Next Article in Journal
Sensory and Physico-Psychological Metaphor Comprehension in Children with ASD: A Preliminary Study on the Outcomes of a Treatment
Next Article in Special Issue
Myoclonic Disorders
Previous Article in Journal
Cognitive Changes in the Spinocerebellar Ataxias Due to Expanded Polyglutamine Tracts: A Survey of the Literature
Previous Article in Special Issue
Hypermethylation of Synphilin-1, Alpha-Synuclein-Interacting Protein (SNCAIP) Gene in the Cerebral Cortex of Patients with Sporadic Parkinson’s Disease
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Structural and Functional Neuroimaging of Visual Hallucinations in Lewy Body Disease: A Systematic Literature Review

1
Department of Neuroscience, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, S10 2RX, UK
2
Department of Neurosciences, University of Padua, 35128 Padua, Italy
3
Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico (IRCCS) Fondazione Ospedale San Camillo, 30126 Venice, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 31 May 2017 / Revised: 27 June 2017 / Accepted: 9 July 2017 / Published: 15 July 2017
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Pathophysiology and Genetics of Movement Disorders)

Abstract

:
Patients with Lewy body disease (LBD) frequently experience visual hallucinations (VH), well-formed images perceived without the presence of real stimuli. The structural and functional brain mechanisms underlying VH in LBD are still unclear. The present review summarises the current literature on the neural correlates of VH in LBD, namely Parkinson’s disease (PD), and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB). Following a systematic literature search, 56 neuroimaging studies of VH in PD and DLB were critically reviewed and evaluated for quality assessment. The main structural neuroimaging results on VH in LBD revealed grey matter loss in frontal areas in patients with dementia, and parietal and occipito-temporal regions in PD without dementia. Parietal and temporal hypometabolism was also reported in hallucinating PD patients. Disrupted functional connectivity was detected especially in the default mode network and fronto-parietal regions. However, evidence on structural and functional connectivity is still limited and requires further investigation. The current literature is in line with integrative models of VH suggesting a role of attention and perception deficits in the development of VH. However, despite the close relationship between VH and cognitive impairment, its associations with brain structure and function have been explored only by a limited number of studies.

1. Introduction

The term Lewy body disease (LBD) refers to disorders characterised by the neural inclusion of pathologic α-synuclein aggregates called Lewy bodies [1,2]. Clinical manifestations of Lewy body pathology include dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), Parkinson’s disease (PD), and Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD). These diseases share some clinical characteristics, including motor symptoms, sleep disorders, cognitive impairment, and visual hallucinations (VH) [3]. VHs represent a common symptom experienced by patients with LBD, and they are among the core features of the DLB symptomatology [4]. Recurrent and complex VHs have been defined as repetitive and well-formed images, which are perceived without the presence of real stimuli [5]. The most commonly reported VHs consist of people, animals, and inanimate objects, which appear to have similar features between clinical conditions, especially DLB and PD with dementia [5,6,7]. Early false sensations of presence are also common, mainly in PD [7]. Patients with LBD may also experience illusions or misperceptions, which are defined as incorrect perceptions of real stimuli [5,8]. VHs seem to be more severe and complex in LBD patients with cognitive impairment, which often present with lack of insight about the unreal nature of their experience [7].
The presence of VHs is a strong predictor of Lewy body pathology at autopsy [9,10]. Lewy body pathology associated with VHs was shown to affect temporal lobe areas predominantly, mainly the amygdala [11,12,13]. The structural and functional brain correlates underlying this symptomatology are, however, still not well understood. Moreover, it is not clear whether different diseases within the LBD spectrum share common neural mechanisms associated with VH, or whether they differ between conditions. Neuroimaging techniques represent valuable tools, which may help in detecting in vivo biomarkers that specifically characterise patients with VHs in PD and DLB. This may help the achievement of a more accurate understanding of the biological vulnerabilities leading to VHs, and may help future prediction of patients who are likely to develop these disabling symptoms, leading to the possible implementation of preventive treatments.
The aim of the present review was to summarise the current literature on structural and functional brain abnormalities associated with VHs in LBD, namely PD and DLB. Specifically, findings from structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), functional MRI (fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET), and single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) studies were critically reviewed.

2. Materials and Methods

Articles were identified through a systematic literature search, which was carried out in January 2017 by using the PubMed and Web of Science databases with no time limit. The following key words were used: “visual hallucinations”, “visual hallucination”, “Lewy body", "dementia with Lewy bodies”, “Parkinson’s disease”, “magnetic resonance imaging”, “MRI”, “voxel-based morphometry” (VBM) and “VBM”, “fMRI”, "resting-state", “diffusion tensor imaging”, “DTI”, “positron emission tomography” and “PET”, and “single photon emission computed tomography” and “SPECT”. An additional manual search of references was also undertaken. Studies were excluded according to the following exclusion criteria: (1) pathologies other than DLB, PD, or PDD; (2) neuroimaging analysis not related to VH; (3) patients with medication-induced VH; (4) studies not using MRI, fMRI, DTI, PET, SPECT; (5) PET and studies not investigating glucose metabolism and regional cerebral blood flow; (6) MRI studies using visual rating; (7) magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging; (8) pharmacological studies; (9) case studies (except for fMRI during VH); (10) review and theoretical articles; (11) non-English articles; and (12) non-peer reviewed articles. The search strategy used followed the PRISMA guidelines [14]. The articles included were assessed for scientific suitability to the aim of the present review, by using a set of 14 criteria adapted from Welton et al. [15], (these criteria are listed in Supplementary Materials Table S1). Each article was rated from 0 to 14, assessing the scientific quality of its structural and functional neuroimaging analyses related to VH only.

3. Results

The initial search retrieved 646 titles, among which 387 were duplicate publications, which were excluded. Three studies were identified through manual search. A total number of 262 titles and abstracts were assessed, of which 88 full-text articles were retrieved and screened for eligibility. The final review included 56 studies investigating the structural and functional brain correlates of VH in LBD by using structural and functional MRI, DTI, PET, and SPECT. A flow chart describing the selection process of the studies included in the final review is shown in Figure 1 (adapted from Moher et al. [14]).
Studies investigating VH in LBD using more than one approach or imaging technique were included in more than one section of the review and the findings for each technique reported separately in the relevant section. There were eight studies combining different techniques, specifically structural and functional MRI [16,17]; structural MRI and PET [18,19]; structural MRI and DTI [20,21]; structural MRI, DTI, and fMRI [22]; and fMRI and arterial spin labelling (ASL)-MRI [23]. Moreover, in two structural MRI studies, different methods were used to investigate regional brain volumes [24,25].
Suitability assessment of the articles reviewed revealed that structural (mean = 7.92), and functional (mean = 7.06) neuroimaging analyses related to VH were of comparable quality. Overall, among the main limitations of the studies, we found lack of a priori hypotheses on VH (n = 27 studies), sample sizes <15 participants per group (n = 39 studies), and absence of correlational analyses with VH indices (n = 35 studies) and cognitive measures (n = 49 studies). Quality assessment of each study can be found in supplementary Table S2.

3.1. Structural Brain Imaging

The brain structural changes associated with VH in LBD, detected with MRI, were investigated by 24 studies. The findings are summarised according to the analytic approach used, namely VBM, other methods to investigate brain morphology, namely regional volumes, shape, and cortical thickness; and DTI. For each study, detailed demographic, clinical, and methodological information—and imaging results related to VH—are reported in Appendix A (Table A1).

3.1.1. Voxel-Based Morphometry

A total of 12 VBM studies were identified. Ten studies focused on regional volumetric brain differences between LBD patients with and without VH, including nine studies which used whole brain analyses [21,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31], and three voxel-based analyses restricted to predefined regions of interest (ROI) [28,32,33]. Four studies also included results on the association between grey matter loss and VH or other cognitive variables [27,32,33,34]. Moreover, one study included only the comparison between PD subgroups and controls [35], and another investigated progression of brain atrophy [34]. The VBM methodology implemented was largely consistent between studies. All used the Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) software for imaging analysis, and seven [21,25,26,27,28,31,35] used the Diffeomorphic Anatomical Registration using Exponentiated Lie algebra (DARTEL) algorithm to create a study-specific template. A threshold corrected for multiple comparisons was applied in four whole brain [24,25,30,34], and four ROI [28,32,33,35] studies. The results of those studies which used uncorrected thresholds [21,25,26,27,28,31] should be interpreted more cautiously, since analyses using an uncorrected threshold may generate a higher number of false positives. Clinical and demographic features, including severity of cognitive impairment, disease duration, age, and years of education varied between studies. Some of them reported no differences between patients with and without VH in global cognitive impairment [21,24,25,27,28,31,33], while others showed more severe cognitive decline in hallucinating patients [30,32,34,35]. Furthermore, some studies on PD reported more advanced disease stage (Hoehn and Yahr, H&Y, stage) in patients with VH than in those without [21,28,30,34].
Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. investigated the progression of brain atrophy in PD patients with and without VH [34]. Hallucinating patients presented more extensive grey matter loss over time, accompanied by faster cognitive decline. Progressive grey matter reduction from baseline to follow-up extended to parietal, temporal, frontal, thalamic, and limbic areas in patients with VH, whereas only small clusters in frontal and cerebellar regions showed reductions in those without. Cognitive impairment and disease severity at baseline, however, were greater in patients with VH than in those without [34]. Additionally, significant associations between grey matter loss and cognitive functions were detected in patients with VH, specifically in measures of learning (left hippocampus, r = 0.88), delayed recall (left prefrontal cortex, r = 0.95), semantic fluency (left thalamus, r = 0.95), and language comprehension (left amygdala, r = 0.89).
In a whole brain VBM study, Ramirez-Ruiz et al. found grey matter volumetric reductions in the left lingual gyrus, and bilateral superior parietal lobe in PD patients with VH when compared with those without (p < 0.05 corrected at the cluster level) [30]. In this study, PD patients were matched with healthy controls. In this study, no information about the age of the included cohorts was given, and in the comparison between hallucinating and non-hallucinating patients the authors do not appear to have controlled for age in their analysis. Furthermore, patients with VH had more severe impairments in global cognitive status (as measured by the Mini-Mental State Examination, MMSE [36]), more advanced PD stage (rated with the Hoehn and Yahr scale [37]), and more severe depression (measured by the Hamilton depression rating scale [38]) [30]. These variables were included as covariates in the VBM statistical analyses [30]. Consistently with these findings, other studies reported decreased grey matter volume in PD patients with VH, compared with those without, in occipito-temporal regions, namely in the lingual and fusiform gyri, bilaterally [27,31]. Goldman et al. reported positive associations between VH severity, and grey matter volume of the left parietal lobule, and cuneus, and right lingual gyrus (p < 0.01 uncorrected) [27]. In addition to these findings, grey matter reductions have been reported in widespread brain areas, which were mainly located in the inferior parietal lobes [21,27], cingulate cortex [27,31], frontal [25,31], temporal [25,31], and occipital [27,31] areas bilaterally, and in the right supramarginal gyrus [21,31]. Another whole brain VBM study reported differences between PD patients with and without mild VH (presence and passage hallucinations), both decreases, mainly in the right vermis and precuneus, and increases, mainly in the cerebellum and the left inferior frontal cortex were detected in patients with mild VH [29]. These VBM studies, however, used thresholds uncorrected for multiple comparisons [21,25,27,31]. Differently from the study above, Meppelink et al. did not detect a difference between non-demented PD patients with and without VH [24], which may have been due to the more conservative threshold of p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected for multiple comparisons being used in this study. These apparently contrasting findings could, therefore, be a simple reflection of the application of a less rigorous statistical thresholding approach by the studies reviewed above.
In addition to the whole brain studies described above, two VBM studies investigated volumetric brain differences between hallucinating and non-hallucinating PD patients by using an ROI approach [28,32]. Specifically, Janzen et al. investigated the grey matter volume of the pedunculopontine nucleus due to its cholinergic function, thought to be involved in the development of VH, and the thalamus as one of its projection areas [28]. The authors found reduced grey matter in the left and right pedunculopontine nucleus between non-demented PD patients with and without VH, but not in the thalamus by using a threshold corrected for multiple comparisons. Hallucinating patients had significantly longer disease duration (VH: 11.5 ± 5.2 years; no VH 3.1 ± 3.6 years), were at a more severe Hoehn and Yahr stage (VH: 2.5 ± 0.3; no VH: 2.1 ± 0.5) and were taking higher levodopa equivalent doses. Furthermore, when hallucinating PD patients with and without dementia were combined and compared with non-demented patients without VH, the reduction in volume extended to the thalamus bilaterally [28].
In another voxel-based ROI study, the analyses were restricted to the hippocampus, due to its link with dementia development [32]. This region was chosen since the presence of VH is thought to be a risk factor for dementia, and therefore VH patients might exhibit the same pattern of atrophy shown by patients with dementia. The authors reported no differences in the direct comparison of non-demented PD patients with and without VH. When the two PD groups were independently compared with healthy controls, only hallucinating patients showed reduced grey matter in the anterior hippocampus bilaterally (p < 0.05 corrected); hippocampal volume also correlated with the learning scores achieved on a verbal memory test [32]. Overall, however, the PD patients with VH had more severe cognitive impairment, as shown by their lower MMSE scores and poorer scores on a verbal memory test [32]. Negative findings were reported by another VBM study, which found no differences in the hippocampus in any PD group, when compared with controls [35]. These contrasting findings might be due to differences in clinical and demographic variables—such as cognitive performance, disease duration, and age—and methodological differences in the analyses (i.e., standard VBM vs. DARTEL). In the latter study, when PD patients with and without VH were compared separately with healthy controls, using an ROI approach with ROI in temporal and frontal areas, reduced grey matter was found in the left superior frontal gyrus in both PD groups, while reduction in grey matter in the left frontal operculum was detected only in hallucinating patients [35]. The two PD groups, however, differed from each other in some clinical variables—including global cognitive level (measured with the MMSE), motor symptoms (evaluated with the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale motor score subsection, UPDRS III [39]), and depression (assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory, BDI [40])—with the hallucinating group being more severe in all these measures [35].
Only two studies used VBM to investigate differences between hallucinating and non-hallucinating patients with DLB [26,33]. Despite the small sample size (six DLB with VH, six without VH), Sanchez-Castaneda et al. [33] found a reduction of grey matter volume in the right inferior frontal gyrus in hallucinating patients (p < 0.05 corrected) using a voxel-based ROI approach. In the same study, patients with PDD presented grey matter volumetric reductions in the left orbitofrontal cortex, which was no longer significant when controlling for age [33]. In the hallucinating DLB subgroup, VH severity was strongly associated with reduction in the volume of the right inferior frontal gyrus (r = 0.89) and left precuneus (r = 0.95), while no significant correlations were found in the PDD subgroup. Another VBM study in DLB identified volumetric grey matter reductions posteriorly, in the left cuneus, by using a whole brain approach [26]. These findings, however, should be taken with caution as the threshold used in this study was not corrected for multiple comparisons. Moreover, demographic and clinical comparisons between patients with and without VH were not reported, probably due to the exploratory nature of the analysis of VH (the main aim of the study was to compare DLB with Alzheimer’s disease, AD, patients and with controls) [26].

