (c.f., incremental self-theories of intelligence in which increased ability is assumed to follows from increased effort; see Dweck, 2013), but also indicate a greater preference for the student a study partner (Lord et al., 1990).

Moderating Teacher Variables

With respect to teacher characteristics found to moderate relations between their causal attributions for student achievement and classroom outcomes, findings suggest that teachers with greater teaching experience may be more likely than novice teachers to believe students’ successes or failures to be due to external, uncontrollable factors such as family background or socio-economic status (e.g., parental income, education; Georgiou, 2008). These teachers reported a stronger belief in student achievement as due to uncontrollable factors that are also stable over time, such as biological predispositions (e.g., innate intelligence), thereby de-emphasizing the teachers’ role in, and responsibility for, student achievement relative to novice teachers. In contrast, novice teachers tended to perceive themselves as having a greater impact on their students’ performance and reported stronger beliefs than more experienced teachers in the role of student effort and diligence in student success (Georgiou, 2008). However, contradictory findings were reported by Pirrone (2012) who found although teachers with over 30 years of experience did report a stronger tendency to attribute student failure to student immaturity (internal, uncontrollable), teachers with under 10 years of experience were instead more likely to blame parents or family for student failure (external attributions).

Differences in attributional patterns have also been observed for teachers based on their level of education, with findings showing teachers with university degrees to have a stronger tendency to attribute student failure to factors internal to and controllable by the student, such as lack of interest or motivation to learn, than teachers with a high school degree (Pirrone, 2012). Mixed results have also been reported concerning teachers’ level of instruction as a moderating factor. In early work by Guskey (1982), teachers who taught higher grade levels reported a stronger tendency to attribute student failure to task difficulty, with teachers of lower grade levels instead being more likely to attribute student failure to teachers’ own teaching ability and effort. In contrast, early findings from Hall et al. (1989) showed elementary and junior-high teachers to be more likely than senior-high teachers to attribute student failure both to causes internal to students (e.g., effort, ability, concentration, habits) or to external factors (e.g., task difficulty, home life). More recently, although findings do suggest that junior-high teachers may perceive themselves as more responsible than senior-high teachers when student failures are attributed to lack of student ability, the degree of perceived personal responsibility reported by both types of teachers was still significantly lower than that attributed to students (Matteucci and Gosling, 2004).

Existing research further indicates that teachers’ causal attributions may differ as a function of learning disabilities instruction. For example, Woolfson et al. (2007) found teachers of learning-challenged students at specialized institutions to perceive their students as having more personal control over their academic performance than teachers at mainstream schools who instead tended to make more uncontrollable, stable attributions for student difficulties. Results with British teachers showed teachers with more special education experience to also make more student-external attributions for learning difficulties (e.g., bad luck; Brady and Woolfson, 2008), with another study conducted with teachers in Greece further showing mainstream teachers to be more likely than special education teachers to attribute student failure to student persistent low effort (internal, stable; Vlachou et al., 2014). Finally, studies have examined cultural differences in teachers’ causal attributions, with scattered results suggesting that United States teachers make more effort-related attributions for student performance than German teachers (Kurtz et al., 1990), and that teachers in France perceive themselves as more responsible for student failure than their Italian counterparts (Matteucci and Gosling, 2004).

Moderating Student Variables

The impact of student gender on teacher attributions has consistently been explored in prior research (see Reyna, 2000 for a review). In addition to teachers tending to perceive boys as having higher math ability and girls as more capable with language topics (Georgiou, 2008), teachers additionally tend to attribute girls’ math success to effort and boys’ math success to ability (Fennema et al., 1990; Espinoza et al., 2014). Conversely, findings suggest that teachers are also more likely to attribute poor math performance for females to lack of ability, and for males to lack of effort. Although interventions to counter teachers’ attributional gender biases do show benefits, observed gains tend to be temporary with findings showing biased attributions reported by teachers to return to baseline levels within a year (Espinoza et al., 2014).

Studies also show teachers’ attributions to be influenced by student ethnicity (see Reyna, 2000, 2008 for reviews concerning teacher stereotypes concerning minority students). Specifically, a Netherlands study found teachers to be more likely to attribute satisfactory academic performance to effort for ethnic minority student (vs. ethnic majority students; Wissink and de Haan, 2013), with Canadian research further showing teachers to place greater emphasis on external factors (i.e., family background, socio-economic status) when evaluating the performance of aboriginal students (vs. non-aboriginal student; Riley and Ungerleider, 2012). Studies with United States teachers similarly show teachers to make fewer personally controllable and more external attributions for the performance of African-American students (e.g., family, social environment; Wiley and Eskilson, 1978), with the performance of Caucasian students being more often attributed to student ability, effort, and other personal characteristics (Wiley and Eskilson, 1978; Tom and Cooper, 1986).

Finally, findings indicate that teachers’ attributions for student achievement are likely to differ depending on student learning disabilities (Conway, 1989; Vlachou et al., 2014). Results suggest that for students with learning difficulties, teachers are likely to attribute poor performance more to factors internal to the student, especially to limited ability, followed by low student effort or motivation (Vlachou et al., 2014). In contrast, teachers are less likely to attribute poor performance among students with learning disabilities to teaching methods (e.g., classroom management, curriculum) or student-external factors (e.g., family background) as compared to mainstream students (Conway, 1989). Conversely, findings show teachers tend to attribute academic success for learning-challenged students to low task difficulty or high student effort (Algozzine and Stoller, 1981).

Attributional Disagreement

As implied by the preceding findings, it is possible for teachers and students to differ on not only if an achievement outcome is interpreted as a success or failure, but also on the appropriate causal attribution for a given outcome. Studies suggest that teachers are indeed less certain than students when evaluating if a student’s performance constitutes a failure outcome (Juvonen, 1988), with both students and their parents tending to hold higher performance expectations than their teachers (Bar-Tal and Guttmann, 1981). For example, Juvonen (1988) found teachers’ and students’ attributions for math performance to differ in one third of cases (i.e., 28 of 84) with teachers attributing perceived failures to factors that persisted over time, such as low ability and laziness, and students instead being more inclined to make unstable failure attributions, such as fatigue or sloppiness. Similarly, interviews by Klassen and Lynch (2007) with students with learning disabilities and their teachers showed whereas students predominantly attributed their academic failures to unstable and controllable factors (i.e., lack of effort), most of the teachers interviewed attributed students’ failure to internal, uncontrollable, and stable factors (i.e., lack of ability). Concerning student success, findings from Juvonen (1988) again showed disagreement between the attributions reported by students and teachers, with teachers demonstrating a stronger tendency to attribute students’ success to stable factors, such as interest in math, and students instead reporting more attributions to unstable or external factors, such test-taking mood or support from parents.

Interpersonal Attributions: Student Misbehavior

Attribution Prevalence

In addition to research examining teachers’ causal attributions for student achievement, studies have further explored how teachers attribute students’ behavior in class, particularly with respect to perceived misbehavior. Early work by Medway (1979) comparing teachers’ attributions for students’ learning difficulties (e.g., reading, understanding) and behavioral challenges (e.g., aggression, withdrawal) found that teachers primarily attributed learning problems to ability-related factors, while attributing behavioral problems to peer and parental factors (see also Arcia et al., 2000). Findings have additionally shown that whereas severity of student learning difficulties was positively correlated with teachers’ attributions to student ability (e.g., intelligence), severity of student behavioral problems instead corresponded to more personality attributions (Medway, 1979). Other studies have similarly revealed that whereas ability and effort tend to be the most commonly cited reasons for student learning problems, personality and parental factors are more often endorsed as reasons for behavioral difficulties (e.g., Medway, 1979; Hughes et al., 1993; Savina et al., 2014).

Despite these differences, findings concerning teachers’ attributional tendencies for student misbehavior are largely similar to those observed regarding attributions for low achievement. When faced with student behavioral problems (e.g., disobedience, disruption, aggression; mild, moderate, severe), teachers’ attributions tend to not implicate themselves (e.g., instructional method, curriculum, teacher attitudes, discipline) or administrative factors (e.g., teaching demands, class size) but instead consistently cite factors internal to the student (e.g., ability, effort, personality, social/physical skills) and parental variables (e.g., dysfunction, attitudes, interest; Vernberg and Medway, 1981; Christenson et al., 1983; Miller, 1995; Bibou-Nakou et al., 2000; Mavropoulou and Padeliadu, 2002; Ho, 2004; Kulinna, 2007; McAuliffe et al., 2009; Andreou and Rapti, 2010; Ding et al., 2010; Kauppi and Porhola, 2012; Savina et al., 2014). For example, whereas more than half of the teachers in Ding et al.’s (2010) study attributed students’ misbehaviors to students’ lack of effort, with substantial proportions citing various other factors internal to students (e.g., laziness: 32%, study habits: 23%, low interest: 20%), only 0.8% of teachers attributed students’ behavioral difficulties to their own instructional effectiveness or classroom management skills.

