Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 13 February 2019
Sec. Schizophrenia

Early Somatosensory Processing Over Time in Individuals at Risk to Develop Psychosis

  • 1The Zurich Program for Sustainable Development of Mental Health Services (ZInEP), University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Psychiatric Services Aargau, Academic Hospital of the University of Zurich, Brugg, Switzerland
  • 4Translational Neuromodeling Unit, Institute for Biomedical Engineering, University of Zurich and ETH Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 5Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
  • 6Laboratory of Neuroscience (LIM 27), Institute of Psychiatry, University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
  • 7Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Charité University Medicine, Berlin, Germany

Objective: Somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) enable the investigation of thalamocortical and early cortical processing. Previous studies reported alterations of SEPs in patients with schizophrenia as well as in individuals in the prodromal stage. Moreover, cannabis use as an environmental risk factor for the development of schizophrenia has been demonstrated to influence SEP parameters in individuals at risk to develop psychosis. The aim of this study was to explore the course of SEP changes and the impact of concomitant cannabis use in individuals at risk to develop psychosis who sought medical help.

Methods: Median nerve SEPs including high-frequency oscillations (HFOs) superimposed on the primary cortical response (N20) were investigated using multichannel EEG in individuals (n = 54 at baseline) remaining at risk to develop psychosis at follow-up after 1 year (high-risk: n = 19; ultra-high-risk: n = 27) vs. subjects with conversion to psychosis (n = 8) and a healthy control group (n = 35). Longitudinal and cross-sectional analyses of SEP components as estimated by dipole source analysis were performed.

Results: The longitudinal development of the N20 strength depended on cannabis use. In cannabis non-users, a greater decrease of N20 strengths over time was associated with more negative symptoms at baseline. At baseline, converters did not differ from subjects remaining at risk. At follow-up, converters showed increased low- and high-frequency activity than at-risk subjects and did not differ from controls.

Conclusion: The results of this study lead to the suggestion that the deficits in early somatosensory processing in individuals at risk to develop psychosis may not represent a marker for a genetic risk for psychosis but rather reflect state-dependent factors such as negative symptoms. On the other hand, the transition to psychosis seems to represent an interstage between reduced sensory registration from the at-risk state and gating deficits in the chronic state.

Introduction

The neuropathological picture of psychosis is not static but changes over time (1). Patients with psychosis often show less severe manifestations of the illness for days up to years prior to onset of full-blown clinical picture (2). These early signs are characterized by subtle, self-experienced deficits and mostly non-specific symptoms, also termed “basic symptoms” (3) often accompanied by attenuated negative symptoms (35). Early signs that are more proximal to the onset of psychosis tend to be more specific for psychotic disorders and are mostly characterized by attenuated positive symptoms of psychosis (6, 7). The term “psychosis risk syndrome” was proposed to indicate a possible risk of developing psychosis (4, 8).

Early intervention is thought to reduce the duration of untreated psychosis and to prevent or delay transition to psychosis by attenuating the symptoms of individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis (4, 9). Higher levels of attenuated positive symptoms, poor social functioning, some genetic risk, e.g., having a first-degree relative with a psychotic disorder, have been so far identified as consistent predictors of conversion to psychosis (10). There is additionally some evidence that cannabis use, respectively cannabis use on top of a genetic vulnerability, increases the risk of developing psychosis (1113).

For better prognostic accuracy, neurobiological markers are also increasingly used in early recognition of psychosis. Human median nerve somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs) offer the opportunity to investigate thalamocortical and early cortical processing (1417). The thalamus as a gate of access for sensory information to reach the cerebral cortex (1820) is considered to play a crucial role in the pathophysiology of psychosis (2123) as well as of bipolar disorder, particularly with a history of psychosis (24). Based on their heritability and state-independence, deficits in sensory gating have frequently been discussed as a potential neurophysiological endophenotype of psychosis. They have been shown in subjects at risk as well as in patients with first-episode and chronic psychosis, are present in both predominantly positive and negative symptom patients, and have also been found in unaffected relatives and in schizotypal personality disorder (25). However, this hypothesis needs further elucidation, as sensory gating deficits were significantly more pronounced in chronic psychosis compared to the at-risk state and first-episode psychosis suggesting that they might be a state marker of the expression of the disease rather than a state-independent marker of genetic risk for psychosis (26).

SEPs initially show a cortical low-frequency response occurring 20 ms after stimulation, i.e., the N20 (27), which is supposed to be generated by excitatory postsynaptic potentials in Brodman area 3b pyramidal cells (28). With high-pass filtering, an oscillatory burst of low-amplitude and high-frequency (600 Hz) wavelets superimposed on the N20 can be isolated (27, 2933). These somatosensory high-frequency oscillations (HFOs) in SEP have been shown to play an important role in sensory information processing (34). Early and late HFOs, i.e., the wavelets before and after the peak latency of the initial N20, have been supposed to arise from separate generators since these two components differ in their responsiveness to various modulations, such as sleep-wake cycle or tactile interference (34, 35). The early part of the HFOs is presumably generated from action potentials of thalamocortical fibers (3537). The generation of the late part of HFOs remains unclear as pyramidal “chattering” cells (38), cortical fast-spiking inhibitory interneurons (33) and thalamocortical relay cells (35) have been proposed as neuronal generators.

Like other early evoked potentials, i.e., occurring within 50 ms after stimulus application, SEPs are less susceptible to changes by uncontrollable factors such as attention than later potentials (17). Even if early evoked potentials have been less often studied than later ones, their underlying neurophysiology is better understood than that of the later potentials (14, 17). Therefore, altered SEPs might represent a robust indicator of vulnerability for schizophrenia and may offer the possibility of identifying potential patients before the onset of psychosis and of supporting prevention efforts.

The previously reported analysis of the baseline data of the present at-risk sample showed that alterations of early sensory filtering as measured by SEPs were observable also at the at-risk level (15). Subjects at risk for psychosis showed reduced low- and high-frequency source activity compared to subjects at risk for bipolar disorders and reduced high-frequency amplitudes compared to healthy controls. This is in line with the increasing body of evidence suggesting a sensory gating deficit in schizophrenia (39) and adds evidence to the assumption that specific sensory dysfunctions precede the onset of psychosis.

The aim of the present study was to explore the suitability of SEP as a neurophysiological marker. Therefore, we investigated the course of early sensory processing in help-seeking individuals being at risk to develop psychosis at the time of the baseline examination (15). Specifically, we distinguished between subjects remaining at risk at follow-up vs. subjects who converted to manifest psychosis during this time. Based on the progressive dynamics of structural and functional brain changes throughout the development of psychosis (4042), the at-risk population was further subdivided into a high-risk (HR) and an ultra-high-risk (UHR) group. Low- and high-frequency components of SEPs measured at baseline and at follow-up 1 year later were analyzed in relationship to the clinical symptoms. Given that the previously reported differences in SEP measures at baseline were accentuated among cannabis non-users (15), we aimed to control specifically for the effect of cannabis in the present study, respectively, to explore SEP in cannabis-free subjects in case of an effect of cannabis in the whole group. We thought to find differences in SEP between converters and at-risk subjects as early as baseline, as it would be expected from a neurobiological marker.

