Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 02 October 2020
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic New Paradigm of Attention and Attention Training: Mechanisms and Applications View all 18 articles

Attentional Bias for Imperfect Pictures in Perfectionism: An Eye-Movement Study

  • 1Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University, Tianjin, China
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Tianjin Medical University, Tianjin, China
  • 3Institute of Applied Psychology, Tianjin University, Tianjin, China

Background: Although dot-probe paradigms have been widely used in previous studies to investigate the attentional bias of perfectionists, the exact characteristics of this bias are still unclear.

Methods: In this study, eye-tracking technology was used to compare the attentional patterns of high perfectionists (HP) and low perfectionists (LP). The HP and LP groups (n = 39 vs 34) completed a visual attention task in which they observed perfect vs imperfect picture pairs, during which their eye movements (EMs) were recorded automatically using an EM tracking system.

Results: Both the HP and LP groups showed an overall attentional bias toward imperfect pictures, as indicated by the criteria of initial visual attention orientation and attentional maintenance. There were no significant differences between the HP and LP groups during the early and middle phases of attention: both groups exhibited a longer total fixation duration on imperfect pictures than on perfect pictures. However, during the late phase of attention, the participants in the HP group diverted their attention away from the imperfect pictures and began to pay more attention to the perfect pictures. By contrast, the participants in the LP group consistently exhibited longer fixation times for imperfect pictures than the HP group during the entire duration of the stimulus.

Conclusion: These findings indicate that the participants in the HP group tended to avoid imperfect stimuli during the late phase of attention; this may indicate that avoidance plays an important role in maintaining perfectionism. This study also shows that eye-tracking is a useful methodology for measuring the attentional biases of perfectionists.

Introduction

Perfectionism is marked by a tendency to hold oneself to impossibly high standards and to engage in a high level of self-criticism (Frost et al., 1990). Numerous studies have found that perfectionism is a transdiagnostic process of multiple psychological disorders, and it has been affirmed as an important predisposing and perpetuating factor for such conditions as eating disorders (Boone et al., 2012; Johnston et al., 2018), anxiety (Gnilka et al., 2012; Mitchell et al., 2013), depression (Dry et al., 2015; Mehr and Adams, 2016), and self-harm (O’Connor et al., 2010).

Theoretical Models of Perfectionism

The cognitive–behavioral model of perfectionism has been developed to account for the maintenance of perfectionism in psychological disorders (Shafran et al., 2002, 2010). The model indicates that a number of cognitive biases underlie perfectionism. One such bias is an attentional bias that causes perfectionists to pay more attention to imperfect stimuli. Such attentional bias is characteristic of clinical perfectionism and involves an individual focusing on a mistake or error they have made even if it is comparatively unimportant (e.g., “One punctuation mistake in a document is evidence that I am not good enough at work”) (Egan et al., 2016).

Attentional Biases and Perfectionism

Although attentional bias is regarded as an important maintenance factor in the cognitive–behavioral model of perfectionism, the specific details of the attentional process and the precise nature of the attentional biases remain unclear. To date, studies of attentional bias in perfectionism are very sparse, and what work has been done has typically used words and pictures as stimuli to examine attentional processes. For example, Kobori and Tanno (2012) investigated the differences between high perfectionists (HP) and low perfectionists (LP) on an emotional Stroop task that required the participants to color name failure words (e.g., mistake, unsuccess, and error) and neutral words (e.g., newspaper, map, and furniture). Their results suggested that HP take longer to respond to failure words than do LP. Although this finding supports the notion that HP have an attentional bias to negative perfectionism-relevant information, the emotional Stroop task has been questioned as an effective measure of attentional bias (Bar-Haim et al., 2007). Howell et al. (2016) used an attentional probe task to compare the attentional bias of HP and LP as they responded to stimulus words that differed in emotional valence (positive vs negative) and perfectionism relevance (perfectionism-relevant vs irrelevant). The results demonstrated that HP displayed greater attentional preference to negative than to positive information, but only for perfectionism-relevant stimuli. Tonta et al. (2019) further discriminated attentional bias into two subprocesses of attentional engagement and disengagement. They used a modified dot-probe task to evaluate the ways in which perfectionistic concerns and strivings are relevant to attentional engagement and disengagement. Their findings demonstrated that perfectionistic concerns were associated with a disengagement bias for negative stimuli. Although engagement and disengagement bias were calculated by comparing relative latencies to respond to target probes appearing in either the same or opposite locus of the emotional information, they used response latency tasks to indirectly assess attentional bias, and these could not effectively differentiate the different components of attention (Yiend and Mathews, 2005). The results should, therefore, be interpreted with caution.