3.1.2. Other Structural MRI Studies

In addition to the VBM studies described above, other studies have investigated brain morphological features of predefined ROI, especially their overall volume [18,19,20,22,24,41,42], and shape [22] in LBD. Three studies examined group differences in cortical thickness using the Freesurfer software package (and adopting thresholds corrected for multiple comparisons) [16,17,43]. Seven studies investigated volumetric [20,22,24,25,42] or cortical thickness [16,17] differences between PD patients with and without VH. Only three studies focused on the comparison between DLB and AD patients, and reported an association with VH indices [18,19,41,43].
Medial temporal lobe (MTL) structures, especially the hippocampus, were investigated by five studies [18,19,22,41,42]. Two of them focused on PD, and argued for an involvement of the hippocampus in the formation of VH mainly based on its role in memory, and evidence suggesting the presence of a high burden of Lewy body pathology in this region [22,42]. Yao et al. [22] used a multimodal MRI approach to investigate hippocampal volume, shape, mean diffusivity (MD), and functional connectivity. The authors found no differences between groups (PD with and without VH, and controls) in hippocampal volume and shape (MD and functional connectivity results are described in subsequent sections). Another MRI study reported significant volumetric reduction in hippocampal substructures, namely CA2-3 and CA4-DG, in PD patients with VH compared with those without [42]. Differences in the hippocampus as a whole were reported, however, only when hallucinating patients were compared with healthy controls. The more severe cognitive impairment in patients with VH, however, might have affected the results [42]. The value of the findings of these studies is limited by the relatively small size of the samples included in both studies [22,42]. Three MRI studies investigated the association between VH indices in DLB and MTL [18], hippocampus [19], and hippocampal substructure volumes [41]. A negative correlation was reported between severity of VHs and volumetric measures in MTL (entorhinal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala) [18]. However, these three studies failed to report a priori hypotheses based on the involvement of MTL regions in the development of VH, probably because VH were not the primary objective of investigation.
Lee et al. [20] selected five ROI within the visual pathway to investigate differences between hallucinating and non-hallucinating PD patients in the optic chiasm area, lateral geniculate nucleus, and V1 volumes and white matter microstructure features in the optic nerve and optic radiation (the latter are described in Section 3.1.3). These regions were selected to examine the neural bases of VH in relation to their role in processing visual information. Volumetric reductions in VH patients were reported only in the lateral geniculate nucleus [20]. In addition to the whole brain analysis described in the previous section, Meppelink et al. focused on an ROI in the left fusiform gyrus, detecting no differences between PD with and without VH [24]. Finally, another structural MRI study carried out an ROI analysis by delineating the left and right substantia innominata boundaries (in addition to the VBM analysis reported above) and identified a smaller volume of this structure in hallucinating PD patients (46 PD with VH, 64 PD without VHs) [25]. Furthermore, the volume of this region correlated with scores on verbal memory, semantic fluency, and go/no-go tests [25].
Studies investigating cortical thickness did not detect any differences between PD patients with and without VH [16,17]. However, when hallucinating PD patients were compared with non-hallucinating patients at a less advanced disease stage (H&Y; PD with VH: 3.0 ± 0.5; no VH: 2.1 ± 0.4) and controls, reduced cortical thickness was reported in frontal and parietal regions [16]. In the latter study, the analysis was restricted to regions within the default mode network (DMN) [16]. On the other hand, Yao et al. [17] found no differences between PD subgroups and controls in the analysis of the whole cortical surface. Finally, Delli Pizzi et al. [43] found a significant association between the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) [44] hallucination score, and cortical thickness in right lateralised parietal regions, namely the precuneus, and superior parietal gyrus in DLB patients (p < 0.05 corrected).

3.1.3. Diffusion Tensor Imaging

Four DTI studies were found, two on PD [20,21], and two on DLB [45,46]. Three studies investigated predefined ROIs of grey or white matter [20,45,46], while only one used a whole brain approach, namely tract-based spatial statistics TBSS (using a threshold corrected for multiple comparisons) [21]. Most of the studies used the FMRIB software library (FLS) for DTI analysis [20,21,45]. One study did not include a sample of LBD patients without VHs, and hallucinating patients where compared to healthy controls and AD [45].
Lee et al. [20] reported disrupted white matter integrity in the right optic nerve, and in the left optic radiation by using an ROI approach. In another multimodal study (VBM analysis reviewed above), Lee et al. [21] performed voxelwise analysis of fractional anisotropy (FA) and MD by using TBSS. No differences were found between non-demented PD subgroups with and without VH, and a similar pattern of abnormalities was reported when independently compared with age-matched healthy subjects, specifically in fronto-temporo-parietal and brainstem regions [21]. In these two studies, PD with and without VH did not differ for age, disease duration, MMSE score, and motor symptoms [20,21], but in one of these studies hallucinating patients were at a more advanced disease stage than non-hallucinating patients [21]. From the articles, however, it could not be established whether some of the patients investigated in the latter two references were the same in both studies [20,21].
Although DTI is mainly used to investigate microstructural white matter abnormalities, two studies focused on grey matter [22,45]. Yao et al. [22] reported increased MD in the right hippocampus in hallucinating PD patients. Moreover, Delli Pizzi et al. [45] investigated grey matter MD differences between DLB patients with VH and healthy controls by using a tractography-based subdivision of the thalami. The authors found increased MD in thalamic sub-regions projecting to prefrontal, parieto-occipital cortex (bilaterally), amygdala (right lateralised), and motor cortex (left lateralised). Moreover, MD in the right thalamic sub-region projecting to parietal and occipital cortex was associated with severity of VHs [45]. Finally, among the studies that focused on DLB, Kantarci et al. [46] showed increased MD in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus in patients with VHs compared with those without. Demographic and clinical differences between these groups of patients were not reported, however, probably because the main purpose was to differentiate DLB and AD patients [46].

3.2. Functional Brain Imaging

A total of 37 studies undertook functional imaging focusing on VH in LBD, including studies using task-based and resting-state fMRI, PET, and SPECT.
Details regarding demographic, clinical, and methodological information, and imaging results related to VH are reported in Table A2 for each study.

3.2.1. Task-Based fMRI

Ten studies used fMRI to identify brain activation patterns in response to simple visual stimuli [23,47,48,49,50], perception recognition tasks [51,52,53], and two single cases during VH [54,55]. The majority of them were in PD [48,49,50,51,52,53,54], while only three included patients with DLB [23,47,55]. Two studies performed different analyses on the same sample of DLB patients who had performed a visual task in the scanner [23,47]. Methodology and fMRI paradigm differed between studies, which may partly account for some inconsistencies in the findings. Other differences between studies include the threshold used to report the results, age, cognitive impairment, and duration of the disease. One study described in this section included PD patients experiencing minor VHs, including sensation of passage, presence, or misperceptions [49].
Five studies examined blood-oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) signal in response to simple visual stimuli [23,47,48,49,50]. Specifically, they investigated the perception of moving stimuli [23,47,48], apparent motion [50], circular gratings [49], checkboards, objects [47], and stroboscopic stimulation [50]. Regions of both increased and decreased activation were found in hallucinating PD patients, compared with the non-hallucinating ones [48,49,50]. The most consistent finding was decreased activity in occipital and temporal regions [48,49,50], even though increases in occipital [49], and temporal [48] areas were also reported. One of these studies, however, had a very small sample size (three PD with VHs, three PD without VHs) [48]. In addition, reduced activity was found in the parietal and cingulate cortex [50]. On the other hand, increased activity was reported mainly in the frontal lobe [49,50]. Two studies focusing on DLB reported no correlation between BOLD signal and VH indices, but no comparison with patients without VHs was performed [23,47]. One of the latter studies reported a negative association between the NPI hallucination score and perfusion in V4, detected by using arterial spin labelling (ASL)-MRI [23].
Three studies on PD focused on perceptual recognition of complex visual stimuli [51,52,53]. In comparison with non-hallucinating patients, those with VH presented decreased activity in the right superior frontal gyrus (p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected) during perceptual recognition of faces [52], animals, and objects [51]. In addition to these regions, decreased activation was found in the right inferior frontal (face recognition) [52], left lingual, and bilateral fusiform gyri (animal/object recognition) [51]. In one of these studies [52], however, patients with VH had more severe cognitive impairment and behavioural performance (fMRI task) than those without, which might have partially affected the results. Shine et al. [53] identified dysfunctional connectivity in and between attention networks and the DMN during the bistable percept paradigm (BPP) in PD patients with VH [53]. During this task, patients were asked to discriminate between images containing only one perceptual interpretation (stable, e.g., a candlestick) and images containing more than one (bistable, e.g., two faces and a candlestick) [53]. In this study, PD patients were divided into two groups according to the percentage of misperceptions at the BPP. Patients performing above a previously established cut-off score [56] also presented clinically assessed VH, while those performing below did not [53].
Two fMRI studies recorded brain activity during the occurrence of visual hallucinations in single cases [54,55]. Both patients experienced complex VH, namely seeing animals [54,55] and people [54] in the MRI scanner. Howard et al. [55] scanned a DLB patient in the hallucination-free state (the patient was taking risperidone), and a second time whilst he was hallucinating (seven days after risperidone was stopped). They found decreased activation in V1 and V2 in response to photic stimulation while the patient was hallucinating compared with the hallucination-free scan [55]. On the other hand, Goetz et al. [54] performed an event-related design in order to compare hallucinating and non-hallucinating events in a patient with PD. While the patient was experiencing VH, decreased activity was reported mainly in occipito-temporal areas, but activity increased in the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex [54].

3.2.2. Resting-State fMRI

Seven studies [16,17,22,57,58,59,60] performed resting-state fMRI analysis, including five statistical comparisons between patients with and without VH in PD [16,17,22,59,60]. Only two studies included a sample of hallucinating DLB patients and adopted correlational analyses [57,58]. Heterogeneity in methodology was found between studies. Specifically, independent component analysis (ICA) [16,17], ROI, and seed-based analyses [16,22,60] of functional connectivity, amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) [16,60], and graph analysis [58] were used. Moreover, between-study differences were detected for age, disease duration, motor symptoms, and global cognitive impairment, even though patients with and without VH were usually well matched within single studies. In addition, overall sample sizes were relatively small.
Two resting-state fMRI studies investigated differences between PD patients with and without VH in the functional connectivity of the DMN [16,17]. In both studies, the DMN was identified by performing ICA. The methodology implemented to investigate group differences in functional connectivity, however, was different. Specifically, in Franciotti et al. [16] pairwise ROI centred on the DMN were compared between groups. On the other hand, Yao et al. more broadly investigated the differences in the spatial map of the DMN [17]. Both studies reported increased functional connectivity in hallucinating patients in comparison with the non-hallucinating ones, mainly in fronto-parietal regions. Specifically, Franciotti et al. [16] detected increased connectivity between the superior frontal sulcus bilaterally with ipsilateral and contralateral parietal regions, and also between contralateral parietal regions. Yao et al. found increased activity in the right superior middle frontal lobe and bilateral precuneus and posterior cingulate gyrus within the DMN (p < 0.05 corrected) [17]. Both PD patients with and without VH presented a pattern of decreased functional connectivity when independently compared to healthy controls [16,17]. Furthermore, results from another resting-state fMRI study [59] were consistent with an association between VH and disrupted activity of the DMN, and other attention networks. The authors performed regression analyses to investigate the association between misperceptions at the BPP (the paradigm described above in Section 3.2.1) [56] and resting-state networks connectivity. All patients performing below a predefined BPP cut-off were also clinically classified as hallucinating. BPP error scores predicted connectivity between the ventral attention network (VAN) and the dorsal attention network (DAN), and increased connectivity within the DMN and the VAN [59].
In addition to the functional connectivity analyses reviewed above, Franciotti et al. [16] also investigated the fractional ALFF on the DMN centred ROI. Compared with non-hallucinating patients, PD with VH presented higher spectral power in fronto-parietal areas bilaterally [16]. Yao et al. [60] performed spectral analysis on the same sample in a previous study [17], reviewed above in Section 3.1.2. They found increased ALFF in VH patients in areas located in the cerebellum, temporal, and parietal lobes. Decreased ALFF was reported in occipital regions, namely the lingual gyrus, and cuneus bilaterally. These latter results were used to perform seed-based functional connectivity analysis. Compared with controls, both PD groups showed decreased functional connectivity, but in VH patients it was increased when compared with non-hallucinating patients [60]. In a multimodal MRI study, Yao et al. [22] reported both increased and decreased functional connectivity of the hippocampus in patients with VH compared with those without, using a seed-based approach. Specifically, increased connectivity was found with fronto-parietal regions, while it was decreased with occipito-temporal areas [22]. When compared to controls, however, both PD subgroups presented decreased connectivity of the hippocampus, bilaterally [22]. These latter studies performed different analyses on the same cohort of patients and this needs to be taken into account when interpreting the results [17,22,60] .
Yao et al. [22] also performed correlational analyses between cognitive measures and the regions of differential connectivity between PD groups. Specifically, the functional connectivity of the right hippocampus with right occipital, and medial temporal areas was negatively associated with visuospatial memory performance, which was in turn associated with VH severity [22]. On the other hand, other studies found no association between measures of VH and functional connectivity within regions in the DMN in PD [17], and in the temporal network in DLB [57]. Peraza et al. [58] explored functional connectivity in DLB by using a graph theory approach. They found no significant correlation between the NPI hallucination score and integrated global network measures [58]. However, they found an association with local network measures of node degree (negative for the left postcentral gyrus and positive for the putamen), and nodal betweenness centrality (negative for the right intracalcarine cortex and positive for the fusiform cortex) [58]. In another study, Peraza et al. performed secondary analyses on VHs in DLB, which showed an association between the NPI hallucination score and the left fronto-parietal, and sensory-motor networks [57]. It is not clear whether for two of the latter studies a subsample of the patients was from the same cohort of patients or not [57,58].