Our research questions were therefore:

(1) How do SEP parameters vary over time in subjects at risk with the HR group compared to the UHR group (respectively in cannabis-free subjects)?

(2) How do SEP parameters vary over time in converters (retrospectively)?

(3) Are these neurophysiological alterations (SEP variations over time) correlated with psychopathology (positive and negative symptoms at baseline)?

(4) Are there differences between converters compared to subjects remaining at risk to develop psychosis at follow-up regarding early sensory filtering at baseline (retrospectively) and at follow-up (respectively among cannabis-free subjects)?

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Subjects at risk were recruited in the Canton of Zurich, Switzerland, within the framework of the “Zurich Program for Sustainable Development of Mental Health Services” (Zürcher Impulsprogramm zur nachhaltigen Entwicklung der Psychiatrie, i.e., ZInEP). For details regarding design, recruitment and sample of this prospective longitudinal multi-level approach on early recognition of psychoses, see Theodoridou et al. (43).

Psychiatrists, child and adolescent psychiatrists, psychologists, general practitioners, outreach clinics, counseling services, teachers, and affected persons or their family members could refer to one of the four regional early recognition units employing standardized criteria to identify persons at risk for psychosis and offering appropriate counseling. The assessment was conducted by trained and experienced psychiatrists or psychologists. SEP measurements were carried out in the neurophysiology lab at the central early recognition center.

After a complete description of the study to participants, written informed consent was obtained; in case of minors including the written informed consent of their parents. The study was approved by the regional ethics committee of the canton of Zurich and was performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Subjects had to fulfill at least one of the following three inclusion criteria:

(1) High-risk (HR) status for psychosis assessed by the adult (44) or children-youth (45) version of the Schizophrenia Proneness Interview (SPI-A/SPI-CY), with at least one cognitive-perceptive (COPER) basic symptom or at least two cognitive disturbances (COGDIS) basic symptoms.

(2) Ultra-high-risk (UHR) status for psychosis as rated by the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS) (6) with at least one attenuated psychotic symptom, or at least one brief limited intermittent psychotic symptom, or a positive state-trait criterion (reduction in global assessment of functioning of >30% in the past year, plus either schizotypal personality disorder or first degree relative with psychosis).

The division into high-risk (HR) and ultra-high-risk (UHR) subjects within the psychosis-risk group was made with the aim to distinguish between subjects with a more general risk (HR, only COPER and/or COGDIS) and subjects with imminent risk [UHR, only UHR (= 15%) and UHR plus COPER and/or COGDIS (= 85%)] of transition to manifest psychosis (5, 46). In addition to the scales for early recognition, the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (47) was conducted to measure also the more pronounced psychotic symptoms. Exclusion criteria for study participation were manifest schizophrenic, substance-induced, or organic psychosis or bipolar disorder, current substance or alcohol dependence, as assessed by the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (48); age below 13 or above 35 years; or low intellectual abilities with IQ < 80. Transition to psychosis was determined according to ICD-10 criteria.

Participants completed the SEP recordings shortly after study inclusion (t0, baseline assessment) and after 1 year (mean 12.9 months, SD = 2.5 months; t2, follow-up assessment). At baseline, SEP data were available from 155 subjects fulfilling inclusion criteria for HR and UHR groups. For comparison, 50 healthy controls (HC), matched regarding age and gender rates to the whole at-risk group, were enrolled in the study, 45 of them with available HF-SEPs. For socio-demographic details, see Hagenmuller et al. (15). The presence of any mental illness in the HC group was excluded using the MINI. All participants underwent a structural MRI at baseline prior to testing in order to exclude any brain abnormalities. At follow-up, SEP recordings were obtained from 54 subjects fulfilling inclusion criteria for HR or UHR groups at baseline. Due to project-specific issues, a follow-up assessment of the HC group could not be performed. Values in the HC group at baseline were taken as reference values.

Subjects classified as HR or UHR at baseline who converted to psychosis during the follow-up period (HR n = 1, UHR n = 7) were classified as the CONV group. Subjects classified as HR and UHR at baseline who did not convert still formed the HR resp. UHR groups (together termed the RISK group). Among the individuals from the HR, the UHR and the CONV groups, 37% at baseline and 46% at follow-up were receiving psychotropic medication, essentially antipsychotic and/or anti-depressive medication. The HC group did not differ statistically from the RISK group regarding age, sex and handedness. Specifically, we looked at subjects from the RISK group reporting no cannabis use at baseline, in contrast to cannabis users, i.e., subjects reporting frequent cannabis use (several times per week, weekly, and monthly). Subjects from the RISK group reporting rare use (less frequently than monthly) were not included in the cannabis related analysis. Individuals from the HC group reporting any cannabis use (n = 10) were excluded from the analysis. For details see Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the subjects from the high-risk (HR), the ultra-high-risk (UHR) for schizophrenia and the converter (CONV) groups included in the follow-up analysis compared to the healthy control (HC) group.

SEP Recording

Subjects were requested to sit in a comfortable chair with their eyes open, in a sound-attenuated laboratory room. They were instructed to relax and to avoid movements throughout the stimulus presentation sequence and the recording. Electrical transcutaneous stimulation was performed with two electrodes over the median nerve on the wrist of the dominant hand. Single constant-current square wave pulses of a duration of 0.2 s were delivered with an intensity of 4 mA above individual motor threshold (max. 20 mA) and a stimulus rate of 6 Hz during 12 min. To preserve a stable level of vigilance during stimulus presentation, participants were asked to watch a “Mr. Bean” movie without sound. EEG data were recorded using a BrainAmp amplifier and the Brain Vision Recorder software (both Brain Products GmbH, Munich, Germany). Electrodes were applied to the scalp using carefully positioned nylon caps (BrainCap with 32 channels, Easycap, Herrsching-Breitbrunn, Germany) in accordance with the international 10/20 system. Scalp electrode impedances were kept below 10 kΩ. EEG channels were referenced to FCz. Data were collected with a sampling rate of 2,500 Hz.

SEP Data Analysis

Using the Brain Electrical Source Analysis (BESA 5.1.8: MEGIS, Munich, Germany; www.besa.de) software, dipole source analysis was computed individually for each subject with at least 4,000 artifact-free sweeps over a period of 100 ms, from 20 ms before to 80 ms after the stimulation. Single dipole sources were fitted for each subject for a time period between 14 and 24 ms. An approach with one dipole was considered to be sufficient for demonstrating differences in signal composition between the subgroups, although an optimal source configuration would include at least three dipoles (14). This is in accordance with other studies, such as Norra et al. (50) or Waberski et al. (51). The resulting dipole waveform was digitally filtered with:

(1) A low-pass filter of 450 Hz (12 dB/octave slope, zero phase shift) and a high-pass filter of 40 Hz (12 dB/octave slope, zero phase shift) to determine latency and strength of the low-frequency activity as estimated by dipole source analysis. The strength of the low-frequency activity source was determined semi-automatically as the absolute value of the minimum of the source waveform between 14 and 24 ms (N20). See Figures 1A–F for examples.