In addition to using words as stimulus material, pictorial stimuli (e.g., perfect pictures vs imperfect pictures) have also been used in studies. Fei et al. (2011) proposed that perfectionists have imperfection anxiety when they process damaged, unfinished, or untidy stimuli. They compared the difference in attentional preference between HP and LP individuals to perfect and imperfect figures. They found that HP individuals showed attentional bias to an imperfect graph, while LP individuals showed attentional avoidance to an imperfect graph. Based on Fei and his colleagues’ study, Ren and Liu (2013) used pictures of daily life as experimental materials on the ground that they should have higher ecological validity. They further divided LP into positive perfectionists and non-perfectionists. They found that positive perfectionists showed avoidance of imperfect pictures, negative perfectionists showed attentional preference for imperfect pictures, and non-perfectionists did not show any significant attentional bias. The different results between these two studies may be the product of the various grouping criteria used for screening the subjects. Although previous studies have suggested that the perfectionism-related attentional biases posited by the cognitive–behavioral model of perfectionism do indeed exist, the dot-probe tasks used to investigate such biases mainly focused on a “snapshot” picture of attention at one point in time. They were, therefore, unable to capture the time course of attentional processes during exposure to stimuli (Armstrong and Olatunji, 2012).

Attention is a complex cognitive process consisting of initial orientation, engagement, maintenance, disengagement, re-engagement, and avoidance (Fashler and Katz, 2014). Moreover, different components of attention may play different roles at specific stages of attention course. Eye-tracking technology records continuous visual attention (Yang et al., 2012, 2013; Vervoort et al., 2013; Todd et al., 2016) and is thus an excellent tool for studying visual attention at different phases during the presentation of a stimulus. Surprisingly, there has yet been no study using eye-tracking technology to investigate the attentional bias of perfectionists.

The Current Study

To the best of our knowledge, the present study is the first to empirically test the link between attentional bias and perfectionism using eye-tracking methodology via the investigation of attentional differences between HP and LP groups, thus allowing assessment of visual fixation patterns across relatively long stimulus exposure periods. Based on the cognitive–behavioral model of clinical perfectionism (Shafran et al., 2002), we hypothesized that the HP group would (1) show vigilance toward imperfect pictures in the initial orienting phase, (2) display greater attentional maintenance to imperfect pictures, and (3) show an attentional bias toward imperfect pictures at different stages of visual attentional processing.

Methods

Participants

A total of 1205 participants were recruited and asked to complete an online version of the Chinese version of the FMPS (CFMPS) as a prescreening survey, along with several additional screening measures (e.g., SAS and SDS). Individuals with high or low perfectionism screening scores (i.e., ≤45 or ≥78) were selected for recruitment. These cutoffs were determined by calculating the mean and standard deviation of the participants’ total scores on the four negative dimensions of the CFMPS. Participants whose scores were less than or more than one standard deviation from the mean were contacted and invited to participate in the study. Seventy-eight participants were tested approximately 2 months after screening. When they completed the CFMPS a second time, the participants were classified as HP or LP according to the criterion of whether their total scores on the four negative dimensions were below or above the sample median. (The median score for the initial pool of 1205 college students was 61.5). However, four participants were unable to complete the study due to difficulties with calibration of the eye-tracker, and one additional participant was dismissed due to use of psychiatric medication. Thus, seventy-three participants were included in the final analysis. All of these participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and reported no past and present neurological disease or psychiatric illness. There were 39 participants (9 men, 30 women; Mage = 18.97 years, SD = 0.84) in the HP group and 34 participants (12 men, 22 women; Mage = 19.32 years, SD = 1.17) in the LP group.