3.2.3. Positron Emission Tomography

We identified 11 studies investigating regional cerebral glucose metabolism using PET. Among them, six focused on the differences between patients with and without VH, four in PD [61,62,63,64], and two in DLB [65,66]. Other studies examined associations between glucose metabolism and VH indices, such as severity and frequency [18,19,64,67,68]. Among the voxel-wise whole brain analyses, three out of five studies compared subgroups of patients [61,62,63], and one out of three used a correlation approach [67] and used thresholds corrected for multiple comparisons. Overall, sample sizes were relatively small.
PD patients with VH presented hypometabolism mainly in posterior regions, especially in the parietal and temporal lobes [61,62,64]. The bilateral precuneus and lingual gyrus were particularly affected [61,62]. Gasca-Salas et al. [62] also reported two smaller clusters in the right occipital lobe, while Boecker et al. [61] reported frontal hypometabolism in VH patients. In contrast, Nagano-Saito et al. [63] found frontal hypermetabolism, specifically in the left superior frontal gyrus. Discrepancies might be partially explained by differences in demographic and clinical features between studies, including age, disease duration, and global cognitive impairment. Moreover, in Boecker et al. [61], hallucinating patients were at a more advanced disease stage, and had more severe motor symptoms than non-hallucinating patients. However, UPDRS III scores were included as covariate of no interest in the statistical analysis [61]. Only two studies compared DLB subgroups and reported contrasting findings. In a whole brain analysis, Perneczky et al. [66] showed hypometabolism in right lateralised temporo-occipital and frontal regions. Although results from the latter study were not corrected for multiple comparisons, differences were only expected in regions found to be hypometabolic when compared with controls (occipital, temporo-parietal, and frontal areas) [66]. In contrast, Imamura et al. [65] reported increased regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate in temporal and parietal regions. In the latter study, however, patient groups significantly differed in MMSE scores (DLB with VH: 19.5 ± 3.9; no VH: 15.0 ± 3.0; AD: 19.7 ± 3.5) [65]. Moreover, different methods were used, namely whole brain voxel-wise comparisons [66] and ROI analyses [65]. In addition to these findings, a PET study divided DLB patients into two subgroups, based on the hypermetabolism of peri-motor areas, cerebellum, and basal ganglia. The group with more regions of hypermetabolism was associated with more frequent VHs [69].
Studies investigating correlations between glucose metabolism and VH indices mainly reported negative associations with posterior regions [18,19,64,67,68]. Specifically, occipital hypometabolism has been related to severity [67] and frequency [19,67] of VH. A negative correlation with the NPI hallucination score has also been found in parietal [68] and temporal regions [64]. Finally, Iizuka and Kameyama [18] found a negative association with the standardized uptake value ratio in the precuneus/cuneus (r = −0.62, p < 0.01), and a positive association with the cingulate island sign ratio on [18F]-Fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG)-PET (r = 0.44, p < 0.05).

3.2.4. Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography

Regional cerebral blood flow in LBD patients with VHs has been investigated by nine SPECT studies, four on PD [70,71,72], five on DLB [73,74,75,76], one on PDD and DLB combined [77], and a single case on PDD [78]. Six studies examined the differences between hallucinating and non-hallucinating patients, three whole brain analyses [70,71,73] and three ROI analyses [72,74,76]. Different tracers were used to investigate cerebral blood flow using SPECT, including N-isopropyl-p-[123I]iodoamphetamine, [99mTc]ethyl cysteinate dimer, and 99mTc-HMPAO. Three studies investigated areas of association between perfusion and VH indices [73,75,77] and one study performed a SPECT scan during VH [78]. Overall, hallucinating patients showed regions of reduced brain perfusion compared with non-hallucinating patients. Only two studies reported no differences between groups [72,74].
Occipital hypoperfusion has been reported in both hallucinating PD [70] and DLB [73,76] patients, even though these studies present some limitations. For example, one only reported demographic and clinical characteristics of DLB and AD, without differentiating between patients with (n = 26) and without (n = 4) VH [76]. Moreover, Heitz et al. performed whole brain analyses without correcting the results for multiple comparisons [73]. Other ROI SPECT studies found no differences in occipital perfusion [72,74]. In another whole brain SPECT study, Oishi et al. [71] compared PD patients with (n = 24) and without (n = 41) VH. The authors found reduced cerebral blood flow in the right fusiform gyrus, which remained significant when correcting for multiple comparisons. Other temporal and parietal regions were found to be different between groups by using an uncorrected threshold [71]. In another whole brain study, O’Brien et al. [77] investigated the relationship between changes in brain perfusion and hallucinations over one year in a combined group of patients with DLB and PDD. They found a negative association with left parietal regions, namely the posterior cingulate gyrus and the precuneus (p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected) [77]. Another SPECT study performed factor analysis in order to investigate associations between regional cerebral blood flow and psychotic symptoms in DLB [75]. The authors showed a relationship between parietal and occipital hypoperfusion, and the sense of presence and hallucinations of people, but not of animals, insects and objects [75]. Finally, Kataoka et al. [78] described a patient with PDD having VHs during a SPECT scan, showing increased regional cerebral blood flow in the temporal lobe bilaterally, and in the left inferior frontal gyrus.

4. Discussion

The aim of the present review was to provide an overview of the neuroimaging findings from studies, which have investigated the neural bases of VHs in LBD by critically reviewing the current literature in the field. What emerged is that LBD patients with VH are characterized by widespread structural and functional brain abnormalities in cortical, but also subcortical regions. Given the more limited evidence in DLB and PDD than PD without dementia, it is difficult to infer disease-specific mechanisms within the LBD spectrum. A summary of the most consistent neuroimaging findings associated with VHs in LBD patients is shown in Table 1.
Overall, the most consistent finding among structural MRI studies of VH in LBD is grey matter loss in frontal areas, mainly in patients with dementia [33], and parietal and occipito-temporal regions [30] in non-demented PD patients. The presence of frontal and parietal impairment is consistent with results reported by neuropsychological studies showing more severe deficits in executive functions and visual attention in hallucinating patients [79,80,81,82]. This is in line with multifactorial models of VHs, proposing a role of visual attention deficits and disrupted engagement of attention networks in the development of VHs [5,83].
Notably, cholinergic treatment has been shown to ameliorate VHs and cognitive functioning, especially attention [84], corroborating the hypothesis of an involvement of attention dysfunction in the development of VH. Collerton et al. [5] proposed the Perception and Attention Deficit model, and suggested that VH result from the combination of impaired top-down and bottom-up processes, specifically coexisting deficits in attention and visual perception. These deficits would be supported by impaired activity in the lateral frontal cortex, and the ventral visual stream, respectively [5]. Surprisingly, given the established deficits in visual perception in LBD patients with VH [34,85,86,87], evidence of structural grey matter and brain metabolism differences in the occipital lobe is limited. Occipito-temporal and parietal grey matter loss, and reduction of cerebral blood flow were present mainly in PD [30,71]. Occipital hypoperfusion detected by SPECT has been reported [70,73,76], even though negative findings were also reported [72,74]. Discrepancies were reported in resting state FDG-PET studies. The most consistent finding is parietal and temporal glucose hypometabolism in PD with VH, even though inconsistencies were shown in frontal areas. The findings of these studies were both decreased and increased metabolism in the same regions in DLB, which might reflect demographic, clinical, and methodological differences between studies. In summary, resting state functional studies point towards hypometabolism/reduced blood flow in occipito-temporal and parietal regions in LBD patients with VH. This dysfunction in visual association regions might play a role in the genesis of VHs in LBD. This finding is further supported by the demonstration of disrupted white matter integrity in hallucinating DLB patients in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus [46], a bundle of associative fibres that connects the occipital and temporal lobes, which has been related to visual memory and perception [88].
To date, only a few studies have investigated how resting-state networks are disrupted in VH, and these studies have focused mainly on PD. Overall, increased functional connectivity in the DMN has been shown in hallucinating patients compared with those without hallucinations, while reduction in functional connectivity was a consistent finding in both PD subgroups when compared with healthy controls. Therefore, dysfunctional increased connectivity might play a significant role in the genesis of VH, especially within the DMN and fronto-parietal regions [16,17]. A speculative interpretation can be put forward, suggesting that a dysfunctional compensatory mechanism, resulting in increased functional connectivity in hallucinating patients, may foster the emergence of these symptoms. Functional abnormalities in frontal, temporo-occipital, and occipital areas have been reported by task-based fMRI studies. The direction of such alterations in the BOLD signal activity is still unclear, however, which might be due to differences in the behavioural tasks and in the stimuli used in the different studies.
Taken together, the results of imaging studies in LBD patients with VH are scarce for DLB but more frequent for PD. There is a mismatch between a more prominent involvement of primary and association visual regions in brain metabolism and blood flow studies and a more prominent involvement of more frontal regions when studying GM volume or cortical thickness. None of these findings appears to be associated with a different burden of neuropathological changes. In fact, despite the association between Lewy body pathology and VH in medial temporal lobe areas [11,12,13], substantial structural alterations in these regions have not emerged from this review. Neuropathological findings have shown a negative association between Lewy body pathology and regional brain atrophy, specifically in the frontal lobe, but conflicting evidence has been reported for the amygdala [89,90] and no associations have been found with occipital lobe dysfunction. Neither the macrostructural alterations observed with MRI nor the functional PET/SPECT findings, therefore, appear directly informative of the different underlying cellular events and neuropathology [91]. We can, therefore, speculate that VH in LBD emerge only in the presence of a double hit—i.e., concomitant alterations of large functional and structural attentional networks—of which frontal lobe atrophy may be a surrogate marker, and dysfunction of visual information processing, of which occipital-temporal and parietal hypometabolism is the functional hallmark. Large attentional networks may be impaired by diffuse cortical deposition of synuclein, and even amyloid. The cause of reduction in metabolism in posterior brain regions—i.e., which crucial cortical or subcortical projections are deafferenting the occipital cortex—remains still unexplained.
Visual hallucinations in LBD have been consistently associated with cognitive impairment. Firstly, their prevalence was found to be significantly higher in patients with dementia, and cognitive decline has been shown to be a significant predictor of VH [92,93]. In addition, there is an increased risk of developing dementia in PD in patients with early hallucinations [94]. LBD patients with VH have more severe deficits in a number of cognitive domains, especially visual perception and visual attention in both DLB [79,86,87] and PD [34,81,95,96,97], executive functioning [80,82,98,99], and long-term memory [80,81,82] in PD. Therefore, an important future development of research in this field may be the study of the association between cognitive functions and brain regions and networks specifically altered in LBD patients with VHs.
A limitation of the current literature in the field is that there have been only a few studies investigating structural brain alterations related to VH in DLB. In particular, only two studies compared patients with and without VHs directly [33,46], and no whole brain VBM analysis to date focused on VH in DLB. The neuroanatomical correlates of this symptom were assessed more extensively in PD. Further research is, therefore, needed to clarify better how these structural changes are related to cognitive functioning and connectivity between brain regions. This may be achieved by integrating studies using different imaging techniques, specifically resting-state fMRI and DTI, for the simultaneous study of functional and structural connectivity respectively. Although some resting-state fMRI studies were conducted in PD, none is available in which DLB patients with and without VHs have been directly compared. Similarly, lack of DTI studies examining white matter integrity emerged from this literature review. Future studies may benefit from the investigation of functional and structural networks associated with those cognitive functions impaired in patients with VHs. To our knowledge, only a few studies have explored the relationship between cognitive functioning and brain abnormalities in hallucinating LBD patients [22,25,32,34]. Other studies performed correlational analysis using clinical variables, especially VH severity and frequency, which have been mainly associated with parietal regions by both structural [27,33,43] and functional studies [18,57,68,77], but other correlational analyses showed no relationship with any brain region [17,19,41,46,47,58]. Furthermore, most of the studies reviewed above used the NPI questionnaire, which is not specific to capture the whole spectrum of phenomenology of VHs in this disease. Therefore, a more accurate evaluation of this symptom might be beneficial in the investigation of the neural correlates of visual hallucination. For example, the North-East Visual Hallucinations Interview (NEVHI) is a semi-structured interview, which was designed specifically to assess VH in older people with cognitive impairment [100] and this instrument might be more accurate to fine grain VH in LBD.
Finally, the present review itself presents some limitations. Even though negative results were reported by some studies, publication bias cannot be completely ruled out. In addition, we tried to reduce selection bias by undertaking an extensive literature search in two different databases with no time limit. Despite this, the possibility of having missed suitable studies cannot be fully excluded. We reviewed neuroimaging studies which had analysed VH in LBD that met the inclusion criteria. However, not all the studies had VH as their primary focus of investigation (e.g., analysis on VH in studies assessing differences between different types of dementia), and several studies failed to report clearly stated a priori hypotheses on the mechanisms underlying VH. Moreover, studies performing whole brain analyses were included even when results were not corrected for multiple comparisons, which may increase the occurrence of false positives. These factors, together with the inclusion of small sample sizes and other methodological limitations (e.g., statistical analyses not including covariates of no interest) might contribute to lowering the overall quality of the records included.