(2) A low-pass filter turned off and a high-pass filter of 450 Hz (12 dB/octave slope, zero phase shift) to extract HFOs. Latencies of the negative oscillatory maxima and maximum peak-to-peak amplitudes were measured—peaking before the maximum of N20 for early HFO components, resp. after the N20 maximum for the late HFO components (52).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Examples of N20 curves at baseline (t0) and at follow-up (t2) from the (A) healthy control (HC), (B) converter (CONV), (C) high-risk (HR) cannabis non-users, (D) ultra-high-risk (UHR) cannabis non-users, (E) high-risk (HR) cannabis users, and (F) ultra-high-risk (UHR) cannabis users groups.

All peaks were plotted with the software Python (Python Language Website, http://www.python.org) and inspected visually.

Statistical Analysis

The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test revealed that data were normally distributed (all p = n.s.). As the Levene test revealed homogeneity of most variances, parametrical tests were used.

(1) Regarding the HR, UHR, and CONV groups over time, longitudinal data of interest (SEP parameters) were analyzed with paired t-tests and repeated-measures ANOVA with the within factor “time” and the between factor “cannabis use,” and “age” (in years) and “medication” (yes/no) at baseline as covariates. For clarity, only significant effects of covariates were mentioned. For the cannabis-related analysis, because of the small group size of cannabis users, we grouped the HR and the UHR groups (without CONV) into one RISK group. Significant effects of cannabis led to further analysis among cannabis non-users. To further explore changes over time, within-subject indices of N20 strengths were obtained by subtracting values at baseline from values at follow-up (N20 diff), with a negative result indicating a decrease in N20 strength (t0 > t2).

(2) Correlation coefficients between SEP variations over time and clinical psychopathology were calculated with Pearson's r. The psychopathological data included SIPS positive and negative symptoms at baseline.

(3) With regard to the HR, UHR, CONV, and HC groups at follow-up, cross-sectional group comparisons were performed with Student's t-test, ANOVA (N20 strength as dependent variable) or MANOVA (peak-to-peak amplitudes of HFO early and late parts resp. latencies of N20, HFO early and late parts as dependent variables). Age was entered as covariate (significant effects mentioned). Because the HC group was medication-free, the covariate medication was removed from the cross-sectional analyses including the HC group. In order to reduce possible errors due to multiple testing, simple contrasts were performed to look at the difference of the CONV group vs. the other groups at baseline and at follow-up.

Statistical significance was taken as p < 0.05. Based on the small sample sizes, effect sizes were provided for the group comparisons (Cohen's d, with d = 0.3 indicating a small, d = 0.5 a medium and d = 0.8 a large effect). SEP parameters were standardized by computing z-values based on the SEP values of the HC group at baseline. All statistical tests were performed using the IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0 package (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).

Results

Variation of SEP Parameters in the HR, UHR, and CONV Groups From Baseline to Follow-Up

SEP parameters of the HR and UHR groups at baseline and follow-up are given in Table 2. Within the HR and UHR group (without CONV), repeated measures ANOVA revealed no differences in all SEP parameters between baseline and follow-up, but a significant interaction of measurement time and cannabis use on the N20 strength [F(1,23) = 8.51, p = 0.008]. In particular, cannabis non-users differed from cannabis users regarding the time-dependent evolution of the N20 source activity.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. SEP parameters for the high-risk (HR), the ultra-high-risk (UHR), and the converter (CONV) groups compared to the baseline values of the healthy control (HC) group, given in mean (SD).

The N20 strength of cannabis non-users at risk for psychosis seemed to decrease over time, while the N20 strength of cannabis users seemed to increase. The mean N20 strength of cannabis non-users was lower at follow-up than at baseline, but this difference did not reach significance [t(18) = 1.58, p = 0.131]. Moreover, cannabis non-users showed a significantly weaker mean N20 strength at follow-up compared to the HC group [t(52) = 2.96, p = 0.005, d = 0.821].

On the other hand, the mean N20 strength of cannabis users was significantly higher at follow-up than at baseline [t(7) = −2.59, p = 0.036, d = 1.958]. Interestingly, the mean N20 strength of cannabis users did not, at follow-up, differ significantly from the HC group [t(41) = −0.65, p = 0.523], see Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean N20 strengths in cannabis non-users (n = 19) vs. cannabis users (n = 8) from the RISK group at baseline (t0) and at follow-up (t2), compared to the reference values from the HC group (n = 35).

Contrary to the observations in the HR and UHR groups, SEP parameters of the CONV group seemed to increase over time. In particular, paired t-tests revealed that the CONV group showed a significantly stronger HFO late part at follow-up compared to baseline [t(6) = 2.61, p = 0.040, d = 2.131, see Table 2].

Relationship Between SEP Parameters, Psychopathology, and Antipsychotic Medication

With regard to the psychopathological data, there were no differences in SIPS scores between cannabis non-users and cannabis users neither at baseline [positive, negative, disorganized, and general symptoms, t(28) < 0.7, p > 0.54] nor at follow-up [t(25) < 1.46, p > 0.16]. Moreover, there were no differences in antipsychotic medication between cannabis non-users and cannabis users neither at baseline [chlorpromazine equivalent [CPZe), t(28) < 0.72, p > 0.48] nor at follow-up [t(25) < 1.6, p > 0.12].

To further explore the decrease of the N20 strength in cannabis non-users, correlations between within-subject indices of the evolution of the N20 strengths (N20 diff) over time and the extent of the positive and negative symptoms at baseline were calculated. N20 diff correlated negatively with SIPS negative symptoms at baseline (r = −0.57, p = 0.011), indicating higher scores on the negative symptoms subscale of the SIPS at baseline being associated with a greater decrease of N20 strengths over time (Figure 3). On the other hand, there was no relationship between the N20 decrease and SIPS positive symptoms at baseline.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Correlations of negative symptoms at baseline with the amount of N20 strength decrease over time (N20 diff) in cannabis non-users from the RISK group, with negative values representing smaller values at follow-up than at baseline.

Given the potential impact of antipsychotic medication on the neurophysiological data, additional correlational analyses on the relationship between SEP parameters and CPZe were performed. However, there was no relationship between SEP parameters and antipsychotic medication neither at baseline (r < 0.31, p > 0.30) nor at follow-up (r < −0.18, p > 0.19).