Materials

Questionnaires

Chinese version of the frost multidimensional perfectionism scale

The Chinese version of the frost multidimensional perfectionism scale (CFMPS) was developed by Frost et al. (1990) and translated by Cheng et al. (1999) into its Chinese version. The scale was later revised by Fei and Zhou (2006). The scale contains 27 items and is composed of five subscales, including Concern for Mistakes (CM), Parental Expectations (PE), Doubts About Action (DA), Personal Standards (PS), and Organization (OR). CM, DA, PS, and PE constitute the negative dimensions of the CFMPS (Parker, 1997; Fei and Zhou, 2006), with a higher total score indicating a high level of perfectionism. The five subscales of the CFMPS were shown to have satisfactory internal consistencies (Cronbach α = 0.64–0.76), and acceptable test–retest reliability (0.75–0.82) (Fei and Zhou, 2006). In the current study, the four negative dimensions were found to have adequate internal consistency, with respective alpha coefficients of 0.67, 0.75, 0.76, and 0.88 for CM, DA, PS, and PE. Our criteria for classifying individuals as HP or LP were guided by a previous study reported by Fei et al. (2011), in which the total score of the four negative dimensions was calculated.

Chinese version of the depression, anxiety, and stress scale-21 (CDASS-21)

The DASS was developed by Lovibond and Lovibond (1996) and translated into Chinese by Gong et al. (2010). This 21-item scale was administered to determine whether differences in anxiety, depression, or stress exist between HP and LP. All these factors are potential confounding factors, given that attentional response to information is affected by anxiety and depression (Howell et al., 2016).

The Stimuli

The experimental picture pairs are taken from Ren and Liu’s (2013) research, including articles of daily use (i.e., mirror, towel, etc.) food, and animals. The perfect pictures were clean, complete pictures downloaded from the Internet. Imperfect pictures were created with Photoshop software to introduce defects with regard to the integrity of the object, the balance of the shape, and the cleanliness of the picture. All image pairs were matched as closely as possible with regard to color, complexity, and brightness. The picture size was standardized to a uniform size (198 × 198 pixels; visual angles were 7.3° × 5.5°), and the centers of the two pictures were separated by 558 pixels, subtending a visual angle of 19.9°.

For each trial in the task, two pictures were presented simultaneously. The picture pair consisted of either (1) an imperfect picture (IP) and a perfect picture (PP) (IP–PP pair) or (2) two perfect pictures (PP–PP pair). Each picture pair was presented only once during the task. PP–PP picture pairs acted as filler to mask the experimental intent, and the relevant data were excluded in statistical analyses. These pictures were assessed on their degree of perfectionism by an independent sample (11 women, 9 men) who did not participate in the eye movement (EM) experiment and were required to respond on a seven-point Likert scale from 1 (very imperfect) to 7 (very perfect). The t-test revealed that there was a significant difference between the two types of pictures in their degree of perfection (t = 22.516, p < 0.001, Mpp = 5.34, SD = 0.54; MIP = 3.11, SD = 0.54). The final set of pictures consisted of 60 IP–PP pairs and 40 PP–PP pairs. All the images in the experiment were shown only once to avoid the familiarity effect. Presentation of picture pairs was counterbalanced, controlling for the location of PP and IP (left, right) and the presentation order of trials was randomly generated for each participant.

Apparatus

Participants’ EMs were recorded using an Eye link 1000 EM tracking system (SR Research, Mississauga, ON, Canada). The data were recorded with a sample rate of 1000 Hz. Stimuli were presented on a 17-inch, 65-Hz, 1024 × 768 pixel CRT monitor. Gaze data were acquired from the right eye. A nine-point calibration and validation procedure was conducted prior to starting the task in order to configure the system such that the spatial accuracy error was below 0.5 on average. In addition, calibration accuracy was re-checked after every trial.

Experimental Procedures

After arriving at the lab and providing informed consent, participants filled out a paper survey consisting of the two questionnaires described earlier. Completion of the questionnaires took from 5 to 10 min. Following completion of the questionnaire, the visual task was introduced and described. To conceal the purpose of the experiment, participants were told that their pupil dilations would be measured (Kellough et al., 2008). The instructions emphasized the claim that participants were taking part in a spatial cognition experiment and that they should look at the “+” that marked the beginning of each trial and to view the images naturally on the screen as if they were watching TV or reading a magazine. Next, prior to the visual task, the participant’s head was kept stable using an adjustable head mount, with a distance between the participant’s eyes and the camera of approximately 65 cm. The eye tracker was then calibrated. Participants first completed six practice trials to adjust to the task followed by the 100 experimental trials. The whole task took approximately 25 min. Upon completion of the task, participants were thanked and were paid 20 Yuan (approximately $3 USD) as compensation for their time.