Similarly, Kleftaras and Didaskalou (2006) found teachers to primarily attribute student behavioral and emotional problems to family issues (74%) or student-related factors (17%; e.g., biological or psychological deficits), with only 6% of teachers reporting school context or teaching practices as important contributors. A study by Savina et al. (2014) that specifically examined teachers’ attributions for students’ externalizing behaviors (e.g., disruption, aggression) also showed problematic parental and peer relationships to be most often cited by teachers as critical determinants of students’ overt classroom misbehavior. However, although teachers have consistently been found to perceive the causes of student misbehavior as beyond their personal control and scope of responsibility (e.g., Bibou-Nakou et al., 2000; for atypical contradictory results, see Poulou and Norwich, 2000), research by Hughes et al. (1993) suggests that teachers may nonetheless perceive themselves as responsible for the success of their subsequent in-class behavior management efforts. Additionally, whereas parental factors are often cited by teachers as responsible for students’ classroom misbehavior (e.g., Soodak and Podell, 1994), other research suggests that teachers do not necessarily perceive parents as helpful for managing student behavior (Miller, 1995; also see Wiley et al., 2012 for a review on parents’ and teachers’ attributions concerning misbehaviors).

Relations With Teacher and Student Outcomes

Findings suggest that when teachers attribute student aggression to factors that are internal to students and stable over time, they also tend to report more maladaptive teaching behaviors (e.g., expressing anger, sarcasm) that, in turn, lead to greater peer dislike of the student (McAuliffe et al., 2009). Additionally, when teachers believed that students’ behavioral problems as due to student intelligence or other factors perceived as beyond the teachers’ own control, they also reported a stronger tendency to seek professional consultation or special education services (Soodak and Podell, 1994). Concerning teachers’ attributions for student disruptive behavior to factors external to themselves (e.g., students’ family-related issues, student internal factors), findings suggest that such attributions may reflect lower levels of teacher motivation (i.e., self-efficacy) in dealing with behavioral difficulties as well as greater efforts to seek out parental assistance (Soodak and Podell, 1994). Conversely, teachers who attribute student misbehavior to teaching- or class-related reasons have been found to report not only greater sympathy for and confidence in students with emotional and behavioral difficulties but also a greater willingness to improve their teaching strategies (Soodak and Podell, 1994) as well as to personally intervene and provide assistance (Hughes et al., 1993; Poulou and Norwich, 2000). Research further suggests that whereas teachers’ attributions for student misbehavior to factors internal to students tend to have mixed relations with teacher burnout indices (i.e., higher levels of both personal accomplishment and emotional exhaustion), attributions to contextual factors (e.g., classroom environment) are associated with lower teacher burnout (i.e., lower depersonalization; Bibou-Nakou et al., 1999).

Moderating Variables

Similar to research on teachers’ attributions for student performance, findings indicate that greater teaching experience tends to correspond with a greater likelihood of teachers perceiving students and family-related factors as more important contributors to students’ behavioral problems than school- or teacher-related issues (Mavropoulou and Padeliadu, 2002). However, other research more specifically suggests that older teachers (>47 years) as well as younger teachers (<35 years) are more likely to endorse parenting-related attributions for students’ emotional and behavioral problems than middle-aged teachers, with this intermediate group assumed to hold differing attributional views due to having more experience than younger teachers yet holding less conservative views toward instruction than older teachers (Kleftaras and Didaskalou, 2006). With respect to the moderating role of level of instruction, senior high teachers have been found to demonstrate a stronger tendency to attribute student misbehavior to non-academic factors beyond their personal control than junior high and elementary school teachers (e.g., parental skills, community environment; Kulinna, 2007). In addition, elementary school teachers were found to be more likely to attribute students’ misbehaviors to poor learning habits, whereas middle and high school teachers tended to attribute students’ misbehaviors to low effort (Ding et al., 2010).

Multiple studies have also examined cultural differences as potential moderators of teachers’ attributions for behavioral difficulties. For example, research by Ho (2004) found that whereas Australian teachers focused on student ability, Chinese teachers instead placed greater emphasis on family issues. Findings from Gibbs and Gardiner (2008) further suggested that British teachers were more likely than Irish teachers to attribute challenging student behaviors to factors external to the student such as teacher behavior, parental involvement, or class curriculum. As for student ethnicity, results from Jackson (2002) indicated that teachers more frequently provided situational explanations (e.g., other people) for the misbehavior of Caucasian students, whereas internal explanations (e.g., student personal characteristics) were more often reported for non-Caucasian American students. Finally, studies have also found student gender to moderate teachers’ attributions, with mixed results suggesting that teachers may be more likely to attribute female students’ misbehavior to internal and uncontrollable factors (e.g., personality characteristics; Savina et al., 2014), or more inclined to attribute male students’ behavioral problems to unintentional (i.e., uncontrollable) factors (Arbeau and Coplan, 2007).

Attributional Disagreement

Multiple studies comparing teachers’ and their students’ causal attributions concerning students’ classroom difficulties have observed disagreement between each set of reports. For example, in an interview study with secondary school students and their physical education teachers in the United States, it was found that teachers were more likely than their students to attribute misbehavior to stable factors, such as family-related reasons, whereas students tended to report attributions to unstable factors, such as attention-seeking or boredom (Cothran et al., 2009). In a vignette study by Guttmann (1982), whereas students tended to attribute another student’s misbehavior to contextual features (e.g., teacher attitudes, peer influence), teachers were more likely to attribute misbehavior to factors internal to the student (e.g., students’ psychological problems, need for teacher’s attention). Comparable findings were found in a study by Hui (2001) comparing Hong Kong students’ and teachers’ causal attributions toward students’ classroom difficulties, showing students’ to be more likely to attribute their problems to teacher- or school-related factors (e.g., teacher bias, school rules), and teachers instead tending to blame students, peers, or parents for students’ difficulties. Other findings have further shown teachers to be more likely than students’ parents to not only rate behavioral problems as serious in nature but also as resulting from underlying biological factors (Savina et al., 2014).

Intrapersonal Attributions: Occupational Stress

Attribution Prevalence

Beyond studies exploring the prevalence, correlates, and potential moderators of teachers’ interpersonal attributions for student outcomes, limited research has also explored the implications of teachers’ intrapersonal attributions for their own teaching-related stress. Research on teachers’ causal attributions for their occupational stress found that teachers most frequently attributed their stress to external and uncontrollable factors and were least likely to report occupational stress as due to internal reasons (Wang et al., 2015). More specifically, studies suggest that teachers primarily attribute their occupational stress to administrative factors (e.g., excessive workload, lack of respect, lack of time), structural issues (e.g., high student–teacher ratio; McCormick and Solman, 1992a,b; McCormick, 1997; Manassero et al., 2006), student reasons (e.g., low student motivation), and were less likely to attribute stress to their own classroom management or instructional skills (Manassero et al., 2006). Finally, cross-cultural research by McCormick and Shi (1999) suggests that although both Australian and Chinese teachers make primarily external attributions for teaching-related stress, Australian teachers tend to focus on bureaucratic factors (e.g., government) whereas Chinese teachers tended to blame societal issues.

Relations With Teacher Outcomes

In a study by Manassero et al. (2006), teachers’ causal attributions for personal stress were investigated with respect to three attributional dimensions outlined in Weiner’s (1985) attribution theory (locus, stability, controllability) and two additional dimensions derived from earlier theoretical formulations: intentionality and globality. Whereas intentionality refers to the conscious, deliberate nature of a personally controllable attribution (e.g., forethought as to potential consequences), globality refers to the situational scope of a given attribution (i.e., domain-general vs. context-specific; Abramson et al., 1978). Study results showed teachers who made more stable, global, or intentional attributions for their teaching-related stress to also report higher levels of burnout (emotional exhaustion, depersonalization). Further, although internal or uncontrollable attributions corresponded with greater burnout (exhaustion and depersonalization, respectively), teachers who reported more unstable, controllable, or specific (not global) attributions for teaching stress reported higher levels of personal accomplishment.