5. Conclusions

VHs are severe and disabling symptoms frequently observed in patients with LBD. The present review provides an up to date summary of current knowledge about the neural bases of VH in LBD. Overall, the findings suggest the involvement of structural and functional alterations in several brain areas in frontal, parietal, and occipito-temporal cortex. The mechanisms underlying VH in LBD, especially in patients with dementia, and how these differ between conditions remain still unclear, however. Future research might benefit from a combined investigation of structural and functional connectivity, as well as its association with neuropsychological measures. This might aid the understanding of the pathophysiology underlying VH in LBD and its relationship with cognitive decline. Neuroimaging techniques might help in the detection of symptom-specific biomarkers, which might be used to assess efficacy of treatments in the future, and as targets for new interventions.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/2076-3425/7/7/84/s1. Table S1: Suitability assessment criteria focusing on structural and functional imaging analyses related to VH only. Table S2: Suitability assessment of the neuroimaging studies included. The present suitability assessment focused on structural and functional imaging analyses related to VH only (excluding single cases).

Author Contributions

S.P. and A.V. conceived and designed the literature search; S.P. searched, selected, and reviewed all the records; S.P. drafted the manuscript. A.V., A.C., and O.B. critically revised, reviewed, and contributed to the manuscript. A.V. finalized the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the paper.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Summary of structural neuroimaging studies of hallucinating patients with LBD. Only main results related to VH are reported.
Table A1. Summary of structural neuroimaging studies of hallucinating patients with LBD. Only main results related to VH are reported.
StudySampleAge 1Disease Duration (years) 1VHMMSE 1H&YNeuroimaging Methods—VHMain Results:
Neuroimaging and VH
Blanc et al. (2016) [26]28 pro-DLB67.5 (9.2)NA60.7% DLB patients27.6 (2.1)NAWhole brain VBM
Covariates: age, TIV, sex, group comparison
Grey matter volume
p ≤ 0.005 uncorrected
VH < NVH: L cuneus
27 pro-AD69.3 (7.8)26.9 (1.9)
33 HC72.4 (10.4)29.4 (0.9)
Delli Pizzi et al. (2016) [41]19 DLB VH76.37 (4.35)2.95 (0.91)NPI18.00 (4.83)NAROI MRI
ROIs: hippocampal substructures; covariates: age, ed., TIV; relationship with NPIhall
Grey matter volume
Correlation with NPIhall: NS
15 AD76.47 (7.17)3.00 (0.93)16.73 (6.31)
19 HC76.21 (4.49)-27.58 (0.69)
Delli Pizzi et al. (2014) [43]18 DLB VH75.4 (4.0) 22.9 (0.6) 2NPI19.1 (2.6) 3,4NACortical thickness
Covariates: MMSE, CAF, and UPDRS; correlation with NPIhall (entire cortex and ROI where DLB ≠ AD)
Cortical thickness
p < 0.05 corrected
DLB VH < HC: bilat. pericalcarine, LG, cuneus, precuneus, SPG
Correlation with NPIhall: R precuneus and SPG
15 AD75.6 (7.6)3.1 (0.6)18.3 (4.1)
14 HC75.5 (5.3)-28.1 (1.6)
Delli Pizzi et al. (2014) [45]15 DLB VH 76.3 (4.1) 22.9 (7) 2NPI17.7 (5.2) 4,5NADTI ROI
Tractography-based subdivision of the thalami; nuisance factors: MMSE, CAF, and UPDRS; correlation with NPIhall
Grey matter MD
DLB VH > HC: thalamic sub-regions project. to PFC and parieto-occipital cortex bilaterally; R thalamic region project. to amygdala; L thalamic region project. to motor cortex
Positive association with NPIhall: R thalamus to parietal (VH freq. and sev.) and occipital cortices (VH sev.)
15 AD76.1 (4.9)3.1 (6)16.7 (5.4)
13 HC76.0 (4.2)-28.3 (1.5)
Franciotti et al. (2015) [16]15 sPD VH70 (6) 211.3 (4.3) 6,7NPI and semi-structured interview24.3 (2.2) 23.0 (0.5) 7Cortical thickness
Analysis within DMN cortical regions; nuisance factor: age; group comparison
Resting-state fMRI 8
Cortical thicknesss
sPD VH vs. sPD NVH: NS
sPD VH < ePD: L SFS and IPL, bilat. MFG, LPC
sPD VH < HC: L SFS, bilat. MFG, LPC, IPL, PCC
sPD NVH < HC: R LPC, bilat. IPL, PCC
15 sPD NVH 68 (11)12.0 (4.5) 724.7 (1.8)3.0 (0.6) 7
15 ePD NVH 66 (9)3.3 (1.8)26.2 (1.8) 2.1 (0.4)
15 MSA67 (5)3.9 (1.8)25.2 (1.7)3.2 (0.8)
15 HC69 (6)-28.7 (0.7)-
Gama et al. (2014) [35]11 PD VH70.6 (9.1) 27.4 (6.1) 2Based on definition of VH19.3 (3.7) 9From stage I to stage IVROI VBM
ROIs (SVC): temporal and frontal regions; group comparisons
Grey matter volume
p < 0.05 corrected
VH < HC: L opercula and SFG
NVH < HC: L SFG
28 PD NVH65.7 (7.8)6.5 (5.0)25.6 (4.3)
10 HC68.1 (7.0)-25.6 (4.3)
Goldman et al. (2014) [27]25 PD VH74.8 (6.0) 213.1 (4.6) 2MDS-UPDRS23.9 (5.4) 23 (2-5) 2,10Whole brain VBM
Covariate: TIV; group comparison; association between regions where VH < NVH and VH sev.
Grey matter volume
p < 0.01 uncorrected
VH < NVH: bilat. occipital, occipito-temporal, and parietal regions
Correlation with VH sev.: parietal, occipital, and occipito-temporal regions
25 PD NVH75.4 (6.1)10.8 (4.4)25.1 (4.4)3 (2-5) 10
Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. (2010) [34]12 PD VH73.3 (5.9) 212.1 (5.7) 2 Structured interview; NPI26.9 (1.9) 4,9,113.1 (1.1) 9Whole brain VBM
Covariate: MMSE; comparison between baseline and follow-up in each group; correlations between atrophy progression and cognitive decline in PD VH
Grey matter loss form baseline to follow-up
p < 0.05 corrected
VH: bilat. parietal cortex, insula, STG, ITG, SFG, IFG, thalamus and limbic areas, L dorsal PCC
NVH: R frontal areas and cerebellum
Correlations with grey matter loss in PD VH: learning with L hippocampus, delayed recall with L prefrontal cortex, semantic fluency with L thalamus, language comprehension with L amygdala
14 PD VH 71.1 (5.7) 11.9 (4.3) 29.3 (1.6)2.3 (0.5)
12 HC70.7 (7.2)-29.5 (2.6)-
Ibarretxe-Bilbao et al. (2008) [32]16 PD VH73.5 (5.1) 212.9 (5.9) 2Structured interview26.0 (2.1) 4,93.2 (1.1)ROI VBM
ROI: hippocampus; group comparison; correlation analysis with cognitive measures
Grey matter density
p < 0.05 corrected
VH vs. NVH and NVH vs. HC: NS
VH < HC: bilat. anterior hippocampus
Correlation with grey matter loss in anterior hippocampus bilaterally: learning scores in patients with PD VH, but not PD NVH and PDD
19 PD NVH72.5 (5.8)10.9 (4.2)28.2 (1.7)2.5 (0.7)
9 PDD69.8 (9.5)13.1 (5.4)15.7 (5.4)3.8 (1.0) 12
56 HC73 (6.7)-28.7 (3.1)-
Iizuka and Kameyama (2016) [18]24 DLB73 (68, 79) 132.8 (1.8, 3.2) 13NPI23 (20.5, 24) 13NAROI MRI
ROI: medial temporal lobe, correlation analysis
PET 8
Negative correlation with NPIhall in DLB: MTL atrophy
24 AD74 (69, 81) 132.3 (1.6, 2.6) 1323 (21, 24.5) 13
Janzen et al. (2012) [28]13 PD VH66.0 (6.9) 211.5 (5.2)SCOPA-PC28.0 (1.7)2.5 (0.3) 16ROI VBM
ROI: thalamus and pedunculopontine nucleus; covariates: age, TIV; group comparisons
Whole brain VBM
Post-hoc analysis; group comparisons
Grey matter volume
ROI VBM
p < 0.05 corrected
PD VH < PD NVH: bilat. PPN
PD/PDD VH < PD NVH: bilat. PPN and thalamus
Whole brain VBM
p < 0.001 uncorrected
PD/PDD VH < PD NVH: PPN and thalamus
DLB/PDD VH < PD VH: R MFC
13 PDD VH67.7 (7.1)10.9 (5.5)21.2 (2.7) 12,143.5 (1.0) 12,14
16 PD NVH64.3 (8.0)3.1 (3.6)28.9 (1.6) 152.1 (0.5)
11 DLB62.6 (6.5)4.6 (4.5)24.5 (1.4)NA
Kantarci et al. (2010) [46]30 DLB71 (55, 85) 10NAVH definition; 64% of DLB27 (14, 34) 10,17NADTI ROI
ROI: cortical GM regions and white matter tracts; group comparisons
Mean diffusivity
VH > NVH: ILF
30 AD74 (48, 89) 1024 (5, 33) 10,17
60 HC73 (54, 86) 1036 (28, 38) 10,17
Kantarci et al. (2012) [19]21 DLB73 (60, 87) 10NAFreq. of VH: 4 point scale90% of DLB 22 (6, 29) 10NAROI MRI
Association between freq. of VH and hippocampal volumes in DLB
PET 8
Hippocampal volume
Association with VH freq.: NS
21 AD77 (58, 92) 1021 (6, 28) 10
42 HC74 (59, 87) 1029 (27, 30) 10
Lee et al. (2016) [20] 10 PD VH69.2 (5.2) 27.2 (3.7) 2Semi-structured interview27.7 (1.6) 22.2 (0.5) 2DTI ROI
ROI: optic nerve, optic radiation bilaterally; group comparisons
ROI MRI
ROIs: optic chiasm area; LGN volume; V1 volume; each volume divided by TIV; group comparison
White matter integrity
p < 0.016 corrected
VH < HC: FA: L optic nerve FA
NVH < HC: MD: bilat. optic nerve
VH > NVH and HC: MD: L optic radiation
VH > NVH: axial diffusivity: R optic nerve
VH > HC: RD: L optic radiation
Grey matter volume
VH < NVH: volume: L LGN
VH < HC: volume: bilat. LGN
14 PD NVH66.1 (6.1)7.3 (3.7)28.4 (1.4)2.1 (0.5)
15 HC68.5 (6.6)-NA-
Lee et al. (2017) [21]10 PD VH69.4 (5.3) 27.2 (3.7) 2Based on consensus criteria during clinical follow-up27.6 (1.8)2.2 (0.3) 12,14,16 Whole brain VBM
Covariates: age, gender, TIV; group comparisons;
TBSS DTI
Group comparisons; covariates: age, gender
Grey matter volume
p < 0.001 uncorrected
PD VH < PD NVH: R IPL, and SG
White matter
p < 0.05 corrected
PD VH vs. PD NVH: FA: NS
PD VH < HC: FA: fronto-temporo-parietal and brainstem regions bilaterally
PD NVH < HC: FA: fronto-temporal-parietal areas, midbrain and pons bilaterally
PD VH > HC: MD: L temporo-parietal, and lateral geniculate areas
21 PD NVH66.2 (6.8)7.0 (4.2)28.2 (1.4)1.8 (0.5)
17 PDD VH71.1 (5.0)6.2 (3.0)20.4 (3.7) 12,142.8 (0.8)
30 HC68.6 (6.0)-NA-
Meppelink et al. (2011) [24]11 PD VHNA 28.0 (4.7)NPINA 2NAWhole brain VBM
Group comparisons; covariates: total grey matter
ROI MRI
ROI: L FG
Grey matter volume
Whole brain VBM
p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected
VH vs. NVH: NS
VH < HC: L temporal areas, parietal, occipital, frontal regions bilaterally
NVH < HC: L occipital, temporal regions, frontal and parietal regions bilaterally
ROI: FG: NS
13 PD NVHNA7.9 (2.4)NA
14 HCNA-NA
Pagonabarraga et al. (2014) [29]17 PD mVH64.1 (9) 29.8 (7) 2MDS-UPDRS87.2 (13) 2, 181.9 (0.3) 2Whole brain VBM
Covariates: age, gender, global grey matter volume; group comparisons; cluster threshold of 207 voxels determined through 1000 Monte Carlo simulations
Grey matter volume
p < 0.001 uncorrected
mVH < NVH: R vermis, and precuneus
mVH > NVH: cerebellum, L IFC
29 PD NVH66.3 (8)7.3 (4)90.3 (13)1.9 (0.4)
16 HC66.8 (8)-91.6 (11)-
Pereira et al. (2013) [42]6 PD VH73.7 (5.4) 212.9 (5.7) 2NPI26.1 (2) 4,93.1 (1) 2ROI MRI
ROI: hippocampal subfields; correlations with cognitive measures
Grey matter volume
VH vs. NVH: NS
VH and NVH < HC: CA2-3, CA4-DG
VH < HC: subiculum, whole hippocampus
6 PD NVH73.8 (6.8)12.8 (6.3)27.9 (1.4)2.9 (0.9)
6 HC73.6 (6.7)-27.9 (2.1)-
Ramirez-Ruiz et al. (2007) [30]18 PD VHPD matched with HC12.6 (5.6) 2NPI and structured questionnaire27.0 (2.1) 4,93.2 (1.0) 9Whole brain VBM
Covariates: TIV, MMSE, Hamilton, and H&Y scores; group comparison
Grey matter volume
p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected
VH < NVH: L LG and bilat. SPL
20 PD NVH10.6 (4.3)29.1 (1.4)2.5 (0.7)
21 HC-29.4 (2.3)-
Sanchez-Castaneda et al. (2010) [33]6 DLB VH70.17 (12.4) 232.8 (17.7) 19NPI and Burnes Questionnaire17.5 (5) 22.6 (0.5) 2ROI VBM
ROI: frontal, occipital, parietal, and temporal areas; covariates: TIV, disease duration, duration of dementia; group comparison; correlation analysis
Grey matter volume
p < 0.05 corrected
DLB VH < DLB NVH: R IFG
PDD VH < PDD NVH: L OFC
Correlations with VH sev.
p < 0.05 corrected
DLB: R IFG (r = 0.89), L precuneus (r = 0.95)
PDD: NS
6 DLB NVH70.6 (7.1)30 (11.8) 1921.2 (8.1)3 (0.7)
8 PDD VH75.3 (4.9)40.5 (16.8) 19, 2021.5 (3.5)2.8 (0.6)
7 PDD NVH70.6 (7.1)66 (24.8) 1923.5 (4)2.6 (1)
Shin et al. (2012) [25]46 PD VH71.3 (5.9) 23.3 (3.0) 2NPI25.2 (3.0) 2NAWhole brain VBM
Group comparison; covariates: age, sex, PD duration, intracerebral volume, MMSE score
ROI MRI
Manually delineation of the ROI; group comparisons
Grey matter volume
VBM
VH < NVH
p < 0.001 uncorrected
R IFC, L temporal, and thalamic areas
VH < HC
p < 0.05 corrected
Parahippocampal area, insular cortex
ROI
VH < NVH: SI volume
Correlations with SI volume: verbal memory immediate, and delayed recall, semantic fluency, go/no-go tests
64 PD NVH70.7 (5.7)2.8 (3.0)25.7 (2.9)
Watanabe et al. (2013) [31]13 PD VH66.6 (5.5) 210.0 (5.2) 2UPDRS I27.9 (1.9) 22.9 (0.6) 2Whole brain VBM