The protocol for each trial consisted of two stages. At the start of each trial, a black central fixation cross (“+”) appeared at the center of the screen for random time intervals ranging from 600 to 800 ms. Then, a picture pair (IP–PP or PP–PP) was presented. Both pictures in the pair were presented simultaneously, one on the left and the other on the right side of the screen for 2000 ms. The time interval between two adjacent trials was 1000 ms.

Data Preparation

Prior work has used a 40% criterion for participant exclusion due to excessive signal loss (Graham et al., 2011). In the current study, no participants met this criterion for exclusion. Previous studies (Caseras et al., 2007) also indicated that EM data must meet the condition that saccades be classified as fixations to a position only if they remained stable within a 1° visual angle for at least 100 ms. Fixations on stimuli were identified as effective when the following steps were satisfied: during the initial “+” presentation, participants fixated at the central region; after the presentation of the stimuli pairs, saccades occurred for at least 100 ms; during presentation of the picture pairs, participants fixated on at least one of the pictures instead of other locations around the screen. According to the above criteria, 8.12% trials were excluded from the final analysis. Finally, fixations on either picture during the 2000-ms free-viewing period with durations ≥100 ms were extracted and used for computation of the visual attention indices (Macatee et al., 2017).

Statistical Analysis

Six visual attention indices were used to assess attentional bias for imperfect pictures and its relation to perfectionism. Two indices measured the initial threat vigilance: orientation of first fixation (the percentage of the first fixation on IP or PP) and latency of first fixation (the time between a picture’s presentation and the making of a fixation). One index represents attentional maintenance: dwell time for each image as the cumulative duration of visual fixations (Shechner et al., 2017). In order to decompose the temporal dynamics of attention allocation across the duration of the prolonged stimulus, each trial of 2000 ms was subdivided into three attentional phases: an early phase (0–500 ms), a middle phase (500–1000 ms), and a late phase (1000–2000 ms), and total fixation duration in each of these three time intervals was calculated.

Independent sample t-tests were used to examine whether there were significant differences in age, total score for the negative dimensions of the CFMPS, and scores for anxiety, depression, and stress. To test our hypotheses regarding attentional biases, direction of first fixation on the target image, first fixation latency, and the dwell time and total fixation duration in each of the three attentional phases were analyzed with a 2 × 2 mixed-design repeated-measures analyses of variance (ANOVAs) using group (HP and LP) as the independent samples factor and picture type (perfect and imperfect) as the within-subjects factor.

Results

Group Characteristics

The t-test showed that there were no significant differences in scores for anxiety and stress between the HP and LP groups [t(71) = 1.11, 1.36, both p > 0.05, respectively; Table 1]. The difference in depression scores between the two groups was marginally significant [t(71) = 1.72, p = 0.09], with greater depression scores exhibited in the HP group than in the LP group (Table 1). The HP group had significantly higher values than the LP group for the total score of the negative dimensions of the CFMPS [t(71) = 24.02, p < 0.001; Table 1]. There was no significant difference in age between the two groups of participants [t(71) = −1.44, p > 0.05].

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic comparisons for participants in HP and LP groups (n = 39 vs 34) (means, with standard deviations in parentheses).

EM Results

Initial Visual Attention Orientation

The 2 × 2 mixed-design repeated-measures ANOVA showed that the main effect of picture type significantly impacted first fixation direction [F(1, 71) = 23.56, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.25, Table 2]. There was a greater tendency for all participants to orient initially toward imperfect pictures rather than perfect pictures (M ± SD = 31.49 ± 3.36 and 27.67 ± 3.67, respectively). Similarly, the main effect of picture type significantly affected first fixation latency [F(1,71) = 122.50, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.64, Table 2]: both the HP and LP groups had shorter (i.e., faster) initial fixation latencies on imperfect pictures than on perfect pictures (M ± SD = 0.49 ± 0.08 and 0.57 ± 0.10, respectively). However, neither the interaction between picture type and group nor group itself significantly affected the first fixation direction and first fixation latency (see Figure 1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of eye-movement data.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. First fixation direction (upper panel) and first fixation latency (bottom panel) under combinations of group (HP vs LP) and picture types (perfect pictures vs imperfect pictures). The different lowercases denote significant difference at p < 0.05. Data are presented as mean ± SE.