Furthermore, findings also show significant relations between teachers’ attributions for teaching-related stress and psychosocial outcomes beyond occupational burnout. For example, a study with Canadian teachers by Wang et al. (2015) showed that although attributions for teaching stress to internal or personally controllable causes did correlate with higher job satisfaction, attributions to personally controllable factors further corresponded to lower emotional exhaustion, fewer illness symptoms, and lower quitting intentions. In contrast, teachers who attributed their occupational stress to stable reasons, or factors under the control of others, tended to reported poorer well-being outcomes (e.g., burnout, job satisfaction, illness, quitting intentions). Comparable findings were also observed by McCormick and Barnett (2011) who found that teachers who blamed students for their occupational stress reported higher depersonalization and emotional exhaustion, whereas teachers who blamed themselves or their students also tended to have lower levels of personal accomplishment. Results were also found with the dimension locus of causality, with teachers who blamed external factors involving government or educational policies for their occupational stress reporting higher emotional exhaustion. Moreover, whereas teachers who reported external factors as primarily responsible for their occupational stress reported lower job satisfaction and greater burnout, particularly those who implicated students as their primary source of stress (e.g., lack of administrative support; McCormick, 1997; McCormick and Barnett, 2011), teachers who instead reported a willingness to take personal responsibility for their shortcomings reported higher job satisfaction (McCormick, 1997).

Discussion

As evidenced by the scope of studies reviewed, there exists a long and international history of empirical research on the prevalence and consequences of teachers’ causal attributions for varied instructional challenges. With respect to findings applying an interpersonal approach to Weiner’s (2000) attribution theory, existing research has principally explored the types and correlates of attributions made by teachers for students’ academic performance and misbehavior. In contrast, research on teachers’ intrapersonal attributions for their own occupational stress is limited yet encouraging in showing significant relations with teacher well-being. Consistent with attribution theory, empirical findings indeed showed teachers’ causal attributions to correspond with not only their teaching-related emotions and instructional behaviors, but also observed outcomes in students, with these relations having been further explored with respect to moderating student and teacher variables. Overall, Weiner’s attribution theory has for decades provided a useful, foundational framework within which to examine the implications of teachers’ causal attributions for personal and instructional outcomes, with the pattern of results that emerge across these studies suggesting consistent themes as outlined below.

Prevalence of Teachers’ Attributions: Common Themes

Fundamental Attribution Error

In social psychology research, it has been argued that individuals tend to interpret events in stereotyped ways and as such are motivated to overemphasize the role of dispositions in causal thinking about others (Ross, 1977). Referred to as the fundamental attribution error, this bias involves “the tendency for attributors to underestimate the impact of situational factors and to overestimate the role of dispositional factors in controlling behavior” (Ross, 1977; p. 183). Moreover, this attributional hypothesis further asserts that the “actors” and “observers” of an event consistently differ in the types of attributions they select, with the actors having a greater tendency to attribute behaviors to situational forces or constraints, and observers instead being more inclined to make attributions implicating the actor’s abilities, dispositions, and attitudes (also referred to as “actor–observer bias”; Jones and Nisbett, 1971). By way of explanation, it is commonly asserted that such interpretation patterns serve to enhance self-esteem, minimize negative affect, and also defend against ego threats and compromised perceptions of one’s abilities (Heider, 1958; Jones and Davis, 1965; Ross, 1977).

As outlined above, the fundamental attribution error is clearly evident in research examining the types of attributions made by teachers for student performance, misbehavior, and teaching stress. Moreover, the present review underscores the double-edged nature of biased attributions in showing teachers to not only report self-protective attributions in failure situations but also self-enhancing attributions following success (hedonic bias; Miller and Ross, 1975). Across multiple empirical investigations, teachers have consistently been found to attribute student difficulties (i.e., poor academic performance, behavioral difficulties) to student-related or family-related reasons, and much less frequently report perceiving the problem as teaching-related. Further, teachers and students are often found to report different attributions for students’ academic challenges, with students tending to see their personal failures as temporary and controllable and teachers instead regarding such outcomes as more serious, persistent, and not controllable by students. In addition to teachers’ interpersonal attributions for student outcomes, this ego-defensive attributional pattern is also reflected in the attributions teachers make for their own occupational stress. As suggested by McCormick and Solman (1992a,b) and Wang et al. (2015), teachers also tend to attribute teaching-related stress to factors external to themselves (e.g., students, society, government) presumably in an effort to mitigate negative emotions as well as the implication of teaching-related setbacks as personal failures.

However, critics contend that an ego-defensive explanation for this attributional pattern should be viewed with skepticism, and instead propose a simpler account for this attributional asymmetry. More specifically, it is postulated that personal successes are simply perceived as more likely to occur than failure due to positive self-perceptions requiring quick cognitive access to prior success experiences, with failure-related memories instead being more difficult to cognitively access. As such, whereas personal successes (e.g., a student succeeding) are more likely to be attributed to self-relevant factors (e.g., teaching strategies), personal failures (e.g., a student performing poorly) are less easily attributable to internal factors and thus more likely to be attributed to readily external variables (e.g., observable characteristics of the student). This tendency to attribute student failure to factors internal to the student is then further exacerbated by teachers, as external observers, being less aware than their students of the situational contributors to student failure (Miller and Ross, 1975).

Accordingly, further research is needed to examine teachers’ motivations underlying the types of attributions they select, for example, whether external attributions mitigate the effects of perceived ego threat on subsequent emotions or instead mediate the effects of limited situational awareness on subsequent instructional choices. Moreover, further research on factors that moderate this attributional bias is warranted given findings with teachers that did not demonstrate the fundamental attribution bias, but rather the opposite. For example, results from studies by Burger et al. (1982) showed teachers to in fact be more willing to assume responsibility for student failure than for their success. Findings from Rodriquez and Tollefson (1987) also showed teachers to be more willing to offer assistance to students to whom they attributed low effort rather than low ability. An earlier study by Ross et al. (1974) further showed teachers to rate student ability and aptitude as more important determinants of student success than student failure, and were more likely to attribute student failure as due to teacher ability, aptitude, and motivation.

Moderating Factors

Beyond overall patterns concerning the types of causal attributions made by teachers, studies have also consistently explored potential moderators of attribution prevalence – most notably with respect to teaching experience. More specifically, findings from multiple studies suggest that more experienced teachers tend to make more uncontrollable attributions for students’ academic failure (e.g., innate intelligence, family background; Georgiou, 2008) and misbehavior (e.g., poor parenting; Kleftaras and Didaskalou, 2006). As an explanation for this trend, Georgiou (2008) suggests that novice teachers may hold unrealistic beliefs in their ability to effectively intervene and improve their students’ performance through personal perseverance and encouraging students to invest greater effort. However, it is presumed that as teachers gain more experience they become more realistic and more comfortable with the belief that not all students can succeed due to various uncontrollable and stable factors that can significantly undermine their efforts (Pirrone, 2012). Alternatively, it has been asserted that because teachers with greater experience are more competent and more certain that their instructional strategies are sufficient for student success, they instead focus on factors unrelated to their instructional methods as responsible for student difficulties due to already having maximized the potential impact of their teaching methods (Georgiou, 2008). Additionally, findings suggest that the attributions made by teachers of students with special needs can differ depending on experience in that although teachers generally attribute the failure of special needs students to ability and success to effort (e.g., Algozzine and Stoller, 1981), teachers with more special education experience are more likely to see student failure as more controllable by the student (Woolfson et al., 2007) and acknowledge the role of factors external to the student (e.g., Brady and Woolfson, 2008).

Concerning the moderating effects of level of instruction, it has been suggested that as content difficulty increases, students’ academic performance and behavior are unlikely to be based on high effort alone due to the increasingly evident role of uncontrollable factors such as student intelligence, maturity, or family background. Accordingly, teachers at higher grade levels tend to feel less personally responsible for the performance and behavior of their students than teachers of lower grade levels (Mavropoulou and Padeliadu, 2002; Matteucci and Gosling, 2004). Furthermore, teachers were found to also make different attributions based on student demographics. More specifically, whereas teachers have been found to be more likely to attribute girls’ successes to high effort and failures to low ability, they instead tend to attribute boy’s successes to high ability and failures to low effort (e.g., Espinoza et al., 2014). Similarly, although a girl’s classroom misbehavior is likely to be attributed by teachers to internal and uncontrollable factors (e.g., personality), misbehavior by boys is more often viewed an unintentional (e.g., Arbeau and Coplan, 2007).