Covariates: TIV, age, sex; group comparison
Grey matter volume
VH < NVH
p < 0.005 uncorrected
Bilat. PFC, PFC, L ventral cingulate cortex, primary visual cortex, PCC, occipito-temporal regions, R parietal, temporal, occipital regions
13 PD NVH63.6 (10.7)10.0 (4.0)29.0 (1.5)2.4 (0.8)
22 HC63.4 (6.1)-29.8 (0.4)-
Yao et al. (2016) [22]12 PD VH70 (64, 72.75) 2, 13 9.1 (3.5) 2PPRS28.5 (24, 29.75) 2,133.1 (0.7) 2ROI MRI
Volume, and shape of the hippocampus; covariates: age, MMSE, intracranial volume; group comparison
ROI DTI
MD of the hippocampus; voxel-based MD of the hippocampus; covariates: age, MMSE; group comparison
Resting-state fMRI 8
Hippocampal volume and shape: NS
VH > NVH and HC: MD: R hippocampus; voxel-based MD (p < 0.05 corrected): R posterior hippocampal regions
15 PD NVH66 (62, 72) 27.1 (5.1)29 (28, 30) 132.9 (0.7)
14 HC63 (62, 68.75) 2-29 (28, 29.25) 13-
Yao et al. (2014) [17]12 PD VH67.6 (7.4) 210.0 (3.5) 2PPRS27.6 (2.4) 23.2 (0.7) 2Cortical thickness
Covariates: age, MMSE score and levodopa-equivalent dosage; group comparison
Resting-state fMRI 8
Cortical thickness
p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected
No differences between groups
12 PD NVH63.4 (7.4)8.4 (5.1)28.5 (1.7)2.8 (0.9)
14 HC64.1 (4.0)-29.1 (0.7)-
1 Mean (SD); 2 no significant differences between groups; 3 no significant differences with AD; 4 VH ≠ HC; 5 no differences between DLB and AD; 6 no differences between sPD VH and sPD NVH; 7 sPD VH and NVH ≠ ePD; 8 functional neuroimaging is reported in Table A2; 9 VH ≠ NVH; 10 median (range); 11 MMSE: VH 6.5 point decline after 2 years follow-up (higher percentage of loss compared to NVH); 12 PDD ≠ PD NVH; 13 median (interquartile range); 14 PDD ≠ PD VH; 15 PD NVH ≠ DLB; 16 PD VH ≠ PD NVH; 17 Short test of Mental Status; 18 Parkinson’s disease-Cognitive Rating Scale; 19 months; 20 PDD VH ≠ DLB VH, DLB NVH, PDD VH. AD: Alzheimer’s disease; bilat.: bilateral; CAF: Clinician Assessment of Fluctuations; DLB: dementia with Lewy bodies; ed.: education; ePD: early PD; FA: fractional anisotropy ; FAB: Frontal Assessment Battery; FG: fusiform gyrus; freq.: frequency; H&Y: Hoehn and Yahr stage; HC: healthy controls; IFC: inferior frontal cortex; IFG: inferior frontal gyrus; IPL: inferior parietal lobule; ITG: inferior temporal gyrus; LG: lingual gyrus; LGN: lateral geniculate nucleus; LPC: lateral parietal cortex; MD: mean diffusivity; MDS: Movement Disorders Society; MF: middle frontal gyrus; MFC: middle frontal cortex; MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination; MRI: magnetic resonance imaging; MSA: multiple system atrophy; NA: not available; NPI: Neuropsychiatric Inventory; NPIhall: NPI hallucination score; NS: not significant; NVH: no VH; OFC: orbitofrontal cortex; PCC: posterior cingulate cortex; PD: Parkinson’s disease; PDD: Parkinson’s disease dementia; PFC: prefrontal cortex; PPN: pedunculopontine nucleus; PPRS: Parkinson psychosis rating scale; pro-AD: prodromal AD; pro-DLB: prodromal DLB; project.: projecting; RD: radial diffusivity; ROI: region of interest; SCOPA-PC: Scales for Outcome in Parkinson’s disease Psychiatric Complications; sev.:severity; SFG: superior frontal gyrus; SFS: superior frontal sulcus; SG: supramarginal gyrus; SI: Substantia innominate; sPD: severe PD; SPG: superior parietal gyrus; SPL: superior parietal lobe; STG: superior temporal gyrus; UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; VBM: voxel-based morphometry; VH: visual hallucinations.
Table A2. Summary of functional neuroimaging studies of hallucinating patients with LBD. Only main results related to VH are reported.
Table A2. Summary of functional neuroimaging studies of hallucinating patients with LBD. Only main results related to VH are reported.
StudySampleAge 1Disease Duration (years) 1VHMMSE 1H&YNeuroimaging Methods—VHMain Results:
Neuroimaging and VH
Boecker et al. (2007) [61]8 PD VH72.88 (6.60) 211.00 (6.46) 2NPI25.75 (1.67) 23.31 (0.59) 3 FDG-PET
Whole brain; covariate: UPDRS III; group comparison
Cerebral glucose metabolism
p < 0.05 corrected
VH < NVH: bilat. IPL, precuneus, L SG, MFG, MTG, parahippocampal gyrus, LG, R cingulate gyrus
13 PD NVH70.56 (6.96)8.05 (5.85)26.82 (1.54)2.68 (1.54)
Erskine et al. (2015) [47]17 DLB 81.5 (5.5)NANPI19.0 (5.1)NATask-based fMRI
fMRI protocol: see Taylor et al. (2012) [23]; ROI: LGN; correlation analysis with NPIhall
fMRI activations
Correlations between NPIhall and BOLD activation in the LGN: NS
15 AD82.5 (9.2)20.8 (4.4)
19 HC77.6 (7.1)29.0 (1.2)
Firbank et al. (2016) [67]30 DLB76.4 (6.0)39.3 (27.9) 7NPI21.7 (4.2)NAFDG-PET
Covariates: CAMCOG, UPDRS-III, disease duration; voxel-wise correlation with NPIhall
Cerebral glucose metabolism
p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected
VH sev. and freq. associated with occipital hypometabolism
Franciotti et al. (2015) [16]15 sPD VH70 (6) 211.3 (4.3) 4,5NPI and semi-structured interview24.3 (2.2) 23.0 (0.5) 5Resting-state fMRI
ICA (30 components), 9 ROIs centred on the DMN clusters; nuisance factor: age; fALFF of each ROI; group comparisons
Cortical thickness 6
Functional connectivity of ROIs centred on the DMN
sPD VH > sPD NVH: L SFS – L IPL; LSFS – L IPL; LSFS – R IPL; L SFS – PCC; R SFS – L IPL; R SFS; R IPL; R SFS – PCC; L LPC – R LPC; L IPL – R IPL; L IPL – PCC; R IPL – PCC
sPD VH > ePD NVH
L SFS and L IPL
fALFF
sPD VH > sPD: R SFS, MFG, LPC, IPL; L MFG, IPL
15 sPD NVH 68 (11)12.0 (4.5) 524.7 (1.8)3.0 (0.6) 5
15 ePD NVH 66 (9)3.3 (1.8)26.2 (1.8) 2.1 (0.4)
15 MSA67 (5)3.9 (1.8)25.2 (1.7)3.2 (0.8)
15 HC69 (6)-28.7 (0.7)-
Gasca-Salas et al. (2016) [62]9 PD VH70.7 (3.9) 214.7 (5.4) 2UPDRS I27 (1.7) 2NS differences FDG-PET
Whole brain; covariates: age, ed., group comparison
Cerebral glucose metabolism
p < 0.05 corrected
VH < NVH: R LG, ITG, precuneus, precentral gyrus, L postcentral gyrus, bilat. MOG
12 PD NVH70.8 (3.4)14.3 (6.3)25.9 (2.7)
19 HC70.1 (3.1)-29.1 (1.2)
Goetz et al. (2014) [54]1 PD VH664African tribesmen, chimpanzees, people in civil war uniform, catholic nuns Intact cognitive functionNAfMRI during VH
VH during scan: African tribesmen and chimpanzees; event-related design; comparison between hallucination and non-hallucination
p < 0.001 uncorrected
Increased activations during VH: bilat. ACC, PCC, insula, R medial frontal gyrus, postcentral gyrus, thalamus, brainstem
Decreased activations during VH: R LG, FG, IOG, cingulate, L MFG, R STG
Heitz et al. (2015) [73]36 DLB VH71.7 (10.2) 2NAAssessed by expert neurologists21.7 (5.6) 2NASPECT
[99mTc]ECD; covariates: age, type of tracer; voxel-based comparison; association between cerebral perfusion and sev. of VH
Cerebral perfusion
p < 0.001 uncorrected
VH < NVH: L ACC, orbitofrontal cortex, cuneus
Association with VH sev.: bilat. ACC, R parahippocampal gyrus, L orbitofrontal cortex, L cuneus
30 DLB NVH73.5 (6.9)23.3 (4.3)
Holroyd and Wooten (2006) [48] 3 PD VH 69.7 (8.7) 2 NABased on definition of VH31.7 (2.1) 2,8 NATask-based fMRI
Visual task: coloured geometric shapes moving in random directions; baseline: stationary crosshair; group comparison
fMRI activations
VH > NVH: bilat. cuneus, LG, FG, and MTG
VH < NVH: primary visual cortex
3 PD NVH66.0 (2.7)34.3 (2.1) 8
Howard et al. (1997) [55]1 DLB VH7418 7Markets, factories, busy roads, docks; no insight16NAfMRI during VH
VH during scan: pigeons, sparrows, pheasants; exposure to photic stimulation; comparison between hallucination, and hallucination-free state
Hallucination-free activations: V1 and V2 bilaterally
Activations during VH: limited activation in V1 and V2
Iizuka and Kameyama (2016) [18]24 DLB73 (68, 79) 112.8 (1.8, 3.2) 11NPI23 (20.5, 24) 11NAFDG-PET
Voxel-wise; ROI: CIS ratio; SUVR in PCC and precuneus + cuneus; correlations with NPIhall
Structural MRI 6
Positive correlation with NPIhall in DLB: CIS ratio
Negative correlation with NPIhall in DLB: SUVR in precuneus/cuneus
24 AD74 (69, 81) 112.3 (1.6, 2.6) 1123 (21, 24.5) 11
Imamura et al. (1999) [65]16 DLB VH72.5 (4.6) 223.1 (13.0) 2,7NPI19.5 (3.9)NAFDG-PET
66 ROIs; covariate: MMSE
Cerebral metabolic rate of glucose
VH > NVH: R posterior temporal and parietal areas
6 DLB NVH72.8 (5.3)28.0 (14.4) 715.0 (3.0)
16 AD73.2 (4.6)22.2 (13.8) 719.7 (3.5)
Kantarci et al. (2012) [19]21 DLB73 (60, 87) 9NAFreq. of VH: four-point scale
90% of DLB with VH
22 (6, 29) 9NAFDG-PET
Association between FDG uptake and freq. of VH
Structural MRI 6
Cerebral glucose metabolism
Negative association with VH freq.: occipital FDG uptake
21 AD77 (58, 92) 921 (6, 28) 9
42 HC74 (59, 87) 929 (27, 30) 9
Kataoka et al. (2008) [78]1 PDD VH72NAWell-formed objects, humans, animals; VH developed four years after the onset of parkinsonians symptoms27 10NASPECT during VH
[99mTc]ECD; VH during scan: spider without delusions
Increased regional cerebral blood flow: L STG, MTG, IFG, and apex of R temporal lobe
Lefebvre et al. (2016) [49]18 PD VH63.50 (5.94) 29.06 (4.11) 2NPI
Minor VH
28.00 (1.24) 22 (2, 2) 2,11Task-based fMRI
Visual detection task (threshold evaluation): circular gratings; whole brain analysis; covariates: HDRS, TMT-B/TMT-A, Stroop error score
Behavioural results
Visual detection threshold: NS
fMRI activations at visual threshold
p < 0.01 cluster-level corrected
VH > NVH: R cerebellum, occipital cortex, PFC
VH < NVH: L cingulate, temporal, occipital cortices, caudate nucleus
16 PD NVH62.69 (4.09)8.00 (5.74)28.88 (1.20)2 (2, 2) 2,11
17 HC62.76 (4.19)-28.47 (1.70)-
Lobotesis et al. (2001) [74]23 DLB79.4 (9)60.7 (32) 7Detailed psychiatric history
18 DLB VH
16.0 (6.1)NASPECT
99mTc-HMPAO; ROI: occipital hypoperfusion; group comparison
Regional cerebral blood flow
VH vs. NVH: NS
50 AD81.6 (7)83.1 (34)17.3 (5.5)
20 HC78.1 (5)-28.4 (1.5)
Matsui et al. (2006) [70]31 PD VH71.1 (8.0) 210.9 (5.1) 3Clinical evaluation and information from patients and caregivers25.7 (3.2) 23.2 (0.4) 2SPECT
[123I]IMP; group comparison; multivariate logistic regression analysis with disease duration and levodopa equivalent dose as explanatory variables
Brain perfusion
VH < NVH: bilat. IPL, ITG, precuneus gyrus, occipital cortex
Significant after correcting for disease duration and levodopa equivalent dose: bilat. precuneus, L IPL, R occipital cortex
39 PD NVH69.0 (7.7)6.7 (5.7)26.4 (2.8)3.0 (0.6)
Meppelink et al. (2009) [51] 9 PD VH61.2 (8.2) 28.1 (5.0) 2NPI and questionnaire based on VH characteristics in PD26.8 (1) 2NATask-based fMRI
Perceptual recognition task: animals, well-known objects, and meaningless objects gradually popping out; covariates: movement parameters
Behavioural results
VH vs. NVH (both PD groups were slower than HC): NS; images recognised: VH 76%; NVH 86%
fMRI activations
During pop-out: NS between groups
p < 0.001 cluster-level corrected
Before pop-out: VH < NVH: R SFG, L LG, bilat. FG
14 PD NVH64.6 (7.8)8.7 (4.7)27.4 (1.3)
13 HC58.5 (7.5)-27.9 (0.9)
Miyazawa et al. (2010) [69]22 DLB74.5 (6.9)NAReported by patients and relatives
10/ in group A, and 4/ in group B
NAFDG-PET
Two groups: (A) hypermetab. in peri-motor areas, cerebellum, and basal ganglia; (B) hypermetab. in none, one, or two regions; group comparison in VH freq.
Visual hallucinations more frequent in group A
Group A74.8 (6.44) 216.2 (6.95) 2
Group B76.6 (6.29)21.0 (5.67)
Nagahama et al. (2010) [75]100 DLB76.7 (6.7)NASemi-structured interview
Factor 3: hallucination of person and feeling of presence
Factor 4: hallucination of animals, insects and objects
21.0 (3.9)NADLB patients: Factor analysis, four factors of psychotic symptoms identified
SPECT
ROIs DLB ≠ HC; relationship between psychotic symptoms factors and regional cerebral blood flow; covariates: age, sex, MMSE, UPDRS-III, dysphoria
Regional cerebral blood flow
Areas of hypoperfusion associated to factor 3 compared to the others: bilat. angular gyrus, R SG, L ventral occipital gyrus
No areas of hypoperfusion associated to factor 4
21 HC77.2 (4.8)-
Nagano-Saito et al. (2004) [63]8 PD VH67.6 (6.2) 28.6 (5.0) 2NA28.3 (1.8) 23.6 (0.9) 2FDG-PET