Maintenance of Visual Attention

The main effect of picture type was significant for total dwell time [F(1,71) = 57.62, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.45, Table 2]: both the HP and the LP groups exhibited longer dwell time for imperfect pictures than for perfect pictures (M ± SD = 0.77 ± 0.13 and 0.60 ± 0.12, respectively). The effect of the interaction between group and picture type on dwell time was significant [F(1,71) = 11.18, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.14]. The LP group showed longer dwell time toward imperfect pictures than did the HP group (M ± SD = 0.81 ± 0.16 and 0.74 ± 0.10, respectively), while the HP group showed longer dwell time than the LP group toward perfect pictures (M ± SD = 0.64 ± 0.12 and 0.56 ± 0.09, respectively). There was no significant main effect of group on dwell time (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Dwell time under combinations of group (HP vs LP) and picture types (perfect pictures vs imperfect pictures). The different lowercases denote significant difference at p < 0.05. Data are presented as mean ± SE.

Attentional Phases

For early-phase total fixation duration (0–500 ms), the main effect of picture type was significant [F(1,71) = 39.97, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.36, Table 2]. All participants had longer total fixation duration for imperfect pictures than for perfect pictures (M ± SD = 0.11 ± 0.02 and 0.09 ± 0.02, respectively) in the early phase. Similarly, for middle-phase total fixation duration (500–1000 ms), the main effect of picture type was also significant [F(1,71) = 134.20, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.65]: all participants had longer total fixation duration for imperfect pictures than for perfect pictures (M ± SD = 0.24 ± 0.04 and 0.16 ± 0.03, respectively). However, neither the main effect of group nor the interaction between group and picture type significantly affected total fixation duration in the early and middle phases. For late-phase total fixation duration (1000–2000 ms), the main effect of picture type was significant [F(1,71) = 18.09, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.20, Table 2] in that the total fixation duration for imperfect pictures was significantly longer than that for perfect pictures (M ± SD = 0.43 ± 0.11 and 0.35 ± 0.10, respectively). The interaction between picture and group also had a significant effect on late-phase total fixation duration [F(1,71) = 11.76, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.14, Table 2]. Simple main effects showed that the total fixation duration for imperfect pictures was significantly longer for the LP group than for the HP group in the late phase (M ± SD = 0.45 ± 0.13 and 0.40 ± 0.08, respectively). Conversely, the total fixation duration for perfect pictures was significantly longer for the HP group than for the LP group in the late phase (M ± SD = 0.39 ± 0.10 and 0.31 ± 0.07, respectively). There was no significant main effect of group on late-phase fixation duration (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Total fixation duration between HP and LP groups across three consecutive time intervals toward imperfect pictures (A) and perfect pictures (B). The different lowercases denote significant difference for each phases of three consecutive time intervals at p < 0.05. Data are presented as mean ± SE.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first experimental study to use EM measurements to investigate the attentional patterns associated with perfectionism during exposure to perfect–imperfect picture pairs. The results showed that both the HP and the LP group had an overall attentional bias toward imperfect pictures based on their initial visual attention orientation and their attentional maintenance. A further examination of the results from each of the three phases of attentional processing showed that there was no significant difference between the HP group and the LP group during the first 1000 ms of stimulus presentation, during which time both groups paid more attention to the imperfect pictures. However, during the second 1000 ms, the participants in the HP group diverted their attention away from the imperfect pictures and began to pay more attention to the perfect pictures.

The initial vigilance toward the imperfect pictures by all participants could be attributed to the following reasons. First, perfect things are more in line with people’s expectations. Adler (1956) once claimed that “striving for perfection is innate in the sense that it is a part of life, a striving, an urge, a something without which life would be unthinkable, this feeling of longing for the abrogations of every imperfection, is never absent”. Thus, both groups showed an initial vigilance to the imperfect pictures. Second, imperfect stimuli are visually more complex, and detecting flaws in imperfect pictures may consume more attentional resources. This would result in all participants having longer total fixation durations for imperfect pictures than for perfect pictures in the early and middle attentional phases. Our findings of attentional bias toward imperfect stimuli during the HP group’s early exposure are consistent with previous behavioral studies (Fei et al., 2011; Ren and Liu, 2013; Howell et al., 2016). However, the LP group presented the same pattern as the HP group in the early phase; this result was opposite to earlier studies that showed that LP groups display attentional avoidance to imperfect stimuli (Fei et al., 2011; Ren and Liu, 2013). One possible explanation is that the response latency tasks that were used in past studies to assess attentional bias only indirectly captured a single “snapshot” of attention at a time point of 500 or 750 ms. The measurements may thus have been confounded by motor retardation. Meanwhile, the different components of attention (e.g., orienting attention vs maintenance of attention) could not be differentiated in response latency tasks (Mathews et al., 1996).