SEP Parameters at Baseline

At baseline, there was a significant main effect of group on the strengths of HFO late part [F(3, 57) = 2.88, p = 0.044], but not on the strengths of the N20 nor HFO early part (see Table 2). Planned contrasts revealed that, at baseline, the CONV group showed a statistical trend toward lower amplitudes of HFO late part [0.62, 95%CI (– 0.12, 1.36), p = 0.095, d = 0.85] compared to the HC group. On the other hand, the CONV group did not differ from the HR nor UHR groups. Cannabis use had no significant effect on the SEP parameters at baseline.

SEP Parameters at Follow-Up

At follow-up, there was a significant main effect of group on the N20 strength [F(3,62) = 3.09, p = 0.033, see Table 2]. Planned contrasts revealed that the CONV group had significantly stronger N20 intensity compared to the UHR group [4.96, 95%CI (0.47, 9.45), p = 0.031, d = 1.057]. There was also a significant main effect of cannabis [F(1,62) = 7.04, p = 0.010], so that we repeated group comparisons within the cannabis non-users subgroup.

Among cannabis non-users, ANOVA showed significant differences between groups (HC, HR, UHR, CONV) regarding N20 source strengths [F(3,56) = 3.22, p = 0.029]. Planned contrasts revealed that the UHR group had significantly weaker N20 strengths [−3.89, 95%CI (−6.76, 1.02), p = 0.009, d = 1.01] compared to the HC group. In contrast, the HR and the CONV groups did not significantly differ from the HC group regarding these parameters.

Removing the HC group for an exploration of the differences between at-risk subjects vs. converters at follow-up, ANOVA showed significant differences among cannabis non-users regarding N20 source strengths [F(2,21) = 4.87, p = 0.018] and amplitudes of the late part of HFOs [F(2,13) = 5.31, p = 0.021]. Moreover, there was a trend regarding the amplitudes of the early part of HFOs [F(2,13) = 3.56, p = 0.058]. Planned contrasts revealed that the CONV group had stronger N20 source activity compared to the HR [−3.49, 95%CI (−6.88, −0.09), p = 0.045, d = 1.409] and the UHR group [−4.63, 95%CI (−7.74, −1.51), p = 0.006, d = 1.739], as well as greater early HFO amplitudes [−0.68, 95%CI (−1.30, −0.07), p = 0.032, d = 0.987] and greater late HFO amplitudes compared to the UHR group [−0.62, 95%CI (−1.03, −0.21), p = 0.006, d = 1.434]. In addition, there was a statistical trend for greater HFO amplitudes in the CONV group compared to the HR group [−0.63, 95%CI (−1.28, 0.015), p = 0.055, d = 0.544], as shown in Figure 4. Finally, there was a significant effect of medication on late HFO amplitudes [F(1,13) = 9.98, p = 0.008].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mean SEP amplitudes (z-values) for the cannabis non-user subjects from the high-risk (HR, n = 10), ultra high-risk (UHR, n = 9), and converter (CONV, n = 7) groups at baseline (t0) and follow-up (t2).

Discussion

In this study, low and high frequency SEP data were collected at baseline and 1 year later at follow-up in 54 subjects who were at risk for developing psychosis at time of baseline examination. This study investigated the course of early sensory filtering preceding the (putative) onset of psychosis. To our knowledge, this is the first study investigating SEPs in a longitudinal design in populations at risk for developing psychosis to date. Based on the baseline findings (15), we conducted our analysis regarding cannabis, respectively, we focused specifically on cannabis-free subjects.

The longitudinal development of the N20 strength depended on cannabis use. The N20 strengths of cannabis non-users seemed to decrease over time. SIPS negative symptoms were associated with this decrease, i.e., more negative symptoms at baseline were associated with a greater decrease of N20 strength over time. Additionally, we found that cannabis-free subjects at risk—and specifically at ultra-high-risk for psychosis—had weaker N20 strengths at follow-up, compared to the HC group. These results are in line with our baseline results (15) and previous reports on decreased early evoked responses in the at-risk state for psychosis (26). Amplitude reductions in the context of reduced sensory gating were interpreted as a reduced sensory registration (26). Sensory registration can be characterized by high sensory thresholds, i.e., subjects with low sensory registration pattern may have more difficulties than others to notice sensory stimuli (53). These observed amplitude reductions in subjects at risk suggest that sensory registration deficits may precede the putative onset of psychosis. Furthermore, it has been proposed that gating deficits are mainly associated with the negative symptoms of schizophrenia (5458). Because they are related to decreased functional outcomes and refractory to common psychopharmacological treatments (59), negative symptoms seem to contribute to poorer quality of life early in the course of schizophrenia (60, 61) and may even represent themselves a vulnerability for the risk of developing psychosis (62). Moreover, the thalamus that is supposed to play a key role in somatosensory gating might be specifically associated with negative symptoms in schizophrenia (6366). This association was also suggested in adolescents at ultra-high-risk for psychosis in a recent study in the context of increased sleep dysfunction (67). Negative symptoms have been described as emerging from a failure of learning during social interactions due to abnormal synaptic plasticity, the resulting experience of an unpredictable world being likely to trigger social withdrawal and apathy (68). In this line, Arnfred and Chen (69) have reported an association of early somatosensory information processing deficit and higher social anhedonia scores. They have hypothesized that a lack of somatosensory readiness degrading the fine-tuning of bodily attention would lead to aversive experiences of unpreparedness, secondarily followed by withdrawal and anxiety (69).

The biological basis of sensory registration deficits in subjects at risk as well as in patients with manifest psychosis is still not clear. Alterations in early evoked potentials, such as SEPs, are typically referred to disturbances of afferent sensory tracts and impaired thalamic filtering. However, abnormal functional interactions between the cortex and thalamus should be considered as well. In this context, several imaging studies reported characteristic dysfunctions of thalamocortical networks in schizophrenia that are closely associated with clinical and cognitive symptoms (70). With regard to early sensory information processing, Woodward et al. (71) could demonstrate, by using resting-state fMRI, marked differences in thalamocortical connectivity in patients with schizophrenia compared to healthy controls, with increased motor/somatosensory-thalamic connectivity and reduced pre-frontal-thalamic connectivity. Interestingly, this pattern of thalamocortical dysconnectivity was not only present in chronic schizophrenia but also in early stages of psychosis (72), presumably due to atypical brain maturation and/or distinct genetic factors (73). Moreover, it cannot be ruled out that abnormal functional interactions within specific cortical networks may be involved in the deficits in sensory information processing as described in our analysis. In particular, disturbed functional connectivity and organization within the somatosensory and sensorimotor cortex in schizophrenia have been reported by several neurophysiological studies (7476). Further research is required in order to elucidate the etiological significance.