Moreover, teachers tend to believe the academic performance of ethnic minority students to be due to factors that are external to and uncontrollable by the students (e.g., family background), and instead tend to make student-internal attributions for the academic performance of ethnic majority students (e.g., ability, effort; Wiley and Eskilson, 1978; Riley and Ungerleider, 2012). Concerning student misbehaviors, however, teachers also shown a tendency to believe behavioral problems evidenced by ethnic minority students to be due to factors internal to the students (e.g., student characteristics, personality), with similar behavior exhibited by ethnic majority students’ instead attributed primarily to situational or external factors (e.g., influence of other people; Jackson, 2002). In sum, teachers’ causal attributions have been found to be significantly impacted by their experiences interacting with students as well as their existing knowledge, stereotypes, and perceptions concerning their students. Thus whereas overall findings concerning attribution prevalence suggests that teachers’ attributions may be systematically biased (e.g., fundamental attribution error), this pattern may be mitigated by specific teacher and student characteristics.

Implications of Teachers’ Attributions for Instruction and Well-Being

Students and Instruction

With respect to the potential influences of teachers’ causal attributions on students, attribution theory suggests that student motivation and achievement can indeed be influenced by teachers’ causal attributions by way of attributional cues (e.g., emotional expression, body languages, verbal feedback; see Graham, 1984a,b, 1990; Weiner, 1985, 2000, 2010; Graham and Williams, 2009). Moreover, research on the self-fulfilling prophecy asserts that teachers’ inaccurate causal attributions concerning low ability can develop into persistent stereotypes that lead teachers to expect less of these students, interpret subsequent outcomes as conforming to this stereotype, and interact with these students in a manner that elicits further poor performance (Jussim and Harber, 2005; Jussim et al., 2009). Overall, there exists a substantial existing literature suggesting that teachers’ attributional beliefs concerning the causes of students’ classroom behavior and performance can significantly impact subsequent student outcomes (e.g., Cooper and Burger, 1980; Rejeski and McCook, 1980; Fennema et al., 1990; Lord et al., 1990).

In the current review, these theories were supported by findings showing teachers who attributed their students’ performance to low ability to report more sympathy and lower expectations for these students, as well as greater use of instructional techniques intended to punish as opposed to assist low-ability students (e.g., Butler, 1994; Georgiou et al., 2002a). Students perceived as low ability, in turn, were also not only more likely to attribute their failure to limited ability but report lower expectations for success in their studies (Graham, 1984a). Conversely, teachers who perceived student failure as due to low effort were found to experience greater anger toward these students and sustained or increased performance expectations (Butler, 1994; Georgiou et al., 2002a) and further preferred correctional methods that were utilitarian and informative in nature (Reyna and Weiner, 2001). Poor-performing students perceived as lacking in effort, in turn, perceived teachers’ anger as indicating that improvement was possible and reported more positive success expectations (Graham, 1984a). However, it is important to note that as prior empirical studies have focused specifically on teachers (e.g., self-report attributions, expectations, emotions) or students (e.g., self-report success expectations, perceived teacher attributions) in exploring the influences of teachers’ attributions and affective cues on students, research assessing both teachers and students simultaneously is lacking. Accordingly, the specific theoretical assumptions in Weiner’s (1985, 2000, 2010) theory concerning possible links between interpersonal and intrapersonal attributions have yet to be empirically examined.

Teacher Well-Being

Studies concerning the effects of attributions on teachers’ well-being have yielded mixed yet encouraging results. More specifically, internal attributions concerning teachers’ occupational stress have been found to have both negative effects (e.g., Manassero et al., 2006; McCormick and Barnett, 2011) and positive effects on well-being outcomes (e.g., job satisfaction; Wang et al., 2015), with external attributions for stress showing similarly mixed results (e.g., Bibou-Nakou et al., 1999; McCormick and Barnett, 2011). Nevertheless, findings on the effects of personally controllable attributions on well-being are notably consistent in showing teachers who focus on the controllable aspects of their occupational stress to report lower burnout, fewer illness symptoms, greater job satisfaction, and weaker intentions to quit (McCormick, 1997; Manassero et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2015). Moreover, it has been proposed that the conceptual overlap between personally controllable attributions and internal causal attributions may be responsible for the mixed effects of the latter in existing research. According to Wang et al. (2015), as attributions that are internal to an individual may be either controllable (e.g., effort) or uncontrollable in nature (e.g., ability), analyses that evaluate both internality and controllability simultaneously may show negative effects for internality due to multicollinearity. More specifically, studies in which personal controllability is evaluated alongside internality may in fact be statistically removing the beneficial elements of internality by controlling for personally controllable aspects (e.g., stress due to insufficient effort), leaving the uncontrollable elements of internality to be assessed (e.g., low ability as responsible for occupational stress).

Future Directions

Results from the current review suggest that teachers’ causal attributions can influence their emotions and, in turn, instructional behaviors that have significant implications for students’ academic performance, motivation, and behaviors. However, whereas specific links between teachers’ attributions and their interactions with students have been empirically explored, notably few studies have examined the actual effects of teachers’ attributions on their students by examining both teacher and student data simultaneously. Additionally, in contrast to the majority of published studies on teacher attributions having focused primarily on the implications of attributions for students’ problems (performance, behaviors), little research has been conducted to date concerning the effects of teachers’ attributions on their personal well-being. Accordingly, further research is needed in which the correspondence between reports by both teachers and students is examined. Other than that, research is also needed concerning the influences of teachers’ attributions for occupational stressors and specific classroom experiences on their psychological and physical health to provide a more thorough examination of the implications of teachers’ causal attributions for not only their students’ but also teachers’ own personal development.

Another limitation of the research reviewed is the methodological overreliance on predetermined lists of specific teacher attributions, namely student ability and effort (e.g., Hall et al., 1989; Tollefson et al., 1990; Natale et al., 2009). Accordingly, future studies in which measures that more accurately capture Weiner’s (1985, 2001, 2010) attribution theory by measuring the underlying attributional dimensions are encouraged (e.g., stable vs. persistent lack of student effort, Reyna and Weiner, 2001; for further examples, see Manassero et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2015). Moreover, as studies have to date explored teacher attributions using inconsistent self-report measures (e.g., validated scales with general populations or teachers, self-designed measures, vignettes, interviews; McAuley et al., 1992; Reyna and Weiner, 2001; McCormick et al., 2006; Klassen and Lynch, 2007; Ding et al., 2010), greater research efforts to validate a more comprehensive measure to examine teachers’ attributions concerning both interpersonal challenges (e.g., student difficulties) and intrapersonal struggles (e.g., occupational stress) are warranted to broaden and systematize our understanding of how teachers interpret various classroom and occupational experiences. Similarly, the development and assessment of outcome measures that do not rely solely on self-report methods (questionnaires, interviews) are needed to more objectively examine the real-world impact of teacher attributions on instructional behavior and well-being (e.g., classroom observations, physiological measures; Schwerdtfeger et al., 2008).

With respect to additional methodological considerations, the present overreliance on self-report measures in existing work on teachers’ attributions also warrants consideration of the possibility that empirical relations found between teacher attributions, emotions, and behaviors may be inflated due to common method variance. More specifically, given that prior studies have primarily employed retrospective self-report techniques to assess teachers’ trait-like causal attributions, as compared to more ecologically valid indicators (i.e., experience sampling measures; Goetz et al., 2015), future research employing more objective, real-time measures of teacher attributions that are less confounded by response (e.g., hindsight) bias are needed to underscore the validity of observed results. Furthermore, an existing overemphasis on cross-sectional methods in prior studies on teachers’ causal attributions necessitates future research efforts in which longitudinal designs are administered (for related research on emotions in educational settings, see Pekrun et al., 2014; Wang et al., 2017) to better elucidate causal relations between teachers’ attributions, emotions, and instructional behaviors.

Finally, initiatives aimed at promoting teacher motivation and well-being are encouraged to incorporate attribution-based interventions to counter teachers’ maladaptive attributional tendencies and biases. One such program is Attributional Retraining (AR); an intervention typically administered to students that encourages causal attributions for failure experiences that are personally controllable in nature, while at the same time discouraging attributions that either absolve responsibility through external attributions or focus instead on internal, uncontrollable factors (e.g., low ability; for reviews, see Haynes et al., 2009). Whether addressing teachers’ attributions for student outcomes (interpersonal) or their own teaching-related setbacks (intrapersonal), attribution-informed interventions such as AR are expected to prove effective in facilitating adaptive changes in teachers’ willingness to assist students (e.g., Tollefson and Chen, 1988; Matteucci et al., 2008b; Woodcock and Jiang, 2013) as well as their own psychological and physical health (e.g., see Hall et al., 2007 for similar AR methods with students; Wang et al., 2015).

Author Contributions

HW conducted the literature review, screened, synthesized, as well as scholarly wrote the manuscript. NH supervised the project, reviewed and edited the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This review was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC; #767-2016-1604 and #435-2013-1099).