Both groups showed attention maintenance to imperfect pictures, but a significant difference between the HP and the LP groups developed as the stimulus presentation progressed over time. During the late attentional phase, the participants in the HP group diverted their attention away from the imperfect pictures and began to pay more attention to the perfect pictures. This differential pattern of attention over time supports the conclusions drawn from Crombez et al. (2013) that “conscious and elaborative processes are critical for attentional biases to emerge.” Stimuli were displayed for a relatively long time (2000 ms) in our study. These passive viewing conditions may have engaged emotional regulation strategies in the late phase (Shechner et al., 2017). Previous studies have found that perfectionists experience imperfection anxiety when they process imperfect stimuli, e.g., damaged, unfinished, or untidy stimuli (Fei et al., 2011; Ren and Liu, 2013). Imperfect pictures might, therefore, evoke imperfection anxiety in HP participants, causing voluntary strategic avoidance. Previous studies have shown that self-critical perfectionists were more likely to choose avoidant strategies to distract attention from threatening stimuli (Dunkley et al., 2003). Visual avoidance may be considered to be a strategic, top-down process by which individuals in the HP group attempt to minimize the aversive impact triggered by imperfect stimuli. However, because anxiety was not measured in response to the images, this interpretation is still speculative and should be examined in future studies.

A detailed understanding of the attentional bias of perfectionism is key to clinical prevention and intervention. The present study has important clinical implications regarding the treatment of high perfectionism. The results showed that HP individuals tend to avoid imperfect stimuli in the later phase of attention, indicating that avoidance might play an important role in maintaining perfectionism. Although attention avoidance in later stages of processing was the result of emotional management, these avoidant behaviors do not result in a complete resolution of anxiety and conversely may lead to enduring symptoms (Koster et al., 2005). Interventions for HP should, therefore, focus on developing alternative opinions about imperfection. Perhaps once HP individuals are better able to accept imperfect stimuli, the general symptoms caused by perfectionism such as distress, anxiety, and depression will be reduced.

We also acknowledge that there are several limitations to the present study. First, individual differences in perfectionism were assessed using the CFMPS. Although the CFMPS consistently has very good reliability and validity, it cannot necessarily screen out implicit perfectionists who pursue perfectionism unconsciously. Future research should incorporate the interview or other methods to measure perfectionism and to examine attentional bias in individuals drawn from clinical samples. Second, the numbers of IP and PP were not exactly matched because some perfect pictures were used as fillers. This may have introduced a frequency effect that would cause participants to look at the imperfect pictures more often. Third, the sample in our study was only a non-clinical sample, and this could limit the generalizability of our findings. Future studies should examine the attentional bias in clinical samples. Fourth, only HP and LP were compared in this study. Based on the 2 × 2 model of perfectionism (Gaudreau and Thompson, 2010), four subtypes of perfectionism could be differentiated. Future research should recruit participants from across the full distribution of perfectionism scores and examine the differences in attentional biases among the four subtypes.

Conclusion

It can be concluded that the participants in the HP group tended to avoid imperfect stimuli in the later phase of attention, indicating that avoidance might play an important role in maintaining perfectionism. We hope that there will be more studies on perfectionist cognitive biases in the future by means of eye-tracking technology across various groups with different degrees of attentional bias. In addition, we conclude that the application of eye-tracking technology can be an aid to monitoring the progress and efficacy of clinical interventions.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets presented in this study are included in the article/Supplementary Material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethics Committee in Faculty of Psychology, Tianjin Normal University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JL and XPL designed the study, performed the measurement, and drafted the manuscript together. XCL conducted literature searches. WT was responsible for the interpretation of the data. BY revised the language. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Tianjin Municipal Education Commission.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study is a part of first author’s Ph.D. thesis. We thank the Faculty of Psychology of Tianjin Normal University where the evaluations have been carried out. We also thank all individuals who participated in this study.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566482/full#supplementary-material

References

Adler, A. (1956). The Individual Psychology of Alfred Adler, eds H. L. Ansbacher and R. R. Ansbacher New York, NY: Basic Books.