In our sample, contrary to subjects at risk, the group of converters showed no changes in negative symptoms between baseline and follow-up. Our retrospective analysis showed that, at baseline, the group of converters differed from the control group regarding amplitudes of HFOs, as did at-risk subjects in our baseline study (15). However, at baseline, the group of subjects with future transition to psychosis did not differ from other subjects remaining at risk at follow-up. These results are in accordance with other studies (26, 77) which found deficits in sensory registration in subjects at risk compared to controls but did not find any differences between at-risk with and without conversion. Consequently, it has been suggested that these deficits in sensory registration may not represent a marker for a genetic risk for psychosis but rather be influenced by state-dependent factors (26). Alternatively, it cannot be excluded that putative converters are confounded within the at-risk groups and that a longer follow-up period would have influenced these results by providing more transitions to psychosis. Furthermore, due to different treatment regimens between baseline and follow-up, some conversions have possibly been prevented by treatment. Finally, a lack of statistical power due to the very small sample size of the converter group cannot be excluded.

At follow up, according to our expectations, we found differences in SEP parameters between the group of converters and the subjects remaining at risk, but these differences turned out to be somewhat counterintuitive. Converters showed stronger SEP parameters than at-risk subjects but did not differ from the HC group. It is not clear how this should be interpreted. In contrast to the present findings, previous studies on SEP in patients with chronic schizophrenia reported higher N20 mean amplitudes (16, 17, 50) and later HFO than controls (50). A higher N20 amplitude was interpreted as an impairment of thalamic filtering of the afferent sensory information to the cortex (50). More generally, the present findings stand in contradiction to the extensive literature reporting decreased evoked responses in patients with schizophrenia, particularly decreased early responses such as N100 and P50 (56, 78). However, Valkonen-Korhonen et al. (79) also found no difference in early attention-independent automatic processing (100 ms after stimulus) in never-medicated first episode patients compared to healthy controls and reported that otherwise observed deviances appeared only in attention-dependent processing after about 250 ms. Furthermore, Salisbury et al. (80) reported deviant mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with chronic schizophrenia that were not present at first hospitalization as well as opposite associations of pattern of symptoms with MMN amplitudes in first episode patients (smaller amplitudes) vs. in chronic patients (larger amplitudes). Taken together, a transition to psychosis resp. a first episode may represent an interstage between a reduced sensory registration from the at-risk state and a gating deficit in the chronic state. Our longitudinal analyses accounted for potential medication impact, but we did not differentiate the effects of antidepressants vs. antipsychotics because of the small sample size and the cross-sectional analyses. We omitted the covariate medication in the cross-sectional analyses because it was closely related to the group factor (the HC group was medication-free). Consequently, it cannot be ruled out that medication has an effect on praxis level on all SEP parameters, particularly in the converter group that showed the highest dosage. Moreover, the fact that antipsychotic drugs are not equally effective for all patients with psychosis contribute to the assumption that heterogeneity of different underlying disease mechanisms may lead to a psychopathology with similar clinical presentation. For example, different neuromodulatory dysregulations—such as dopaminergic, cholinergic, or serotonergic dysregulation of NMDA-mediated synaptic plasticity—are hypothesized to lead to the development of psychotic symptoms (68). Therefore, our sample of subjects with resp. without transition to psychosis would represent a heterogeneous group (81) with regard to underlying synaptic plasticity. Furthermore, symptoms of depression, anxiety and psychosis often overlap in subjects at high risk of psychosis (82). Besides this, different patterns of brain structure alteration were suggested in subjects in at-risk state with genetic risk vs. with attenuated psychotic symptoms, the subgroup with genetic risk showing more similarities of morphology to patients with a first episode psychosis, one of them affecting the right thalamus (83). Given that most of the at-risk subjects do not convert to manifest psychosis, the biological significance of the decreased N20 strengths in our at-risk groups remains unclear and needs further replication.

Contrary to the decrease in N20 strengths over time in the cannabis non-users group, the N20 strengths of cannabis users seemed to increase over time, being similar at follow-up to the reference values from the HC group. As it was hypothesized previously (11, 84), cannabis may produce transient symptoms and deficits resembling those seen in schizophrenia, thereby increasing clinical false positive ratings. Additionally, a superior cognitive profile in schizophrenia-spectrum disorders with concomitant cannabis use has been reported (8587), but this was suggested to be associated with better social skills (in order to be able to acquire and sustain drug habit) rather than to reflect a neuroprotective effect of THC (84, 88). In this line, Koenders et al. (89) have provided the possible explanation that non-users are a group in whom psychosis may develop without the extra-risk factor of cannabis use, i.e., that patients who use cannabis may be less intrinsically vulnerable for psychosis than non-users.

Our study has methodological limitations. First, the group of converters was very small, so that statistic power may be limited and results in this group have to be interpreted with caution. Based on the heuristic and exploratory nature of the study, we did not consider a Bonferroni correction for multiple testing and recommend to replicate the findings in future studies with greater sample sizes. Moreover, the HC group has not been screened for prodromal symptoms at baseline by standardized measures, such as SPI-A or SIPS. Therefore, the validity of the results may be limited as the potential influence of undiscovered pre-psychotic syndromes at the low-intensity level cannot be completely ruled out. Then, cannabis analysis is limited because many data is missing, we relied on self-reports and did not perform urinary screening as recommended in the literature (90) and we did not differentiate between acute and chronic effects of cannabis (91). However, an analysis of cannabis effects was not the aim of this paper. On the other hand, the observed results might be confounded with an effect of alcohol and tobacco, as several subjects being at risk for psychosis may be prone to use these substances. At last, an examination of the HC group was performed only at baseline. We assumed that the time effect of 1 year on SEP parameters would be negligible in healthy participants. We are aware that small developmental changes in the somatosensory afferent pathway from spinal cord to thalamus seem to be complete by age 17 (92) and some of our participants were younger than this age. Increases in EP latency with age were reported (92). Then, the amplitude of the cortical primary response is known to follow a U-shape curve with aging, being high during adolescence, low during middle age and high again in old age (9395). Additionally, a comparison of participants older than 60 years with participants in the mid-twenties suggested that the later part of HFOs is associated with aging (96). However, our groups were all in similar ages and statistical analysis did not show any effect of age. None the less, a follow-up examination of the HC group would have provided further insight. Finally, the presently reported method of dipole source analysis is not suitable for clinical practice. Further experimentation is needed to determine if a simple one-channel information would lead to comparable results.

In summary, the observed deficits in sensory registration may not represent a marker for a genetic risk for psychosis but rather reflect state-dependent factors, e.g., negative symptoms. These amplitude reductions in subjects at risk vs. the lack of differences between converters and controls resp. the amplitude increases in chronic schizophrenia previously reported in the literature might suggest that a transition to psychosis resp. a first episode may represent an interstage between a reduced sensory registration from the at-risk state and a gating deficit in the chronic state.

Author Contributions

FH, KH, HH, AT, SW, WR, and WK designed the study and wrote the protocol. FH, KH, and PR managed the literature searches and analyses. FH undertook the statistical analysis. FH, KH, and PR wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The Zurich Program for Sustainable Development of Mental Health Services (ZInEP) was supported by a private donation. The donor had no further role in the experimental design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, writing, and submitting this paper for publication. The authors are grateful for the assistance from Pascal Hagenmuller in programming data plotting and peak export as well as from Jacqueline Lutz, Magali Meier, and Lukas Moll in data acquisition.