Whole brain; ROI: dorsolateral PFC, primary visual cortex, occipital association cortex, primary motor cortex; group comparison
Relative regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose
p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected
VH > NVH: L SFG
ROI: dorsal PFC
11 PD NVH66.0 (7.5)5.1 (3.8)28.5 (1.7)3.2 (0.5)
13 HC66.2 (4.9)-29.1 (1.0)-
O’Brien et al. (2005) [77]15 DLB73.8 (7)2.8 (2.1)NPIhall decreased over one year16.5 (4)NASPECT
99mTc-HMPAO; changes after one year; multiple regression between change in perfusion and change in NPIhall
Change in perfusion
p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected
DLB/PDD: negative association between hallucination score and perfusion in L PCC and precuneus
14 PDD71.9 (6)2.9 (2.8)20.9 (4)
Oishi et al. (2005) [71]24 PD VH 69.5 (7.2) 2 11.1 (5.0) 2 Clinical evaluation and information from patients and caregivers 25.1 (3.7) 2 3.3 (0.5) 2 SPECT
[123I]IMP; voxel-by-voxel comparison; covariates: MMSE, duration of disease
Regional cerebral blood flow
p < 0.05 corrected
VH < NVH: R FG
41 PD NVH68.6 (7.3)9.1 (5.0)26.5 (3.1)3.0 (0.5)
Osaki et al. (2005) [72]20 PD60.0 (11.3)8.4 (4.2)Structured clinical assessment
9 PD with VH, 10 PDD with VH
26.4 (4.1)NASPECT
[123I]IMP; 23 ROIs: frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital areas, pons; group comparison
Regional cerebral blood flow
VH vs. NVH: NS
10 PDD61.0 (8.5)11.6 (4.1)17.4 (6.3)
Park et al. (2013) [64]7 PD VH 71.0 (4.7) 25.4 (3.5) 2NPI26.1 (1.7) 122 .0 (0.0) 2FDG-PET
Whole brain; group comparison; whole brain correlation with NPIhall
Cerebral glucose metabolism
p < 0.001 uncorrected
VH < NVH: bilat. middle and inferior temporal cortex, L LG, and L angular gyrus
CI VH < NVH: temporo-parieto-occipital cortices
Negative correlation with NPI hallucinations score: glucose metabolism in bilat. STG, L FG, L Heschl’s gyrus
8 PD CI VH 67.8 (6.8)6.8 (3.1)21.6 (5.1)2.2 (0.7)
13 NVH66.3 (5.0)5.1 (3.1)26.9 (1.4)1.5 (0.8)
Pasquier et al. (2002) [76]34 DLB73.8 (8)3.3 (2.5)NPI
26 DLB VH
17.1 (6.7)NASPECT
[99mTc]ECD; ROI: R and L occipital region; group comparison
Cerebral perfusion
VH < NVH: R occipital region
28 AD76.3 (7.3)3.4 (2.7)15.8 (6.5)
Peraza et al. (2014) [57]16 DLB76.2 (5.7)NANPI24.2 (3.75)NAResting-state fMRI
Covariates: age, sex, grey matter; regression of sev. and freq. of VH with seeded significant cluster from dual regression DLB < HC
Association with NPIhall: L fronto-parietal, and sensory-motor networks (uncorrected)
No association with temporal resting-state network
17 HC77.3 (4.7)29.1 (0.83)
Peraza et al. (2015) [58]18 DLB77.2 (6.18)NANPI23.6 (3.9)NAResting-state fMRI
Graph analysis; correlation between NPIhall and integrated networks measures
No association between integrated network measures and NPIhall
Correlation with NPIhall: node degree (L postcentral gyrus, putamen), nodal betweenness centrality (R intracalcarine cortex and FG)
19 AD74.7 (8.5)22.58 (2.9)
17 HC76.8 (5.7)29.1 (0.85)
Perneczky et al. (2008) [66]14 DLB VH69.86 (6.76) 25.85 (4.88) 2NPI19.57 (5.27) 2NAFDG-PET
Whole brain; covariate: MMSE, UPDRS III, group comparison
Relative cerebral metabolic rate of glucose
p < 0.001 uncorrected
VH < NVH: R temporo-occipital conjunction, and MFG
7 DLB NVH68.86 (3.02)5.71 (2.67)23.14 (2.55)
16 HC65 (8)-30 (0)
Ramirez-Ruiz et al. (2008) [52]10 PD VH73 (1.9) 211.1 (1.7) 2Presence of VHs at least seven times per week, NPI25.8 (0.6) 3,133.1 (0.4) 2Task-based fMRI
One back repetition detection task (face recognition); group comparison
Behavioural results
VH < NVH: number of correct responses
VH > NVH: false-positive recognition
fMRI activations
p < 0.05 cluster-level corrected
VH < NVH: R IFG (controlling for antipsychotic intake) and SFG
VH > NVH: R IFG
10 PD NVH72.5 (1.9)11 (1.5)29.4 (0.4)2.5 (0.2)
10 HC71.6 (1.6)-29.9 (0.5)-
Shine et al. (2015) [59]10 PD pBPP69.5 (8) 26.9 (4) 2MDS criteria for VH
Patients with high % of misperceptions on the BPP (PD pBPP, based on a cut score) also presented VH
26.0 (3) 2,14NAResting-state fMRI
ROI: DAN, DMN, VAN, visual network; multiple regression analysis between BPP error scale, strength of mental imagery, and strength of connectivity within and between each network
BPP error score and strength of mental imagery predicted increased connectivity within the VAN and DMN, and decreased connectivity between the DAN and VAN, and the VAN and visual network, and DAN and visual network.
BPP error score predicted increased connectivity within VAN and DMN, and impaired connectivity between the VAN and DAN.
Strength of mental imagery was related to degree of impaired connectivity between the VAN and visual network
9 PD nBPP67.1 (7)4.4 (3)27.6 (2) 14
10 HC63.5 (8)-28.5 (1) 14
Shine et al. (2015) [53]21 PD pBPP69.3 (6) 26.0 (3) 2UPDRS-II, SCOPA-PC
Patients with high % of misperceptions on the BPP (PD pBPP, based on a cut score) also presented VH
27.2 (2) 2,142.2 (1) 2Task-based fMRI
BPP paradigm during fMRI; ICA, networks of interest: DMN, DAN, VAN, VIS; activity during correct stable items; comparison in BOLD signal between misperceptions and correct stable images; functional coupling between networks; group comparisons
Activity during correctly identified stable items:
Increased activity in the VIS, VH vs. NVH: VIS, DMN, VAN: NS; decreased activity in DAN
Activity during misperceptions: decreased activity in the DAN
Misperceptions vs. correct stable images in VH: visual misperception increased activity in VAN, DMN
Functional coupling between networks during misperceptions in VH: increase in functional coupling between DMN and VIS; decreased in functional coupling between DAN and DMN, and VAN
14 PD nBPP66.3 (5)4.7 (4)28.6 (2) 142.1 (1)
Stebbins et al. (2004) [50]12 PD VH71.08 (6.39) 213.92 (4.89) 2Self-report, NPI, SAPS26.17 (2.25) 23 (2–4) 2,9Task-based fMRI
Stroboscopic task; kinematic task (apparent motion); whole brain and SVC; covariate: MMSE; group comparisons
fMRI activations
Whole brain, p < 0.001 uncorrected
Stroboscopic stimulation:
NVH > VH: L IPL, R cingulate gyrus
VH > NVH: R IFG, R caudate nucleus
Kinematic stimulation:
NVH > VH: R middle temporal/occipital lobe, R cingulate gyrus, bilat. SG, L IPL
VH > NVH: L SFG
SMV during apparent kinematic stimulation, p < 0.05 corrected
VH < NVH: area V5/MT
12 PD NVH73.25 (7.58)11.17 (3.90)27.96 (2.09)3 (2–4)
Taylor et al. (2012) [23]17 DLB81.2 (5.6)45.4 (32.2) 7,15NPI18.8 (5.1)NATask-based fMRI
Passively view of simple visual stimuli (checkboards, pictures of objects, moving dot fields); block design; whole brain analysis; ROIs: V5/MT, V1, V2, and V3 combined, ROI in the lateral occipital cortex; correlation between fMRI beta values in the ROIs and NPIhall
ASL-MRI
Perfusion; voxel-based analysis; ROI: same as fMRI analysis, precuneus and superior lateral occipital region; correlation between ASL perfusion and NPIhall
fMRI activations
No correlation with NPIhall
Perfusion
Negative association with NPIhall: V4 perfusion
19 HC77.6 (7.1)-29.0 (1.2)
Uchiyama et al. (2015) [68]11 PD VH68.3 (1.6) 26.2 (1.1) 2NPI27.6 (0.6) 23.0 (2, 4) 3FDG-PET
Covariates: age and sex; whole brain correlation analysis with NPIhall
Glucose cerebral metabolic rate
p < 0.001 uncorrected
Negative correlation with NPIhall: metabolism in the L IPL
42 PD NVH65.5 (1.0)6.3 (0.6)28.2 (0.3) 2.5 (1, 4)
24 HC68.0 (1.0)-28.6 (0.3)-
Yao et al. (2016) [22]12 PD VH70 (64, 72.75) 2, 119.1 (3.5) 2PPRS
VH duration: 2.4 (1.1) years
28.5 (24, 29.75) 2, 113.1 (0.7) 2Resting-state fMRI
Seed-based approach: hippocampus FC; covariates: age, MMSE; correlation between cognitive tests and mean FC scores in clusters where FC was different between VH and NVH (controlling for age and visual accuracy scores)
Structural MRI and DTI (ROI) 6
Functional connectivity of the hippocampus
p < 0.05 corrected
VH < NVH: R hippocampus: occipital, temporal regions; L hippocampus: temporal, occipital regions, and cerebellum
VH > NVH: R hippocampus: frontal and temporal regions; L hippocampus: frontal and parietal regions
Negative correlation between R hippocampal FC with R occipital gyrus, and medial temporal lobe and visuospatial memory performance
15 PD NVH66 (62, 72) 117.1 (5.1)29 (28, 30) 112.9 (0.7)
14 HC63 (62, 68.75) 11-29 (28, 29.25) 11-
Yao et al. (2015) [60]12 PD VH67.6 (7.4) 210.0 (3.5) 2PPRS
VH duration: 22.6 (17.3) months
27.6 (2.4) 23.2 (0.7) 2Resting-state fMRI
ALFF; FC seed-region based on ALFF analysis (occipital lobe)
ALFF
p < 0.05 corrected
VH < NVH: occipital lobe: LG, cuneus bilaterally
VH > NVH: R cerebellum posterior lobe, MTL, IPL/STL
Functional connectivity of occipital seed-region
VH > NVH: bilat. IFG, SFG, MFG, medial frontal gyrus and STL; R STG, caudate/thalamus, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex/ventral medial PFC
12 PD NVH63.4 (7.4)8.4 (5.1)28.5 (1.7)2.8 (0.9)
14 HC64.1 (4.0)-29.1 (0.7)-
Yao et al. (2014) [17]12 PD VH67.6 (7.4) 210.0 (3.5) 2PPRS
VH duration: 22.6 (17.3) months
27.6 (2.4) 23.2 (0.7) 2Resting-state fMRI
Covariates: age, MMSE score, and levodopa-equivalent dosage; ICA (40 components)
FC in the DMN; group comparison; correlation between VH sev. and FC in the clusters that differed between PD groups
Cortical thickness 6
Functional connectivity in the DMN
p < 0.05 corrected
VH > NVH: L and R precuneus/ PCC, R superior middle frontal lobe
No correlation with VH
12 PD NVH63.4 (7.4)8.4 (5.1)28.5 (1.7)2.8 (0.9)
14 HC64.1 (4.0)-29.1 (0.7)-
1 Mean (SD); 2 no significant differences between groups; 3 VH ≠ NVH; 4 no differences between sPD VH and sPD NVH; 5 sPD VH and NVH ≠ ePD; 6 structural neuroimaging is reported in Table A1; 7 months; 8 Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status; 9 median (range); 10 one year before the development of VH, while three years after was 23; 11 median (interquartile range); 12 differences between groups; 13 VH ≠ HC; 14 MoCA; 15 duration of dementia. ACC: anterior cingulate cortex; AD: Alzheimer’s disease; ALFF: amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation; ASL: arterial spin labelling; BOLD: blood-oxygenation level-dependent; BPP: bistable percept paradigm; CAMCOG: Cambridge cognitive examination; ChEI: cholinesterase inhibitor; CI: cognitive impairment; CIS: cingulate island sign; DAN: dorsal attention network; DLB: dementia with Lewy bodies; DMN: default mode network; DTI: diffusion tensor imaging; ed.: education; ePD: early PD; fALFF: fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation; FC: functional connectivity; FDG: [18F]-Fluorodeoxyglucose; freq.: frequency; FG: fusiform gyrus; fMRI: functional MRI; H&Y: Hoehn and Yahr stage; HDRS: Hamilton depression rating scale; hypermetab.: hypermetabolism; ICA: independent component analysis; IFG: inferior frontal gyrus; IOG: inferior occipital gyrus; IPL: inferior parietal lobule; ITG: inferior temporal gyrus; L: left; LG: lingual gyrus; LGN: lateral geniculate nucleus; LPC: lateral parietal cortex sPD; MFG: middle frontal gyrus; MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination; MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment; MOG: middle occipital gyrus; MRI: magnetic resonance imaging; MSA: multiple system atrophy; MTG: middle temporal gyrus; MTL: medial temporal lobe; NA: not available; nBPP; NPI: Neuropsychiatric Inventory; NPIhall: NPI hallucination score; NS: not significant; NVH: no VH; pBPP: positive bistable percept paradigm; PCC: posterior cingulate cortex; PD: Parkinson’s disease; PDD: Parkinson’s disease dementia; PET: positron emission tomography; PFC: prefrontal cortex; R: right; ROI: region of interest; sev.: severity; SFS: superior frontal sulcus; SG: supramarginal gyrus; STG: superior temporal gyrus; STL: superior temporal lobe; SUVR: standardized uptake value ratio; SVC: small volume correction; TMT: trial making test; UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; VAN: ventral attention network; VH: visual hallucinations; VIS: visual network.