References

Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E., and Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 87, 49–74. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.87.1.49

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Algozzine, B., and Stoller, L. (1981). Effects of labels and competence on teachers’ attributions for a student. J. Exp. Educ. 49, 132–136. doi: 10.1080/00220973.1981.11011776

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ames, C., and Ames, R. (1984). Systems of student and teacher motivation: toward a qualitative definition. J. Educ. Psychol. 76, 535–556. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.76.4.535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ames, R. (1975). Teachers’ attributions of responsibility: some unexpected nondefensive effects. J. Educ. Psychol. 67, 668–676. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.67.5.668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreou, E., and Rapti, A. (2010). Teachers’ causal attributions for behaviour problems and perceived efficacy for class management in relation to selected interventions. Behav. Change 27, 53–67. doi: 10.1375/bech.27.1.53

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbeau, K. A., and Coplan, R. J. (2007). Kindergarten teachers’ beliefs and responses to hypothetical prosocial, asocial, and antisocial children. Merrill Palmer Q. 53, 291–318. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2007.0007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arcia, E., Frank, R., Sánchez-LaCay, A., and Fernández, M. C. (2000). Teacher understanding of ADHD as reflected in attributions and classroom strategies. J. Atten. Disord. 4, 91–101. doi: 10.1177/108705470000400203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An Introduction to Motivation. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

Google Scholar

Bar-Tal, D., and Darom, E. (1979). Pupils’ attributions of success and failure. Child Dev. 50, 264–267. doi: 10.2307/1129069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Tal, D., and Guttmann, J. (1981). A comparison of teachers’, pupils’ and parents’ attributions regarding pupils’ academic achievements. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 51, 301–311. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1981.tb02488.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., Raviv, A., and Levit, R. (1981). Teachers’ reactions to attributions of ability and effort and their predictions of students’ reactions. Educ. Psychol. 1, 231–240. doi: 10.1080/0144341810010303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beckman, L. J. (1976). Causal attributions of teachers and parents regarding children’s performance. Psychol. Schools 13, 212–218. doi: 10.1002/1520-6807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertrand, M., and Marsh, J. A. (2015). Teachers’ sensemaking of data and implications for equity. Am. Educ. Res. J. 52, 861–893. doi: 10.3102/0002831215599251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bibou-Nakou, I., Stogiannidou, A., and Kiosseoglou, G. (1999). The relation between teacher burnout and teachers’ attributions and practices regarding school behaviour problems. School Psychol. Int. 20, 209–217. doi: 10.1177/0143034399020002004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bibou-Nakou, L., Kiosseoglou, G., and Stogiannidou, A. (2000). Elementary teachers’ perceptions regarding school behavior problems: implications for school psychological services. Psychol. Schools 37, 123–137. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1520-6807(200003)37:2<123::AID-PITS4>3.0.CO;2-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brady, K., and Woolfson, L. (2008). What teacher factors influence their attributions for children’s difficulties in learning? Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 78, 527–544. doi: 10.1348/000709907X268570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broverman, I. K., Broverman, D., Clarkson, F. E., Rosenkrantz, P. S., and Vogel, S. (1970). Sex-role stereotypes and clinical judgements of mental health. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 34, 1–7. doi: 10.1037/h0028797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burger, J. M., Cooper, H. M., and Good, T. L. (1982). Teacher attributions of student performance: effects of outcome. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 8, 685–690. doi: 10.1177/0146167282084013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, R. (1994). Teacher communications and student interpretations: effects of teacher responses to failing students on attributional inferences in two age groups. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 64, 277–294. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1994.tb01102.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, R. (2012). Striving to connect: extending an achievement goal approach to teacher motivation to include relational goals for teaching. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 726–742. doi: 10.1037/a0028613

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, M., Kurtz, B. E., Schneider, W., Turner, L. A., and Borkowski, J. G. (1989). Strategy acquisition and transfer among american and german children: environmental influences on metacognitive development. Dev. Psychol. 25, 765–771. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.5.765

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, L. M., Williford, A. P., and LoCasale-Crouch, J. (2014). Reliability and validity of a measure of preschool teachers’ attributions for disruptive behavior. Early Educ. Dev. 25, 949–972. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2014.898358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, M. L. (2009). An appraisal perspective of teacher burnout: examining the emotional work of teachers. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 21, 193–218. doi: 10.1007/s10648-009-9106-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chedzoy, S., and Burden, R. (2009). Primary school children’s reflections on physical education lessons: an attributional analysis and possible implications for teacher action. Think. Skills Creat. 4, 185–193. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2009.09.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christenson, S., Wang, J. J. E., and Algozzine, B. (1983). Teachers’ attributions for problems that result in referral for psychoeducational evaluation. J. Educ. Res. 76, 174–180. doi: 10.1080/00220671.1983.10885445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, M. D. (1997). Teacher response to learning disability: a test of attributional principles. J. Learn. Disabil. 30, 69–79. doi: 10.1177/002221949703000106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, A. (1989). Teachers’ explanations for children with learning difficulties: an analysis of written reports. Early Child Dev. Care 53, 53–61. doi: 10.1080/0300443890530107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, H. M., and Burger, J. M. (1980). How teachers explain students’ academic performance: a categorization of free response academic attributions. Am. Educ. Res. J. 17, 95–109. doi: 10.3102/00028312017001095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, H. M., and Lowe, C. A. (1977). Task information and attributions for academic performance by professional teachers and roleplayers. J. Pers. 45, 469–483. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1977.tb00165.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coplan, R. J., Hastings, P. D., Lagace-Seguin, D. G., and Moulton, C. E. (2002). Authoritative and authoritarian mothers’ parenting goals, attributions, and emotions across different childrearing contexts. Parent. Sci. Pract. 2, 1–26. doi: 10.1207/S15327922PAR0201_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cothran, D. J., Kulinna, P. H., and Garrahy, D. A. (2009). Attributions for and consequences of student misbehavior. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagogy 14, 155–167. doi: 10.1080/17408980701712148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, M.-X., Li, Y.-P., Li, X.-B., and Kulmb, G. (2010). Chinese teachers’ attributions and coping strategies for student classroom misbehavior. Asia Pac. J. Educ. 30, 321–337. doi: 10.1080/02188791.2010.495832

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobbs, J., and Arnold, D. H. (2009). Relationship between preschool teachers’ reports of children’s behavior and their behavior toward those children. School Psychol. Q. 24, 95–105. doi: 10.1037/a0016157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, N. (2000). Self-presentation strategies and social judgments – desirability and social utility of causal explanations. Swiss J. Psychol. 59, 170–182. doi: 10.1024//1421-0185.59.3.170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, N., and Le Poultier, F. (1991). “Internalité et évaluation scolaire,” in Perspectives Cognitives Et Conduites Sociales, Vol. 3, eds J.-L. Beauvois, R.-V. Joule, and J.-M. Monteil (Neuchatel: Delachaux et Niestle), 153–166.

Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S. (2013). Self-Theories: Their Role in Motivation, Personality, and Development. New York, NY: Psychology Press. doi: 10.4324/9781315783048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emmerich, W., Rock, D. A., and Trapani, C. S. (2004). Multiple Personality Domains in Relation to Occupational Choice and Performance Among Established Teachers. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

Google Scholar

Emmerich, W., Rock, D. A., and Trapani, C. S. (2006). Personality in relation to occupational outcomes among established teachers. J. Res. Personal. 40, 501–528. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espinoza, P., Arêas da Luz Fontes, A. B., and Arms-Chavez, C. J. (2014). Attributional gender bias: teachers’ ability and effort explanations for students’ math performance. Soc. Psychol. Educ. Int. J. 17, 105–126. doi: 10.1007/s11218-013-9226-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felson, R. B. (1980). Physical attractiveness, grades and teachers’ attributions of ability. Represent. Res. Soc. Psychol. 11, 64–71.

Google Scholar

Fennema, E., Peterson, P. L., Carpenter, T. P., and Lubinski, C. A. (1990). Teachers’ attributions and beliefs about girls, boys, and mathematics. Educ. Stud. Math. 21, 55–69. doi: 10.1007/BF00311015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsterling, F. (1985). Attributional retraining: a review. Psychol. Bull. 98, 495–512. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.3.495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C. (2014). “Teacher emotions,” in International Handbook of Emotions in Education, eds E. A. Linnenbrink-Garcia and R. Pekrun (New York, NY: Routledge), 494–519.

Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C., Becker-Kruz, B., Pekrun, R., and Goetz, T. (2015). Teaching this class drives me nuts! – examining the person and context specificity of teacher emotions. PLoS One 10:e0129630. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., and Jacob, B. (2009). “Antecedents and effects of teachers’ emotional experiences: an integrated perspective and empirical test,” in Advances in Teacher Emotion Research: The Impact on Teachers’ Lives, eds P. A. Schutz and M. Zembylas (New York, NY: Springer), 129–152.

Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C., and Stephens, E. J. (2013). “Emotions,” in Emotion, Motivation, and Self-regulation: A Handbook for Teachers, eds N. C. Hall and T. Goetz (Bingley: Emerald), 1–56.

Google Scholar

Georgiou, S. N. (2008). Beliefs of experienced and novice teachers about achievement. Educ. Psychol. 28, 119–131. doi: 10.1080/01443410701468716

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgiou, S. N., Christou, C., Stavrinides, P., and Panaoura, G. (2002a). Teacher attributions of student failure and teacher behavior toward the failing student. Psychol. Schools 39, 583–595. doi: 10.1002/pits.10049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgiou, S. N., Stavrinides, P., and Panaoura, G. (2002b). School failure: teacher attributions and behavior. Educ. Rev. 33, 115–136.

PubMed Abstract

Ghanizadeh, A., and Ghonsooly, B. (2014). A tripartite model of EFL teacher attributions, burnout, and self-regulation: toward the prospects of effective teaching. Educ. Res. Policy Pract. 13, 145–166. doi: 10.1007/s10671-013-9155-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghanizadeh, A., and Ghonsooly, B. (2015). Designing and validating a language teacher attribution scale: a structural equation modeling approach. Teach. Dev. 19, 553–572. doi: 10.1080/13664530.2015.1091785

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbs, S., and Gardiner, M. (2008). The structure of primary and secondary teachers’ attributions for pupils’ misbehaviour: a preliminary cross-phase and cross-cultural investigation. J. Res. Spec. Educ. Needs 8, 68–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-3802.2008.00104.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goetz, T., Becker, E. S., Bieg, M., Keller, M. M., Frenzel, A. C., and Hall, N. C. (2015). The glass half empty: how emotional exhaustion affects the state-trait discrepancy in self-reports of teaching emotions. PLoS One 10:e0137441. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gosling, P. (1994). The attribution of success and failure: the subject/object contrast. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 9, 69–83. doi: 10.1007/BF03172886

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goyette, R., Doré, R., and Dion, E. (2000). Pupil’s misbehaviors and the reactions and causal attributions of physical education student teachers: a sequential analysis. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 20, 3–14. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.20.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S. (1984a). Communicating sympathy and anger to black and white children: the cognitive (attributional) antecedents of affective cues. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 47, 40–54. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.1.40

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S. (1984b). Teacher feelings and student thoughts: an attributional approach to affect in the classroom. Elem. School J. 85, 91–104. doi: 10.1086/461394

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S. (1990). “On communicating low ability in the classroom,” in Attribution Theory: Applications to Achievement, Mental Health, and Interpersonal Conflict, eds S. Graham and V. Folkes (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 17–36.

Google Scholar

Graham, S. (1991). A review of attribution theory in achievement contexts. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 3, 5–39. doi: 10.1007/BF01323661

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, S., and Williams, C. (2009). “An attributional approach to motivation in school,” in Handbook of Motivation at School, eds C. Wentzel and A. Wigfield (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 11–33.

Google Scholar

Guskey, T. R. (1982). Differences in teachers’ perceptions of personal control of positive versus negative student learning outcomes. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 7, 70–80. doi: 10.1016/0361-476X(82)90009-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guttmann, J. (1982). Pupils’, teachers’, and parents’ causal attributions for problem behavior at school. J. Educ. Res. 76, 14–21. doi: 10.1080/00220671.1982.10885417

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, B. W., Villeme, M. G., and Burley, W. (1989). Teachers’ attributions for students’ academic success and failure and the relationship to teaching level and teacher feedback practices. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 14, 133–144. doi: 10.1016/0361-476X(89)90031-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, N. C., Perry, R. P., Goetz, T., Ruthig, J. C., Stupnisky, R. H., and Newall, N. E. (2007). Attributional retraining and elaborative learning: improving academic development through writing-based interventions. Learn. Individ. Differ. 17, 280–290. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2007.04.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hastings, P. D., and Coplan, R. J. (1999). “Conceptual and empirical links between children’s social spheres: relating maternal beliefs and preschoolers’ behav- iors with peers,” in Conflict as a Context for Understanding Maternal Beliefs About Child Rearing and Children’s Misbehavior, eds P. D. Hastings and C. C. Piotrowski (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass), 43–59.

Hau, K. T., and Salili, F. (1996). “Achievement goals and causal attributions of Chinese students,” in Growing Up the Chinese Way: Chinese Child and Adolescent Development, ed. S. Lau (Hong Kong: The Chinese University Press), 121–145.

Google Scholar

Haynes, T. L., Perry, R. P., Stupnisky, R. H., and Daniels, L. M. (2009). “A review of attributional retraining treatments: fostering engagement and persistence in vulnerable college students,” in Higher Education: Handbook of Theory and Research, ed. M. B. Paulsen (Dordrecht: Springer Publishing), 227–272.

Google Scholar

Heider, F. (1958). The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. New York, NY: Wiley. doi: 10.1037/10628-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, I. T. (2004). A comparison of Australian and Chinese teachers’ attributions for student problem behaviors. Educ. Psychol. 24, 375–391. doi: 10.1080/0144341042000211706

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, J. N., Barker, D., Kemenoff, S., and Hart, M. (1993). Problem ownership, causal attributions, and self-efficacy as predictors of teachers’ referral decisions. J. Educ. Psychol. Consult. 4, 369–384. doi: 10.1207/s1532768xjepc0404_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hui, E. K. P. (2001). Hong Kong students’ and teachers’ beliefs on students’ concerns and their causal explanation. Educ. Res. 43, 279–294. doi: 10.1080/00131880110081044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A. (2002). A study of teachers’ perceptions of youth problems. J. Youth Stud. 5, 313–322. doi: 10.1080/1367626022000005992

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jager, L., and Denessen, E. (2015). Within-teacher variation of causal attributions of low achieving students. Soc. Psychol. Educ. Int. J. 18, 517–530. doi: 10.1007/s11218-015-9295-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, C., Fine, S., Weiss, M., Weiss, J., Weiss, G., and Freeman, W. S. (2000). Effects of stimulant medication treatment on mother’s and children’s attributions for the behavior of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 28, 371–382. doi: 10.1023/A:1005121110278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonasson, C. (2014). Defining boundaries between school and work: teachers and students’ attribution of quality to school-based vocational training. J. Educ. Work 27, 544–563. doi: 10.1080/13639080.2013.787483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, E. E., and Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions the attribution process in person perception. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2, 219–266. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60107-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, E. E., and Nisbett, R. E. (1971). “The actor and the observer divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior,” in Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behavior, eds E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, and B. Weiner (Morristown, NJ: General Learning Press), 79–94.

Google Scholar

Jussim, L., and Harber, K. D. (2005). Teacher expectations and self-fulfilling prophecies: knowns and unknowns, resolved and unresolved controversies. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 9, 131–155. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0902_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jussim, L., Robustelli, S. L., and Cain, T. R. (2009). “Teacher expectations and self-fulfilling prophecies,” in Handbook of Motivation at School, eds K. R. Wentzel and A. Wigfield (New York, NY: Routledge), 349–380.

Google Scholar

Juvonen, J. (1988). Outcome and attributional disagreements between students and their teachers. J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 330–336. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.330

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kauppi, T., and Porhola, M. (2012). School teachers bullied by their students: teachers’ attributions and how they share their experiences. Teach. Teach. Educ. 28, 1059–1068. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2012.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klassen, R. M., and Lynch, S. L. (2007). Self-efficacy from the perspective of adolescents with LD and their specialist teachers. J. Learn. Disabil. 40, 494–507. doi: 10.1177/00222194070400060201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klassen, R. M., Tze, V. M., Betts, S. M., and Gordon, K. A. (2011). Teacher efficacy research 1998-2009: signs of progress or unfulfilled promise? Educ. Psychol. Rev. 23, 21–43. doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9141-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kleftaras, G., and Didaskalou, E. (2006). Incidence and teachers’ perceived causation of depression in primary school children in Greece. School Psychol. Int. 27, 296–314. doi: 10.1177/0143034306067284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulinna, P. H. (2007). Teachers’ attributions and strategies for student misbehavior. J. Classroom Interact. 42, 21–30.