References

1. Bakhshi K, Chance SA. The neuropathology of schizophrenia: a selective review of past studies and emerging themes in brain structure and cytoarchitecture. Neuroscience (2015) 303:82–102. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.06.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Häfner H, van der Heiden W. The course of schizophrenia in the light of modern follow-up studies: the ABC and WHO studies. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (1999) 249:S14–26.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

3. Schultze-Lutter F. Subjective symptoms of schizophrenia in research and the clinic: the basic symptom concept. Schizophr Bull. (2009) 35:5–8. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Correll CU, Hauser M, Auther AM, Cornblatt BA. Research in people with psychosis risk syndrome: a review of the current evidence and future directions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2010) 51:390–431. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02235.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Bechdolf A, Addington J, Riecher-Rössler A, Schultze-Lutter F, et al. The psychosis high-risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry (2013) 70:107–20. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. McGlashan TH, Miller TJ, Woods SW, Rosen JL, Hoffman RE, Davidson L. Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes. New Haven, CT: PRIME Research Clinic, Yale School of Medicine (2001).

Google Scholar

7. Yung AR, McGorry PD, McFarlane CA, Jackson HJ, Patton GC, Rakkar A. Monitoring and care of young people at incipient risk of psychosis. Schizophr Bull. (1996) 22:283–303. doi: 10.1093/schbul/22.2.283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. McGorry PD, Nelson B, Amminger GP, Bechdolf A, Francey SM, Berger G, et al. Intervention in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis: a review and future directions. J Clin Psychiatry (2009) 70:1206–12. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08r04472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Schultze-Lutter D-PF, Picker H, Ruhrmann S, Klosterkötter J. Das kölner früh-erkennungs- & therapie-zentrum für psychische Krisen (FETZ). Med Klin. (2008) 103:81–9. doi: 10.1007/s00063-008-1012-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Gee DG, Cannon TD. Prediction of conversion to psychosis: review and future directions. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. (2011) 33:129–42. doi: 10.1590/S1516-44462011000600002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. DeLisi LE. The effect of cannabis on the brain: can it cause brain anomalies that lead to increased risk for schizophrenia? Curr Opin Psychiatry (2008) 21:140–50. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3282f51266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Marconi A, Di Forti M, Lewis CM, Murray RM, Vassos E. Meta-analysis of the association between the level of cannabis use and risk of psychosis. Schizophr Bull. (2016) 42:1262–9. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Roser P, Vollenweider FX, Kawohl W. Potential antipsychotic properties of central cannabinoid (CB1) receptor antagonists. World J Biol Psychiatry (2010) 11:208–19. doi: 10.3109/15622970801908047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Buchner H, Adams L, Müller A, Ludwig I, Knepper A, Thron A, et al. Somatotopy of human hand somatosensory cortex revealed by dipole source analysis of early somatosensory evoked potentials and 3D-NMR tomography. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol EP Sect. (1995) 96:121–34.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

15. Hagenmuller F, Heekeren K, Theodoridou A, Walitza S, Haker H, Rössler W, et al. Early somatosensory processing in individuals at risk for developing psychoses. Front Behav Neurosci. (2014) 8:308. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Lehtonen J. Somatosensory evoked potentials and the psychology of chronic schizophrenia. An integrative view. J Nerv Ment Dis. (1981) 169:256–8. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198104000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Shagass C, Straumanis JJ Jr, Roemer RA, Amadeo M. Evoked potentials of schizophrenics in several sensory modalities. Biol Psychiatry (1977) 12:221–35.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

18. Geyer MA, Swerdlow NR, Mansbach RS, Braff DL. Startle response models of sensorimotor gating and habituation deficits in schizophrenia. Brain Res Bull. (1990) 25:485–98. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(90)90241-Q

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Javitt DC, Freedman R. Sensory processing dysfunction in the personal experience and neuronal machinery of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2015) 172:17–31. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13121691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. McCormick DA, Bal T. Sensory gating mechanisms of the thalamus. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (1994) 4:550–6. doi: 10.1016/0959-4388(94)90056-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Andreasen NC. The role of the thalamus in schizophrenia. Can J Psychiatry (1997) 42:27–33. doi: 10.1177/070674379704200104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Byne W, Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS, Kemether E. The thalamus and schizophrenia: current status of research. Acta Neuropathol. (2009) 117:347–68. doi: 10.1007/s00401-008-0404-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Klingner CM, Langbein K, Dietzek M, Smesny S, Witte OW, Sauer H, et al. Thalamocortical connectivity during resting state in schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2014) 264:111–9. doi: 10.1007/s00406-013-0417-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Cheng CH, Chan PS, Liu CY, Hsu SC. Auditory sensory gating in patients with bipolar disorders: a meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2016) 203:199–203. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Turetsky BI, Calkins ME, Light GA, Olincy A, Radant AD, Swerdlow NR. Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophr Bull. (2007) 33:69–94. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Brockhaus-Dumke A, Schultze-Lutter F, Mueller R, Tendolkar I, Bechdolf A, Pukrop R, et al. Sensory gating in schizophrenia: P50 and N100 gating in antipsychotic-free subjects at risk, first-episode, and chronic patients. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 64:376–84. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Yamada T, Kameyama S, Fuchigami Y, Nakazumi Y, Dickins QS, Kimura J. Changes of short latency somatosensory evoked potential in sleep. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1988) 70:126–36. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90113-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Allison T, McCarthy G, Wood CC, Jones SJ. Potentials evoked in human and monkey cerebral cortex by stimulation of the median nerve. A review of scalp and intracranial recordings. Brain (1991) 114:2465–503. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.6.2465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Curio G, Mackert BM, Burghoff M, Koetitz R, Abraham-Fuchs K, Härer W. Localization of evoked neuromagnetic 600 Hz activity in the cerebral somatosensory system. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1994) 91:483–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

30. Eisen A, Roberts K, Low M, Hoirch M, Lawrence P. Questions regarding the sequential neural generator theory of the somatosensory evoked potential raised by digital filtering. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol EP Sect. (1984) 59:388–95. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(84)90040-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Emerson RG, Sgro JA, Pedley TA, Hauser WA. State-dependent changes in the N20 component of the median nerve somatosensory evoked potential. Neurology (1988) 38:64–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.38.1.64

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Emori T, Yamada T, Seki Y, Yasuhara A, Ando K, Honda Y, et al. Recovery functions of fast frequency potentials in the initial negative wave of median SEP. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1991) 78:116–23. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90111-G