References

  1. Dickson, D.W.; Braak, H.; Duda, J.E.; Duyckaerts, C.; Gasser, T.; Halliday, G.M.; Hardy, J.; Leverenz, J.B.; Del Tredici, K.; Wszolek, Z.K.; et al. Neuropathological assessment of parkinson’s disease: Refining the diagnostic criteria. Lancet Neurol. 2009, 8, 1150–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Lippa, C.F.; Duda, J.E.; Grossman, M.; Hurtig, H.I.; Aarsland, D.; Boeve, B.F.; Brooks, D.J.; Dickson, D.W.; Dubois, B.; Emre, M.; et al. Dlb and pdd boundary issues: Diagnosis, treatment, molecular pathology, and biomarkers. Neurology 2007, 68, 812–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Aarsland, D. Cognitive impairment in parkinson’s disease and dementia with lewy bodies. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2016, 22 (Suppl. 1), S144–S148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. McKeith, I.G.; Dickson, D.W.; Lowe, J.; Emre, M.; O’Brien, J.T.; Feldman, H.; Cummings, J.; Duda, J.E.; Lippa, C.; Perry, E.K.; et al. Diagnosis and management of dementia with lewy bodies: Third report of the dlb consortium. Neurology 2005, 65, 1863–1872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Collerton, D.; Perry, E.; McKeith, I. Why people see things that are not there: A novel perception and attention deficit model for recurrent complex visual hallucinations. Behav. Brain Sci. 2005, 28, 737–757; discussion 757–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Mosimann, U.P.; Rowan, E.N.; Partington, C.E.; Collerton, D.; Littlewood, E.; O’Brien, J.T.; Burn, D.J.; McKeith, I.G. Characteristics of visual hallucinations in parkinson disease dementia and dementia with lewy bodies. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2006, 14, 153–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Onofrj, M.; Taylor, J.P.; Monaco, D.; Franciotti, R.; Anzellotti, F.; Bonanni, L.; Onofrj, V.; Thomas, A. Visual hallucinations in pd and lewy body dementias: Old and new hypotheses. Behav. Neurol. 2013, 27, 479–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Diederich, N.J.; Fenelon, G.; Stebbins, G.; Goetz, C.G. Hallucinations in parkinson disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2009, 5, 331–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Tiraboschi, P.; Salmon, D.P.; Hansen, L.A.; Hofstetter, R.C.; Thal, L.J.; Corey-Bloom, J. What best differentiates lewy body from alzheimer’s disease in early-stage dementia? Brain 2006, 129, 729–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Williams, D.R.; Lees, A.J. Visual hallucinations in the diagnosis of idiopathic parkinson’s disease: A retrospective autopsy study. Lancet Neurol. 2005, 4, 605–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Harding, A.J.; Broe, G.A.; Halliday, G.M. Visual hallucinations in lewy body disease relate to lewy bodies in the temporal lobe. Brain 2002, 125, 391–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Harding, A.J.; Stimson, E.; Henderson, J.M.; Halliday, G.M. Clinical correlates of selective pathology in the amygdala of patients with parkinson’s disease. Brain 2002, 125, 2431–2445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Papapetropoulos, S.; McCorquodale, D.S.; Gonzalez, J.; Jean-Gilles, L.; Mash, D.C. Cortical and amygdalar lewy body burden in parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucinations. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2006, 12, 253–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The prisma statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Welton, T.; Kent, D.; Constantinescu, C.S.; Auer, D.P.; Dineen, R.A. Functionally relevant white matter degradation in multiple sclerosis: A tract-based spatial meta-analysis. Radiology 2015, 275, 89–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Franciotti, R.; Delli Pizzi, S.; Perfetti, B.; Tartaro, A.; Bonanni, L.; Thomas, A.; Weis, L.; Biundo, R.; Antonini, A.; Onofrj, M. Default mode network links to visual hallucinations: A comparison between parkinson’s disease and multiple system atrophy. Mov. Disord. 2015, 30, 1237–1247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Yao, N.; Shek-Kwan Chang, R.; Cheung, C.; Pang, S.; Lau, K.K.; Suckling, J.; Rowe, J.B.; Yu, K.; Ka-Fung Mak, H.; Chua, S.E.; et al. The default mode network is disrupted in parkinson’s disease with visual hallucinations. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2014, 35, 5658–5666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Iizuka, T.; Kameyama, M. Cingulate island sign on fdg-pet is associated with medial temporal lobe atrophy in dementia with lewy bodies. Ann. Nucl. Med. 2016, 30, 421–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Kantarci, K.; Lowe, V.J.; Boeve, B.F.; Weigand, S.D.; Senjem, M.L.; Przybelski, S.A.; Dickson, D.W.; Parisi, J.E.; Knopman, D.S.; Smith, G.E.; et al. Multimodality imaging characteristics of dementia with lewy bodies. Neurobiol. Aging 2012, 33, 2091–2105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Lee, J.Y.; Yoon, E.J.; Lee, W.W.; Kim, Y.K.; Jeon, B. Lateral geniculate atrophy in parkinson’s with visual hallucination: A trans-synaptic degeneration? Mov. Disord. 2016, 31, 547–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Lee, W.W.; Yoon, E.J.; Lee, J.Y.; Park, S.W.; Kim, Y.K. Visual hallucination and pattern of brain degeneration in parkinson’s disease. Neurodegener. Dis. 2017, 17, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Yao, N.; Cheung, C.; Pang, S.; Shek-kwan Chang, R.; Lau, K.K.; Suckling, J.; Yu, K.; Ka-Fung Mak, H.; Chua, S.E.; Ho, S.L.; et al. Multimodal mri of the hippocampus in parkinson’s disease with visual hallucinations. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016, 221, 287–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Taylor, J.P.; Firbank, M.J.; He, J.; Barnett, N.; Pearce, S.; Livingstone, A.; Vuong, Q.; McKeith, I.G.; O’Brien, J.T. Visual cortex in dementia with lewy bodies: Magnetic resonance imaging study. Br. J. Psychiatry 2012, 200, 491–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Meppelink, A.M.; de Jong, B.M.; Teune, L.K.; van Laar, T. Regional cortical grey matter loss in parkinson’s disease without dementia is independent from visual hallucinations. Mov. Disord. 2011, 26, 142–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Shin, S.; Lee, J.E.; Hong, J.Y.; Sunwoo, M.K.; Sohn, Y.H.; Lee, P.H. Neuroanatomical substrates of visual hallucinations in patients with non-demented parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2012, 83, 1155–1161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Blanc, F.; Colloby, S.J.; Cretin, B.; de Sousa, P.L.; Demuynck, C.; O’Brien, J.T.; Martin-Hunyadi, C.; McKeith, I.; Philippi, N.; Taylor, J.P. Grey matter atrophy in prodromal stage of dementia with lewy bodies and alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2016, 8, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Goldman, J.G.; Stebbins, G.T.; Dinh, V.; Bernard, B.; Merkitch, D.; de Toledo-Morrell, L.; Goetz, C.G. Visuoperceptive region atrophy independent of cognitive status in patients with parkinson’s disease with hallucinations. Brain 2014, 137, 849–859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Janzen, J.; van ’t Ent, D.; Lemstra, A.W.; Berendse, H.W.; Barkhof, F.; Foncke, E.M. The pedunculopontine nucleus is related to visual hallucinations in parkinson’s disease: Preliminary results of a voxel-based morphometry study. J. Neurol. 2012, 259, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Pagonabarraga, J.; Soriano-Mas, C.; Llebaria, G.; Lopez-Sola, M.; Pujol, J.; Kulisevsky, J. Neural correlates of minor hallucinations in non-demented patients with parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2014, 20, 290–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Ramirez-Ruiz, B.; Marti, M.J.; Tolosa, E.; Gimenez, M.; Bargallo, N.; Valldeoriola, F.; Junque, C. Cerebral atrophy in parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucinations. Eur. J. Neurol. 2007, 14, 750–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Watanabe, H.; Senda, J.; Kato, S.; Ito, M.; Atsuta, N.; Hara, K.; Tsuboi, T.; Katsuno, M.; Nakamura, T.; Hirayama, M.; et al. Cortical and subcortical brain atrophy in parkinson’s disease with visual hallucination. Mov. Disord. 2013, 28, 1732–1736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N.; Ramirez-Ruiz, B.; Tolosa, E.; Marti, M.J.; Valldeoriola, F.; Bargallo, N.; Junque, C. Hippocampal head atrophy predominance in parkinson’s disease with hallucinations and with dementia. J. Neurol. 2008, 255, 1324–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Sanchez-Castaneda, C.; Rene, R.; Ramirez-Ruiz, B.; Campdelacreu, J.; Gascon, J.; Falcon, C.; Calopa, M.; Jauma, S.; Juncadella, M.; Junque, C. Frontal and associative visual areas related to visual hallucinations in dementia with lewy bodies and parkinson’s disease with dementia. Mov. Disord. 2010, 25, 615–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N.; Ramirez-Ruiz, B.; Junque, C.; Marti, M.J.; Valldeoriola, F.; Bargallo, N.; Juanes, S.; Tolosa, E. Differential progression of brain atrophy in parkinson’s disease with and without visual hallucinations. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2010, 81, 650–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Gama, R.L.; Bruin, V.M.; Tavora, D.G.; Duran, F.L.; Bittencourt, L.; Tufik, S. Structural brain abnormalities in patients with parkinson’s disease with visual hallucinations: A comparative voxel-based analysis. Brain Cogn. 2014, 87, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Hoehn, M.M.; Yahr, M.D. Parkinsonism: Onset, progression, and mortality. Neurology 2001, 57, S11–S26. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  38. Hamilton, M. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 1960, 23, 56–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Fahn, S.; Elton, R.L. Members of the unified parkinson’s disease rating scale development committee: Unified parkinson’s disease rating scale. In Recent Developments in Parkinson’s Disease; Fahn, S., Marsden, C.D., Calne, D.B., Goldstein, M., Eds.; McMillan Healthcare Information: New York, NY, USA, 1987; pp. 153–164. [Google Scholar]
  40. Beck, A.T.; Steer, R.A. Internal consistencies of the original and revised beck depression inventory. J. Clin. Psychol. 1984, 40, 1365–1367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Delli Pizzi, S.; Franciotti, R.; Bubbico, G.; Thomas, A.; Onofrj, M.; Bonanni, L. Atrophy of hippocampal subfields and adjacent extrahippocampal structures in dementia with lewy bodies and alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2016, 40, 103–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Pereira, J.B.; Junque, C.; Bartres-Faz, D.; Ramirez-Ruiz, B.; Marti, M.J.; Tolosa, E. Regional vulnerability of hippocampal subfields and memory deficits in parkinson’s disease. Hippocampus 2013, 23, 720–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Delli Pizzi, S.; Franciotti, R.; Tartaro, A.; Caulo, M.; Thomas, A.; Onofrj, M.; Bonanni, L. Structural alteration of the dorsal visual network in dlb patients with visual hallucinations: A cortical thickness mri study. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e86624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Cummings, J.L.; Mega, M.; Gray, K.; Rosenberg-Thompson, S.; Carusi, D.A.; Gornbein, J. The neuropsychiatric inventory: Comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 1994, 44, 2308–2314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Delli Pizzi, S.; Maruotti, V.; Taylor, J.P.; Franciotti, R.; Caulo, M.; Tartaro, A.; Thomas, A.; Onofrj, M.; Bonanni, L. Relevance of subcortical visual pathways disruption to visual symptoms in dementia with lewy bodies. Cortex 2014, 59, 12–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Kantarci, K.; Avula, R.; Senjem, M.L.; Samikoglu, A.R.; Zhang, B.; Weigand, S.D.; Przybelski, S.A.; Edmonson, H.A.; Vemuri, P.; Knopman, D.S.; et al. Dementia with lewy bodies and alzheimer disease: Neurodegenerative patterns characterized by dti. Neurology 2010, 74, 1814–1821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Erskine, D.; Taylor, J.P.; Firbank, M.J.; Patterson, L.; Onofrj, M.; O’Brien, J.T.; McKeith, I.G.; Attems, J.; Thomas, A.J.; Morris, C.M.; et al. Changes to the lateral geniculate nucleus in alzheimer’s disease but not dementia with lewy bodies. Neuropathol. Appl. Neurobiol. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Holroyd, S.; Wooten, G.F. Preliminary fmri evidence of visual system dysfunction in parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucinations. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2006, 18, 402–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Lefebvre, S.; Baille, G.; Jardri, R.; Plomhause, L.; Szaffarczyk, S.; Defebvre, L.; Thomas, P.; Delmaire, C.; Pins, D.; Dujardin, K. Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in parkinson’s disease. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 36284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Stebbins, G.T.; Goetz, C.G.; Carrillo, M.C.; Bangen, K.J.; Turner, D.A.; Glover, G.H.; Gabrieli, J.D. Altered cortical visual processing in pd with hallucinations: An fmri study. Neurology 2004, 63, 1409–1416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Meppelink, A.M.; de Jong, B.M.; Renken, R.; Leenders, K.L.; Cornelissen, F.W.; van Laar, T. Impaired visual processing preceding image recognition in parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucinations. Brain 2009, 132, 2980–2993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Ramirez-Ruiz, B.; Marti, M.J.; Tolosa, E.; Falcon, C.; Bargallo, N.; Valldeoriola, F.; Junque, C. Brain response to complex visual stimuli in parkinson’s patients with hallucinations: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Mov. Disord. 