Google Scholar

Armstrong, T., and Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: a meta-analytic review and synthesis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 32, 704–723. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., and Van, I. M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 133, 1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boone, L., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., and Braet, C. (2012). Is there a perfectionist in each of us? An experimental study on perfectionism and eating disorder symptoms. Appetite 59, 531–540. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caseras, X., Garner, M., Bradley, B. P., and Mogg, K. (2007). Biases in visual orienting to negative and positive scenes in dysphoria: an eye movement study. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116, 491–497. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, S. K., Chong, G. H., and Wong, C. W. (1999). Chinese frost multidimensional perfectionism scale: a validation and prediction of self-esteem and psychological distress. J. Clin. Psychol. 55, 1051–1061.

Google Scholar

Crombez, G., Van Ryckeghem, D. M. L., Eccleston, C., and Van Damme, S. (2013). Attentional bias to pain-related information: a meta-analysis. PAIN 154, 497–510. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dry, S. M., Kane, R. T., and Rooney, R. M. (2015). An investigation into the role of coping in preventing depression associated with perfectionism in preadolescent children. Front. Public Health 3:190. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2015.00190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunkley, D. M., Zuroff, D. C., and Blankstein, K. R. (2003). Self-critical perfectionism and daily affect: dispositional and situational influences on stress and coping. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 84, 234–252.

Google Scholar

Egan, S. J., Shafran, R., Lee, M., Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., Doll, H. A., et al. (2016). The reliability and validity of the clinical perfectionism questionnaire in eating disorder and community samples. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 44, 79–91. doi: 10.1017/S1352465814000629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fashler, S., and Katz, J. (2014). More than meets the eye: visual attention biases in individuals reporting chronic pain. J. Pain Res. 7, 557–570. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S67431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fei, Z., and Zhou, X. (2006). The Chinese frost multidimensional perfectionism scale: an examination of its reliability and validity. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 14, 560–563. doi: 10.1016/S1005-8885(07)60038-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fei, Z., Zhou, X., and Liu, X. (2011). Perfectionism and attention to imperfect figures. J. Psychol. Sci. 34, 532–537.

Google Scholar

Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., and Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of perfectionism. Cogn. Ther. Res. 14, 449–468. doi: 10.1007/bf01172967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaudreau, P., and Thompson, A. (2010). Testing a 2×2 model of dispositional perfectionism. Personal. Individ. Differ. 48, 532–537. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gnilka, P. B., Ashby, J. S., and Noble, C. M. (2012). Multidimensional perfectionism and anxiety: differences among individuals with perfectionism and tests of a coping-mediation model. J. Counsel. Dev. 90, 427–436. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2012.00054.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gong, X., Xie, X., Xu, R., and Yuejia, L. (2010). Psychometric properties of the Chinese versions of DASS-21 in Chinese college students. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 18, 443–446.

Google Scholar

Graham, R., Hoover, A., Ceballos, N. A., and Komogortsev, O. (2011). Body mass index moderates gaze orienting biases and pupil diameter to high and low calorie food images. Appetite 56, 577–586. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howell, J. A., McEvoy, P. M., Grafton, B., Macleod, C., Kane, R. T., Anderson, R. A., et al. (2016). Selective attention in perfectionism: dissociating valence from perfectionism-relevance. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 51, 100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, J., Shu, C. Y., Hoiles, K. J., Clarke, P. J. F., Watson, H. J., Dunlop, P. D., et al. (2018). Perfectionism is associated with higher eating disorder symptoms and lower remission in children and adolescents diagnosed with eating disorders. Eat. Behav. 30, 55–60. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2018.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kellough, J. L., Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A. J., and Wells, T. T. (2008). Time course of selective attention in clinically depressed young adults: an eye tracking study. Behav. Res. Ther. 46, 1238–1243. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kobori, O., and Tanno, Y. (2012). Self-oriented perfectionism and its relationship to selective attention: an experimental examination using social cognitive paradigm 1. Jap. Psychol. Res. 54, 418–423. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5884.2012.00514.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koster, E. H. W., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., and Van Damme, S. (2005). Time course of attention for threatening pictures in high and low trait anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 1087–1098. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, S. H., and Lovibond, P. F. (1996). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. Sydney: Psychology Foundation of Australia.