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hashimoto I, Mashiko T, Imada T. Somatic evoked high-frequency magnetic oscillations reflect activity of inhibitory interneurons in the human somatosensory cortex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1996) 100:189–203. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(95)00244-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Ozaki I, Hashimoto I. Exploring the physiology and function of high-frequency oscillations (HFOs) from the somatosensory cortex. Clin Neurophysiol. (2011) 122:1908–23. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Curio G. Linking 600-Hz “spikelike” EEG/MEG wavelets (“sigma-bursts”) to cellular substrates: concepts and caveats. J Clin Neurophysiol. (2000) 17:377–96. doi: 10.1097/00004691-200007000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gobbelé R, Waberski TD, Simon H, Peters E, Klostermann F, Curio G, et al. Different origins of low- and high-frequency components (600 Hz) of human somatosensory evoked potentials. Clin Neurophysiol. (2004) 115:927–37. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Klostermann F, Nolte G, Curio G. Multiple generators of 600 Hz wavelets in human SEP unmasked by varying stimulus rates. Neuroreport (1999) 10:1625–9. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199906030-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Gray CM, McCormick DA. Chattering cells: superficial pyramidal neurons contributing to the generation of synchronous oscillations in the visual cortex. Science (1996) 274:109–13. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5284.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Braff DL. Prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex: a window on the brain in schizophrenia. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2001) 4:349–71. doi: 10.1007/7854_2010_61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Frommann I, Pukrop R, Brinkmeyer J, Bechdolf A, Ruhrmann S, Berning J, et al. Neuropsychological profiles in different at-risk states of psychosis: executive control impairment in the early–and additional memory dysfunction in the late–prodromal state. Schizophr Bull. (2011) 37:861–73. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Cropley VL, Lin A, Nelson B, Reniers RLEP, Yung AR, Bartholomeusz CF, et al. Baseline grey matter volume of non-transitioned “ultra-high risk” for psychosis individuals with and without attenuated psychotic symptoms at long-term follow-up. Schizophr Res. (2016) 173:152–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.05.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Wood SJ, Pantelis C, Velakoulis D, Yücel M, Fornito A, McGorry PD. Progressive changes in the development toward schizophrenia: studies in subjects at increased symptomatic risk. Schizophr Bull. (2008) 34:322–9. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Theodoridou A, Heekeren K, Dvorsky D, Metzler S, Franscini M, Haker H, et al. Early recognition of high risk of bipolar disorder and psychosis: an overview of the ZInEP “Early Recognition” study. Front Public Health (2014) 2:166. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2014.00166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Schultze-Lutter F, Addington J, Ruhrmann S, Klosterkötter J. Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument, Adult Version (SPI-A). Rome: Giovanni Fioriti (2007).

Google Scholar

45. Schultze-Lutter F, Koch E. Schizophrenia Proneness Instrument, Child & Youth Version (SPI-CY). Rome: Giovanni Fioriti (2010).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

46. Klosterkötter J, Schultze-Lutter F, Bechdolf A, Ruhrmann S. Prediction and prevention of schizophrenia: what has been achieved and where to go next? World Psychiatry (2011) 10:165–74. doi: 10.1002/j.2051-5545.2011.tb00044.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (1987) 13:261–76. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry (1998) 20:22–33.

Google Scholar

49. Andreasen NC, Pressler M, Nopoulos P, Miller D, Ho BC. Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized method for comparing exposure to different drugs. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 67:255–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Norra C, Waberski TD, Kawohl W, Kunert HJ, Hock D, Gobbelé R, et al. High-frequency somatosensory thalamocortical oscillations and psychopathology in schizophrenia. Neuropsychobiology (2004) 49:71–80. doi: 10.1159/000076413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Waberski TD, Norra C, Kawohl W, Thyerlei D, Hock D, Klostermann F, et al. Electrophysiological evidence for altered early cerebral somatosensory signal processing in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology (2004) 41:361–6. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.2004.00163.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Gobbelé R, Waberski TD, Thyerlei D, Thissen M, Darvas F, Klostermann F, et al. Functional dissociation of a subcortical and cortical component of high-frequency oscillations in human somatosensory evoked potentials by motor interference. Neurosci Lett. (2003) 350:97–100. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00877-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Dunn W. The impact of sensory processing abilities on the daily lives of young children and their families: a conceptual model. Infants Young Child (1997) 9:23–35. doi: 10.1097/00001163-199704000-00005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Arnfred SM. Exploration of auditory P50 gating in schizophrenia by way of difference waves. Behav Brain Funct. (2006) 2:1–6. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-2-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Boutros NN, Brockhaus-Dumke A, Gjini K, Vedeniapin A, Elfakhani M, Burroughs S, et al. Sensory-gating deficit of the N100 mid-latency auditory evoked potential in medicated schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res. (2009) 113:339–46. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Boutros NN, Mucci A, Diwadkar V, Tandon R. Negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Clin Schizophr Relat Psychoses (2014) 8:28–35B. doi: 10.3371/CSRP.BOMU.012513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Louchart-de la Chapelle S, Levillain D, Ménard JF, Van der Elst A, Allio G, Haouzir S, et al. P50 inhibitory gating deficit is correlated with the negative symptomatology of schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. (2005) 136:27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2003.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Ringel TM, Heidrich A, Jacob CP, Pfuhlmann B, Stoeber G, Fallgatter AJ. Sensory gating deficit in a subtype of chronic schizophrenic patients. Psychiatry Res. (2004) 125:237–45. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Buchanan RW. Persistent negative symptoms in schizophrenia: an overview. Schizophr Bull. (2007) 33:1013–22. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbl057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Ho BC, Nopoulos P, Flaum M, Arndt S, Andreasen NC. Two-year outcome in first-episode schizophrenia: predictive value of symptoms for quality of life. Am J Psychiatry (1998) 155:1196–201. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.9.1196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Tandon R, Nasrallah HA, Keshavan MS. Schizophrenia “just the facts” 4. Clinical features and conceptualization. Schizophr Res. (2009) 110:1–23. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Piskulic D, Addington J, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, Cornblatt BA, Heinssen R, et al. Negative symptoms in individuals at clinical high risk of psychosis. Psychiatry Res. (2012) 196:220–4. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Krause M, Hoffmann WE, Mihàly H. Auditory sensory gating in hippocampus and reticular thalamic neurons in anesthetized rats. Biol Psychiatry (2003) 53:244–53. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01463-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Liemburg EJ, Dlabac-De Lange JJ, Bais L, Knegtering H, van Osch MJP, Renken RJ, et al. Neural correlates of planning performance in patients with schizophrenia - relationship with apathy. Schizophr Res. (2015) 161:367–75. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.11.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Millan MJ, Fone K, Steckler T, Horan WP. Negative symptoms of schizophrenia: clinical characteristics, pathophysiological substrates, experimental models and prospects for improved treatment. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2014) 24:645–92. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Preuss UW, Zetzsche T, Jäger M, Groll C, Frodl T, Bottlender R, et al. Thalamic volume in first-episode and chronic schizophrenic subjects: a volumetric MRI study. Schizophr Res. (2005) 73:91–101. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Lunsford-Avery JR, Orr JM, Gupta T, Pelletier-Baldelli A, Dean DJ, Watts AKS, et al. Sleep dysfunction and thalamic abnormalities in adolescents at ultra-high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. (2013) 151:148–53. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Stephan KE, Friston KJ, Frith CD. Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring. Schizophr Bull. (2009) 35:509–27. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Arnfred SM, Chen ACN. Exploration of somatosensory P50 gating in schizophrenia spectrum patients: reduced P50 amplitude correlates to social anhedonia. Psychiatry Res. (2004) 125:147–60. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2003.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Swerdlow NR. Integrative circuit models and their implications for the pathophysiologies and treatments of the schizophrenias. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2010) 4:555–83. doi: 10.1007/7854_2010_48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Woodward ND, Karbasforoushan H, Heckers S. Thalamocortical dysconnectivity in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2012) 169:1092–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Woodward ND, Heckers S. Mapping thalamocortical functional connectivity in chronic and early stages of psychotic disorders. Biol Psychiatry (2016) 79:1016–25. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.06.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Karlsgodt KH, Sun D, Jimenez AM, Lutkenhoff ES, Willhite R, van Erp TG, et al. Developmental disruptions in neural connectivity in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Dev Psychopathol. (2008) 20:1297–327. doi: 10.1017/S095457940800062X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Huang MX, Lee RR, Gaa KM, Song T, Harrington DL, Loh C, et al. Somatosensory system deficits in schizophrenia revealed by MEG during a median-nerve oddball task. Brain Topogr. (2010) 23:82–104. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0122-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Krukow P, Jonak K, Karakula-Juchnowicz H, Podkowinski A, Jonak K, Borys M, et al. Disturbed functional connectivity within the left prefrontal cortex and sensorimotor areas predicts impaired cognitive speed in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging (2018) 275:28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Reite M, Teale P, Rojas DC, Benkers TL, Carlson J. Anomalous somatosensory cortical localization in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2003) 160:2148–53. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.12.2148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Shaikh M, Dutt A, Broome MR, Vozmediano AG, Ranlund S, Diez A, et al. Sensory gating deficits in the attenuated psychosis syndrome. Schizophr Res. (2015) 161:277–82. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.12.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Onitsuka T, Oribe N, Nakamura I, Kanba S. Review of neurophysiological findings in patients with schizophrenia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2013) 67:461–70. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Valkonen-Korhonen M, Purhonen M, Tarkka IM, Sipila P, Partanen J, Karhu J, et al. Altered auditory processing in acutely psychotic never-medicated first-episode patients. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. (2003) 17:747–58. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00199-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, Griggs CB, Bonner-Jackson A, McCarley RW. Mismatch negativity in chronic schizophrenia and first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2002) 59:686–94. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.8.686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Cornblatt BA, Carrión RE. Deconstructing the psychosis risk syndrome: moving the field of prevention forward. JAMA Psychiatry (2015) 73:105–6. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2454.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Fusar-Poli P, Nelson B, Valmaggia L, Yung AR, McGuire PK. Comorbid depressive and anxiety disorders in 509 individuals with an at-risk mental state: impact on psychopathology and transition to psychosis. Schizophr Bull. (2014) 40:120–31. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Nenadic I, Dietzek M, Schönfeld N, Lorenz C, Gussew A, Reichenbach JR, et al. Brain structure in people at ultra-high risk of psychosis, patients with first-episode schizophrenia, and healthy controls: a VBM study. Schizophr Res. (2015) 161:169–76. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Solowij N, Michie PT. Cannabis and cognitive dysfunction: parallels with endophenotypes of schizophrenia? J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2007) 32:30–52.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