Google Scholar

Kurtz, B. E., Schneider, W., Carr, M., Borkowski, J. G., and Rellinger, E. (1990). Strategy instruction and attributional beliefs in West Germany and the United States: do teachers foster metacognitive development? Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 15, 268–283. doi: 10.1016/0361-476X(90)90024-U

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leithwood, K., and Jantzi, D. (1997). Explaining variation in teachers’ perceptions of principals’ leadership: a replication. J. Educ. Admin. 35, 312–331. doi: 10.1108/09578239710171910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, D. E., Catanese, S. P., Coakley, R. M., and Scharff, L. (2007). Chronic pain in the classroom: teachers’ attributions about the causes of chronic pain. J. School Health 77, 248–256. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2007.00200.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C. G., Umezaki, K., and Darley, J. M. (1990). Developmental differences in decoding the meanings of the appraisal actions of teachers. Child Dev. 61, 191–200. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1990.tb02771.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lowe, C. A., Medway, F. J., and Beers, S. E. (1978). Individual Differences in Causal Attributions: The Personal-Environmental Causal Attribution Scale. Paper Presented at the Annual Convention. Toronto: American Psychological Association.

Manassero, M., Garcia Buades, E., Torrens, G., Ramis, C., Vazquez, A., and Ferrer, V. A. (2006). Teacher burnout: attributional aspects. Psychol. Spain 10, 66–74.

Google Scholar

Matteucci, M. C. (2007). Teachers facing school failure: the social valorization of effort in the school context. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 10, 29–53. doi: 10.1007/s11218-006-9011-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matteucci, M. C., Carugati, F., Selleri, P., Mazzoni, E., and Tomasetto, C. (2008a). “Teachers’ judgment from a European psychosocial perspective,” in Teachers and Teaching Strategies: Innovations and Problem Solving, ed. G. F. Ollington (New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers), 31–54.

Google Scholar

Matteucci, M. C., Tomasetto, C., Selleri, P., and Carugati, F. (2008b). Teacher judgments and pupils’ causal explanations: social valorization of effort-based explanations in school context. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 23, 421–432. doi: 10.1007/BF03172750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matteucci, M. C., and Gosling, P. (2004). Italian and French teachers faced with pupil’s academic failure: the ”norm of effort”. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 19, 147–166. doi: 10.1007/BF03173229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mavropoulou, S., and Padeliadu, S. (2002). Teachers’ causal attributions for behaviour problems in relation to perceptions of control. Educ. Psychol. 22, 191–202. doi: 10.1080/01443410120115256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAuley, E., Duncan, T. E., and Russell, D. W. (1992). Measuring causal attributions: the revised causal dimension scale (CDSII). Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 18, 566–573. doi: 10.1177/0146167292185006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAuliffe, M. D., Hubbard, J. A., and Romano, L. J. (2009). The role of teacher cognition and behavior in children’s peer relations. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 665–677. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9305-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, J. (1997). An attribution model for teachers’ occupational stress and job satisfaction in a large educational system. Work Stress 11, 17–32. doi: 10.1080/02678379708256819

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, J., Ayres, P. L., and Beechey, B. (2006). Teaching self-efficacy, stress and coping in a major curriculum reform: applying theory to context. J. Educ. Admin. 44, 53–70. doi: 10.1108/09578230610642656

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, J., and Barnett, K. (2011). Teachers’ attributions for stress and their relationships with burnout. Int. J. Educ. Manag. 25, 278–293. doi: 10.1108/09513541111120114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, J., and Shi, G. (1999). Teachers’ attributions of responsibility for their occupational stress in the People’s Republic of China and Australia. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 69, 393–407. doi: 10.1348/000709999157798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, J., and Solman, R. (1992a). Teachers’ attributions of responsibility for occupational stress and satisfaction: an organisational perspective. Educ. Stud. 18, 201–222. doi: 10.1080/0305569920180206

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, J., and Solman, R. (1992b). The externalized nature of teachers’ occupational stress and its association with job satisfaction. Work Stress 6, 33–44. doi: 10.1080/02678379208257037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medway, F. J. (1979). Casual attributions for school-related problems: teacher perceptions and teacher feedback. J. Educ. Psychol. 71, 809–818. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.71.6.809

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meel, D. E. (2011). On the edge: a case study and resources for mathematics teachers. Primus 21, 485–511. doi: 10.1080/10511970903270309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, A. (1995). Teachers’ attributions of causality, control and responsibility in respect of difficult pupil behaviour and its successful management. Educ. Psychol. 15, 457–471. doi: 10.1080/0144341950150408

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, A., Ferguson, E., and Byrne, I. (2000). Pupils’ causal attributions for difficult classroom behaviour. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 70, 85–96. doi: 10.1348/000709900157985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, A., Ferguson, E., and Moore, E. (2002). Parents’ and pupils’ causal attributions for difficult classroom behaviour. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 72, 27–40. doi: 10.1348/000709902158757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, D. T., and Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: fact or fiction. Psychol. Bull. 82, 213–225. doi: 10.1037/h0076486

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, G. M., and Hastings, R. P. (1998). Special educators’ understanding of challenging behaviours in children with learning disabilities: sensitivity to information about behavioural function. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 26, 43–52. doi: 10.1017/S1352465898000058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Natale, K., Viljaranta, J., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Poikkeus, A.-M., and Nurmi, J.-E. (2009). Cross-lagged associations between kindergarten teachers’ causal attributions and children’s task motivation and performance in reading. Educ. Psychol. 29, 603–619. doi: 10.1080/01443410903165912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Sullivan, J., and Howe, M. (1996). Causal attributions and reading achievement: individual differences in low income families. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 21, 363–387. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1996.0027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative Evaluation and Research Methods, 3rd Edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishing.

Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Hall, N. C., Goetz, T., and Perry, R. P. (2014). Boredom and academic achievement: testing a model of reciprocal causation. J. Educ. Psychol. 106, 696–710. doi: 10.1037/a0036006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirrone, C. (2012). The influence of teachers’ preexisting notions about students on scholastic achievement. AASA J. Scholarsh. Pract. 9, 18–28.

Google Scholar

Poulou, M., and Norwich, B. (2000). Teachers’ causal attributions, cognitive, emotional and behavioural responses to students with emotional and behavioural difficulties. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 70, 559–581. doi: 10.1348/000709900158308

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pounder, D. G. (1987). Testing the generalizability of bias in teacher selection ratings across different modes of screening. J. Educ. Equity Leadersh. 7, 49–60.

Google Scholar

Power, T. G., Bindler, R. C., Goetz, S., and Daratha, K. B. (2010). Obesity prevention in early adolescence: student, parent, and teacher views. J. School Health 80, 13–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2009.00461.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prawat, R. S., Byers, J. L., and Anderson, A. H. (1983). An attributional analysis of teachers’ affective reactions to student success and failure. Am. Educ. Res. J. 20, 137–152. doi: 10.3102/00028312020001137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prihadi, K., Hairul, N. I., and Hazri, J. (2012). Mediation effect of locus of control on the causal relationship between students’ perceived teachers’ expectancy and self-esteem. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 10, 713–736.

Rejeski, W. J., and McCook, W. (1980). Individual differences in professional teachers’ attributions for children’s performance outcomes. Psychol. Rep. 46, 1159–1163. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1980.46.3c.1159

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reyna, C. (2000). Lazy, dumb, or industrious: when stereotypes convey attribution information in the classroom. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 12, 85–110. doi: 10.1023/A:1009037101170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reyna, C. (2008). Ian is intelligent but Leshaun is lazy: antecedents and consequences of attributional stereotypes in the classroom. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 23, 439–458. doi: 10.1007/BF03172752

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reyna, C., and Weiner, B. (2001). Justice and utility in the classroom: an attributional analysis of the goals of teachers’ punishment and intervention strategies. J. Educ. Psychol. 93, 309–319. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.2.309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riley, T., and Ungerleider, C. (2012). Self-fulfilling prophecy: how teachers’ attributions, expectations, and stereotypes influence the learning opportunities afforded aboriginal students. Can. J. Educ. 35, 303–333.