Google Scholar

Macatee, R. J., Albanese, B. J., Schmidt, N. B., and Cougle, J. R. (2017). Attention bias towards negative emotional information and its relationship with daily worry in the context of acute stress: an eye-tracking study. Behav. Res. Ther. 90, 96–110. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathews, A., Ridgeway, V., and Williamson, D. A. (1996). Evidence for attention to threatening stimuli in depression. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 695–705. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(96)00046-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehr, K. E., and Adams, A. C. (2016). Self-compassion as a mediator of maladaptive perfectionism and depressive symptoms in college students. J. College Stud. Psychother. 30, 132–145. doi: 10.1080/87568225.2016.1140991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, J. H., Newall, C., Broeren, S., and Hudson, J. L. (2013). The role of perfectionism in cognitive behaviour therapy outcomes for clinically anxious children. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 547–554. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, R. C., Rasmussen, S., and Hawton, K. (2010). Predicting depression, anxiety and self-harm in adolescents: the role of perfectionism and acute life stress. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 52–59. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, W. D. (1997). An empirical typology of perfectionism in academically talented children. Am. Educ. Res. J. 34, 545–562. doi: 10.2307/1163249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ren, L., and Liu, A. (2013). Attentional bias for imperfect pictures in perfectionist. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 21, 541–544.

Google Scholar

Shafran, R., Cooper, Z., and Fairburn, C. G. (2002). Clinical perfectionism: a cognitive–behavioural analysis. Behav. Res. Ther. 40, 773–791. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00059-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shafran, R., Egan, S. J., and Wade, T. D. (2010). Overcoming Perfectionism: A Self-Help Guide to Using Cognitive Behavioral Techniques. London: Robinson.

Google Scholar

Shechner, T., Jarcho, J. M., Wong, S., Leibenluft, E., Pine, D. S., and Nelson, E. E. (2017). Threats, rewards, and attention deployment in anxious youth and adults: an eye tracking study. Biol. Psychol. 122, 121–129. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, J., Sharpe, L., Colagiuri, B., and Khatibi, A. (2016). The effect of threat on cognitive biases and pain outcomes: an eye-tracking study. Eur. J. Pain 20, 1357–1368. doi: 10.1002/ejp.887

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tonta, K. E., Howell, J. A., Hasking, P. A., Boyes, M. E., and Clarke, P. J. F. (2019). Attention biases in perfectionism: biased disengagement of attention from emotionally negative stimuli. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 64, 72–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vervoort, T., Trost, Z., Prkachin, K. M., and Mueller, S. C. (2013). Attentional processing of other’s facial display of pain: an eye tracking study. Pain 154, 836–844.

Google Scholar

Yang, Z., Jackson, T., and Chen, H. (2013). Effects of chronic pain and pain-related fear on orienting and maintenance of attention: an eye movement study. J. Pain 14, 1148–1157. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Z., Jackson, T., Gao, X., and Chen, H. (2012). Identifying selective visual attention biases related to fear of pain by tracking eye movements within a dot-probe paradigm. Pain 153, 1742–1748. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yiend, J., and Mathews, A. (2005). “Selective attention tasks in clinical research,” in Cognitive Methods and Their Applications to Clinical Research, eds A. Wenzel and D. C. Rubin (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 97–117.

Google Scholar

Keywords: attentional bias, imperfect pictures, perfectionism, eye movements, visual attention task

Citation: Li J, Liu X, Yu B, Tang W and Liu X (2020) Attentional Bias for Imperfect Pictures in Perfectionism: An Eye-Movement Study. Front. Psychol. 11:566482. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566482

Received: 28 May 2020; Accepted: 31 August 2020;
Published: 02 October 2020.

Edited by:

Fushun Wang, Nanjing University of Chinese Medicine, China

Reviewed by:

Fei Luo, Chinese Academy of Sciences (CAS), China
Poppy Watson, University of New South Wales, Australia

Copyright © 2020 Li, Liu, Yu, Tang and Liu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiping Liu, lxp3771@sina.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.