85. Rentzsch J, Buntebart E, Stadelmeier A, Gallinat J, Jockers-Scherübl MC. Differential effects of chronic cannabis use on preattentional cognitive functioning in abstinent schizophrenic patients and healthy subjects. Schizophr Res. (2011) 130:222–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Roser P, Pichler EM, Habermeyer B, Kawohl W, Juckel G. Impact of chronic cannabis use on auditory mismatch negativity generation in schizophrenia patients. Pharmacopsychiatry (2018). doi: 10.1055/a-0573-9866. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Yücel M, Bora E, Lubman DI, Solowij N, Brewer WJ, Cotton SM, et al. The impact of cannabis use on cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of existing findings and new data in a first-episode sample. Schizophr Bull. (2012) 38:316–30. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Potvin S, Joyal CC, Pelletier J, Stip E. Contradictory cognitive capacities among substance-abusing patients with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. (2008) 100:242–51. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Koenders L, Machielsen MWJ, van der Meer FJ, van Gasselt ACM, Meijer CJ, van den Brink W, et al. Brain volume in male patients with recent onset schizophrenia with and without cannabis use disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2015) 40:197–206. doi: 10.1503/jpn.140081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Winton-Brown T, Kumari V, Windler F, Moscoso A, Stone J, Kapur S, et al. Sensorimotor gating, cannabis use and the risk of psychosis. Schizophr Res. (2015) 164:21–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.02.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Quednow BB, Kuhn KU, Hoenig K, Maier W, Wagner M. Prepulse inhibition and habituation of acoustic startle response in male MDMA (“ecstasy”) users, cannabis users, and healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology (2004) 29:982–90. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Allison T, Hume AL, Wood CC, Goff WR. Developmental and aging changes in somatosensory, auditory and visual evoked potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1984) 58:14–24. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(84)90196-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Desmedt JE, Cheron G. Somatosensory evoked potentials to finger stimulation in healthy octogenarians and in young adults: wave forms, scalp topography and transit times of parietal and frontal components. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1980) 50:404–25. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90007-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Lüders H. The effects of aging on the wave form of the somatosensory cortical evoked potential. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. (1970) 29:450–60.

Google Scholar

95. Shagass C, Schwartz M. Age, personality, and somatosensory cerebral evoked responses. Science (1965) 148:1359–61. doi: 10.1126/science.148.3675.1359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Nakano S, Hashimoto I. The later part of high-frequency oscillations in human somatosensory evoked potentials is enhanced in aged subjects. Neurosci Lett. (1999) 276:83–6. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00811-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: somatosensory processing, SEP, thalamus, psychosis, at-risk, transition, cannabis use

Citation: Hagenmuller F, Heekeren K, Roser P, Haker H, Theodoridou A, Walitza S, Rössler W and Kawohl W (2019) Early Somatosensory Processing Over Time in Individuals at Risk to Develop Psychosis. Front. Psychiatry 10:47. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00047

Received: 11 June 2018; Accepted: 23 January 2019;
Published: 13 February 2019.

Edited by:

Błażej Misiak, Wroclaw Medical University, Poland

Reviewed by:

Łukasz Gawęda, University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf, Germany
Paweł Krukow, Medical University of Lublin, Poland

Copyright © 2019 Hagenmuller, Heekeren, Roser, Haker, Theodoridou, Walitza, Rössler and Kawohl. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wolfram Kawohl, wolfram.kawohl@pdag.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.