Google Scholar

Robinson, G. (2011). The contradictions of caring: social workers, teachers, and attributions for poverty and welfare reform. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 41, 2374–2404. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2011.00825.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodriquez, R., and Tollefson, N. (1987). Consequences of costa rican teachers’ attributions for students’ failure. Instr. Sci. 16, 381–387. doi: 10.1007/BF00117753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolison, M. A., and Medway, F. J. (1985). Teachers’ expectations and attributions for student achievement: effects of label performance pattern, and special education intervention. Am. Educ. Res. J. 22, 561–573. doi: 10.3102/00028312022004561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roness, D. (2011). Still motivated? The motivation for teaching during the second year in the profession. Teach. Teach. Educ. 27, 628–638. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2010.10.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: distortions in the attribution process. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 10, 173–220. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60357-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, L., Bierbrauer, G., and Polly, S. (1974). Attribution of educational outcomes by professional and nonprofessional instructors. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 29, 609–618. doi: 10.1037/h0036740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, G., Assor, A., Kanat-Maymon, Y., and Kaplan, H. (2007). Autonomous motivation for teaching: how self-determined teaching may lead to self-determined learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 99, 761–774. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.4.761

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, K., and Eisenberg, N. (1983). The effects of children’s height on teachers’ attributions of competence. J. Genet. Psychol. 143, 45–50. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1983.10533532

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychol. Monogr. 80, 1–28. doi: 10.1037/h0092976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousvoal, J. (1987). Les représentations de l’enfant au cours préparatoire. Leurs rapports avec la réussite ou l’échec scolaire. Rev. Française de Pédagogie 79, 15–34. doi: 10.3406/rfp.1987.1477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savina, E., Moskovtseva, L., Naumenko, O., and Zilberberg, A. (2014). How russian teachers, mothers and school psychologists perceive internalising and externalising behaviours in children. Emot. Behav. Diffic. 19, 371–385. doi: 10.1080/13632752.2014.891358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwerdtfeger, A., Konermann, L., and Schonhofen, K. (2008). Self-efficacy as a health-protective resource in teachers? A biopsychological approach. Health Psychol. 27, 358–368. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.3.358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shields, C. C., and Edens, K. M. (2009). Thin-slicing teacher dispositions through assessment practices. Teach. Educ. Pract. 22, 131–144.

Google Scholar

Soodak, L. C., and Podell, D. M. (1994). Teachers’ thinking about difficult-to-teach students. J. Educ. Res. 88, 44–51. doi: 10.1080/00220671.1994.9944833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, R. E. (2007). “Teachers’ anger, frustration, and self-regulation,” in Emotion in education, eds P. A. Schutz and R. Pekrun (Amsterdam: Academic Press), 259–274.

Google Scholar

Tõeväli, P. K., and Kikas, E. (2016). Teachers’ ability and help attributions and children’s math performance and task persistence. Early Child Dev. Care 186, 1259–1270. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2015.1089434

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tollefson, N., and Chen, J. S. (1988). Consequences of teachers’ attributions for student failure. Teach. Teach. Educ. 4, 259–265. doi: 10.1016/0742-051X(88)90005-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tollefson, N., Hsia, S., and Townsend, J. (1991). Teachers’ perceptions of students’ excuses for academic difficulties. Psychol. Schools 28, 146–155. doi: 10.1002/1520-6807(199104)28:2<146::AID-PITS2310280209>3.0.CO;2-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tollefson, N., Melvin, J., and Thippavajjala, C. (1990). Teachers’ attributions for students’ low achievement: a validation of cooper and good’s attributional categories. Psychol. Schools 27, 75–83.

Google Scholar

Tom, D., and Cooper, H. (1986). The effect of student background on teacher performance attributions: evidence for counterdefensive patterns and low expectancy cycles. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 7, 53–62. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp0701_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Upadyaya, K., Viljaranta, J., Lerkkanen, M.-K., Poikkeus, A.-M., and Nurmi, J.-E. (2012). Cross-lagged relations between kindergarten teachers’ causal attributions, and children’s interest value and performance in mathematics. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 15, 181–206. doi: 10.1007/s11218-011-9171-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vernberg, E. M., and Medway, F. J. (1981). Teacher and parent causal perceptions of school problems. Am. Educ. Res. J. 18, 29–37. doi: 10.3102/00028312018001029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlachou, A., Eleftheriadou, D., and Metallidou, P. (2014). Do learning difficulties differentiate elementary teachers’ attributional patterns for students’ academic failure? A comparison between Greek regular and special education teachers. Eur. J. Spec. Needs Educ. 29, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/08856257.2013.830440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H., Hall, N. C., Goetz, T., and Frenzel, A. C. (2017). Teachers’ goal orientations: effects on classroom goal structures and emotions. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 87, 90–107. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, H., Hall, N. C., and Rahimi, S. (2015). Self-efficacy and causal attributions in teachers: effects on burnout, job satisfaction, illness, and quitting intentions. Teach. Teach. Educ. 47, 120–130. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2014.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B. (1972). Attribution theory, achievement motivation, and the educational process. Rev. Educ. Res. 42, 203–215. doi: 10.3102/00346543042002203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B. (1976). An attributional approach for educational psychology. Rev. Res. Educ. 4, 179–209. doi: 10.3102/0091732X004001179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. J. Educ. Psychol. 71, 3–25. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.71.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B. (1985). An attribution theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychol. Rev. 92, 548–573. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B. (2000). Intrapersonal and interpersonal theories of motivation from an attributional perspective. Educ. Psychol. Review 12, 1–14. doi: 10.1023/A:1009017532121

CrossRef Full Text

Weiner, B. (2001). “An attributional approach to perceived responsibility,” in Responsibility: The Many Faces of a Social Phenomenon, eds A. E. Auhangen and H. W. Bierhoff (New York, NY: Routledge), 49–60.

Google Scholar

Weiner, B. (2003). The classroom as a courtroom. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 6, 3–15. doi: 10.1023/A:1021736217048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B. (2010). The development of an attribution-based theory of motivation: a history of ideas. Educ. Psychol. 45, 28–36. doi: 10.1080/00461520903433596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiner, B., and Kukla, A. (1970). An attributional analysis of achievement motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 15, 1–20. doi: 10.1037/h0029211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiley, A. L., Tankersley, M., and Simms, A. (2012). “Teachers’ causal attributions for student problem behavior: implications for school-based behavioral interventions and research,” in Classroom Behavior, Contexts, and Interventions Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities, Vol. 25, eds B. G. Cook, M. Tankersley, and T. J. Landrum (Bingley: Emerald), 279–300.

Google Scholar

Wiley, M. G., and Eskilson, A. (1978). Why did you learn in school today? Teachers’ perceptions of causality. Soc. Educ. 51, 261–269. doi: 10.5731/pdajpst.2013.00940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, D. W., and Linville, P. W. (1982). Improving the academic performance of college freshmen: attribution therapy revisited. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 42, 367–376. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.42.2.367

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wissink, I. B., and de Haan, M. (2013). Teachers’ and parental attribution for school performance of ethnic majority and minority children. Int. J. Higher Educ. 2, 65–76. doi: 10.5430/ijhe.v2n4p65

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodcock, S., and Jiang, H. (2013). Teachers’ causal attributional responses of students with learning disabilities in China. Learn. Individ. Differ. 25, 163–170. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodcock, S., and Vialle, W. (2011). Are we exacerbating students’ learning disabilities? An investigation of preservice teachers’ attributions of the educational outcomes of students with learning disabilities. Ann. Dyslexia 61, 223–241. doi: 10.1007/s11881-011-0058-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolfson, L., Grant, E., and Campbell, L. (2007). A comparison of special, general and support teachers’ controllability and stability attributions for children’s difficulties in learning. Educ. Psychol. 27, 295–306. doi: 10.1080/01443410601066826

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolfson, L. M., and Brady, K. (2009). An investigation of factors impacting on mainstream teachers’ beliefs about teaching students with learning difficulties. Educ. Psychol. 29, 221–238. doi: 10.1080/01443410802708895

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wyatt, L. W., and Haskett, M. E. (2001). Aggressive and nonaggressive young adolescents’ attributions of intent in teacher/student interactions. J. Early Adolesc. 21, 425–446. doi: 10.1177/0272431601021004003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yehudah, Y. B. (2002). Self-serving attributions in teachers’ explanations of students’ performance in a national oral essay competition. Soc. Behav. Personal. 30, 411–416. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2002.30.4.411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, G. (2006). Attribution characteristics of Chinese teachers and students: from some interaction experiences on campus. Asia Pac. Educ. Rev. 7, 144–151. doi: 10.1007/BF03031538

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: teachers, causal attributions, interpersonal, intrapersonal, occupational stress, review

Citation: Wang H and Hall NC (2018) A Systematic Review of Teachers’ Causal Attributions: Prevalence, Correlates, and Consequences. Front. Psychol. 9:2305. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02305

Received: 15 May 2018; Accepted: 05 November 2018;
Published: 17 December 2018.

Edited by:

Ann X. Huang, Duquesne University, United States

Reviewed by:

Jie Zhang, The College at Brockport, United States
Malte Jansen, Institute for Educational Quality Improvement (IQB), Germany

Copyright © 2018 Wang and Hall. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hui Wang, hwang@eduhk.hk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.