2008, 23, 2335–2343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Shine, J.M.; Muller, A.L.; O’Callaghan, C.; Hornberger, M.; Halliday, G.M.; Lewis, S.J.G. Abnormal connectivity between the default mode and the visual system underlies the manifestation of visual hallucinations in parkinson’s disease: A task-based fmri study. npj Parkinson’s Dis. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Goetz, C.G.; Vaughan, C.L.; Goldman, J.G.; Stebbins, G.T. I finally see what you see: Parkinson’s disease visual hallucinations captured with functional neuroimaging. Mov. Disord. 2014, 29, 115–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Howard, R.; David, A.; Woodruff, P.; Mellers, I.; Wright, J.; Brammer, M.; Bullmore, E.; Williams, S. Seeing visual hallucinations with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 1997, 8, 73–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Shine, J.M.; Halliday, G.H.; Carlos, M.; Naismith, S.L.; Lewis, S.J. Investigating visual misperceptions in parkinson’s disease: A novel behavioral paradigm. Mov. Disord. 2012, 27, 500–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Peraza, L.R.; Kaiser, M.; Firbank, M.; Graziadio, S.; Bonanni, L.; Onofrj, M.; Colloby, S.J.; Blamire, A.; O’Brien, J.; Taylor, J.P. Fmri resting state networks and their association with cognitive fluctuations in dementia with lewy bodies. Neuroimage Clin. 2014, 4, 558–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Peraza, L.R.; Taylor, J.P.; Kaiser, M. Divergent brain functional network alterations in dementia with lewy bodies and alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 2015, 36, 2458–2467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Shine, J.M.; Keogh, R.; O’Callaghan, C.; Muller, A.J.; Lewis, S.J.; Pearson, J. Imagine that: Elevated sensory strength of mental imagery in individuals with parkinson’s disease and visual hallucinations. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2015, 282, 20142047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Yao, N.; Pang, S.; Cheung, C.; Chang, R.S.; Lau, K.K.; Suckling, J.; Yu, K.; Mak, H.K.; McAlonan, G.; Ho, S.L.; et al. Resting activity in visual and corticostriatal pathways in parkinson’s disease with hallucinations. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2015, 21, 131–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Boecker, H.; Ceballos-Baumann, A.O.; Volk, D.; Conrad, B.; Forstl, H.; Haussermann, P. Metabolic alterations in patients with parkinson disease and visual hallucinations. Arch. Neurol. 2007, 64, 984–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Gasca-Salas, C.; Clavero, P.; Garcia-Garcia, D.; Obeso, J.A.; Rodriguez-Oroz, M.C. Significance of visual hallucinations and cerebral hypometabolism in the risk of dementia in parkinson’s disease patients with mild cognitive impairment. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016, 37, 968–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Nagano-Saito, A.; Washimi, Y.; Arahata, Y.; Iwai, K.; Kawatsu, S.; Ito, K.; Nakamura, A.; Abe, Y.; Yamada, T.; Kato, T.; et al. Visual hallucination in parkinson’s disease with fdg pet. Mov. Disord. 2004, 19, 801–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Park, H.K.; Kim, J.S.; Im, K.C.; Kim, M.J.; Lee, J.H.; Lee, M.C.; Kim, J.; Chung, S.J. Visual hallucinations and cognitive impairment in parkinson’s disease. Can. J. Neurol. Sci. 2013, 40, 657–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Imamura, T.; Ishii, K.; Hirono, N.; Hashimoto, M.; Tanimukai, S.; Kazuai, H.; Hanihara, T.; Sasaki, M.; Mori, E. Visual hallucinations and regional cerebral metabolism in dementia with lewy bodies (DLB). Neuroreport 1999, 10, 1903–1907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Perneczky, R.; Drzezga, A.; Boecker, H.; Forstl, H.; Kurz, A.; Haussermann, P. Cerebral metabolic dysfunction in patients with dementia with lewy bodies and visual hallucinations. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2008, 25, 531–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Firbank, M.J.; Lloyd, J.; O’Brien, J.T. The relationship between hallucinations and fdg-pet in dementia with lewy bodies. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016, 10, 636–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Uchiyama, M.; Nishio, Y.; Yokoi, K.; Hosokai, Y.; Takeda, A.; Mori, E. Pareidolia in parkinson’s disease without dementia: A positron emission tomography study. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2015, 21, 603–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Miyazawa, N.; Shinohara, T.; Nagasaka, T.; Hayashi, M. Hypermetabolism in patients with dementia with lewy bodies. Clin. Nucl. Med. 2010, 35, 490–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Matsui, H.; Nishinaka, K.; Oda, M.; Hara, N.; Komatsu, K.; Kubori, T.; Udaka, F. Hypoperfusion of the visual pathway in parkinsonian patients with visual hallucinations. Mov. Disord. 2006, 21, 2140–2144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Oishi, N.; Udaka, F.; Kameyama, M.; Sawamoto, N.; Hashikawa, K.; Fukuyama, H. Regional cerebral blood flow in parkinson disease with nonpsychotic visual hallucinations. Neurology 2005, 65, 1708–1715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Osaki, Y.; Morita, Y.; Fukumoto, M.; Akagi, N.; Yoshida, S.; Doi, Y. Three-dimensional stereotactic surface projection spect analysis in parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Mov. Disord. 2005, 20, 999–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Heitz, C.; Noblet, V.; Cretin, B.; Philippi, N.; Kremer, L.; Stackfleth, M.; Hubele, F.; Armspach, J.P.; Namer, I.; Blanc, F. Neural correlates of visual hallucinations in dementia with lewy bodies. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2015, 7, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Lobotesis, K.; Fenwick, J.D.; Phipps, A.; Ryman, A.; Swann, A.; Ballard, C.; McKeith, I.G.; O’Brien, J.T. Occipital hypoperfusion on spect in dementia with lewy bodies but not ad. Neurology 2001, 56, 643–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Nagahama, Y.; Okina, T.; Suzuki, N.; Matsuda, M. Neural correlates of psychotic symptoms in dementia with lewy bodies. Brain 2010, 133, 557–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Pasquier, J.; Michel, B.F.; Brenot-Rossi, I.; Hassan-Sebbag, N.; Sauvan, R.; Gastaut, J.L. Value of (99m)Tc-ecd spet for the diagnosis of dementia with lewy bodies. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging 2002, 29, 1342–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. O’Brien, J.T.; Firbank, M.J.; Mosimann, U.P.; Burn, D.J.; McKeith, I.G. Change in perfusion, hallucinations and fluctuations in consciousness in dementia with lewy bodies. Psychiatry Res. 2005, 139, 79–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Kataoka, H.; Furiya, Y.; Morikawa, M.; Ueno, S.; Inoue, M. Increased temporal blood flow associated with visual hallucinations in parkinson’s disease with dementia. Mov. Disord. 2008, 23, 464–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Cagnin, A.; Gnoato, F.; Jelcic, N.; Favaretto, S.; Zarantonello, G.; Ermani, M.; Dam, M. Clinical and cognitive correlates of visual hallucinations in dementia with lewy bodies. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2013, 84, 505–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Grossi, D.; Trojano, L.; Pellecchia, M.T.; Amboni, M.; Fragassi, N.A.; Barone, P. Frontal dysfunction contributes to the genesis of hallucinations in non-demented parkinsonian patients. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2005, 20, 668–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Hepp, D.H.; da Hora, C.C.; Koene, T.; Uitdehaag, B.M.; van den Heuvel, O.A.; Klein, M.; van de Berg, W.D.; Berendse, H.W.; Foncke, E.M. Cognitive correlates of visual hallucinations in non-demented parkinson’s disease patients. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2013, 19, 795–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Santangelo, G.; Trojano, L.; Vitale, C.; Ianniciello, M.; Amboni, M.; Grossi, D.; Barone, P. A neuropsychological longitudinal study in parkinson’s patients with and without hallucinations. Mov. Disord. 2007, 22, 2418–2425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Shine, J.M.; Halliday, G.M.; Naismith, S.L.; Lewis, S.J. Visual misperceptions and hallucinations in parkinson’s disease: Dysfunction of attentional control networks? Mov. Disord. 2011, 26, 2154–2159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Burghaus, L.; Eggers, C.; Timmermann, L.; Fink, G.R.; Diederich, N.J. Hallucinations in neurodegenerative diseases. CNS Neurosci. Ther. 2012, 18, 149–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Koerts, J.; Borg, M.A.; Meppelink, A.M.; Leenders, K.L.; van Beilen, M.; van Laar, T. Attentional and perceptual impairments in parkinson’s disease with visual hallucinations. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2010, 16, 270–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Mori, E.; Shimomura, T.; Fujimori, M.; Hirono, N.; Imamura, T.; Hashimoto, M.; Tanimukai, S.; Kazui, H.; Hanihara, T. Visuoperceptual impairment in dementia with lewy bodies. Arch. Neurol. 2000, 57, 489–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Mosimann, U.P.; Mather, G.; Wesnes, K.A.; O’Brien, J.T.; Burn, D.J.; McKeith, I.G. Visual perception in parkinson disease dementia and dementia with lewy bodies. Neurology 2004, 63, 2091–2096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Catani, M.; Thiebaut de Schotten, M. A diffusion tensor imaging tractography atlas for virtual in vivo dissections. Cortex 2008, 44, 1105–1132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Burton, E.J.; Mukaetova-Ladinska, E.B.; Perry, R.H.; Jaros, E.; Barber, R.; O’Brien, J.T. Neuropathological correlates of volumetric mri in autopsy-confirmed lewy body dementia. Neurobiol. Aging 2012, 33, 1228–1236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Cordato, N.J.; Halliday, G.M.; Harding, A.J.; Hely, M.A.; Morris, J.G. Regional brain atrophy in progressive supranuclear palsy and lewy body disease. Ann. Neurol. 2000, 47, 718–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Zatorre, R.J.; Fields, R.D.; Johansen-Berg, H. Plasticity in gray and white: Neuroimaging changes in brain structure during learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2012, 15, 528–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Fenelon, G.; Alves, G. Epidemiology of psychosis in parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 2010, 289, 12–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. Fenelon, G.; Mahieux, F.; Huon, R.; Ziegler, M. Hallucinations in parkinson’s disease: Prevalence, phenomenology and risk factors. Brain 2000, 123, 733–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Aarsland, D.; Andersen, K.; Larsen, J.P.; Lolk, A.; Kragh-Sorensen, P. Prevalence and characteristics of dementia in parkinson disease: An 8-year prospective study. Arch. Neurol. 2003, 60, 387–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  95. Barnes, J.; Boubert, L.; Harris, J.; Lee, A.; David, A.S. Reality monitoring and visual hallucinations in parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2003, 41, 565–574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Ramirez-Ruiz, B.; Junque, C.; Marti, M.J.; Valldeoriola, F.; Tolosa, E. Neuropsychological deficits in parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucinations. Mov. Disord. 2006, 21, 1483–1487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  97. Ramirez-Ruiz, B.; Junque, C.; Marti, M.J.; Valldeoriola, F.; Tolosa, E. Cognitive changes in parkinson’s disease patients with visual hallucinations. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord. 2007, 23, 281–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Barnes, J.; Boubert, L. Executive functions are impaired in patients with parkinson’s disease with visual hallucinations. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2008, 79, 190–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Ozer, F.; Meral, H.; Hanoglu, L.; Ozturk, O.; Aydemir, T.; Cetin, S.; Atmaca, B.; Tiras, R. Cognitive impairment patterns in parkinson’s disease with visual hallucinations. J. Clin. Neurosci. 2007, 14, 742–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Mosimann, U.P.; Collerton, D.; Dudley, R.; Meyer, T.D.; Graham, G.; Dean, J.L.; Bearn, D.; Killen, A.; Dickinson, L.; Clarke, M.P.; et al. A semi-structured interview to assess visual hallucinations in older people. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008, 23, 712–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow chart describing the selection process of the studies included (adapted from Moher et al. [14]). 1 Exclusion criteria: (1) pathologies other than dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), Parkinson’s disease (PD), or Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD); (2) neuroimaging analysis not related to visual hallucinations (VH); (3) patients with medication-induced VH; (4) studies not using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), functional MRI (fMRI), diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), positron emission tomography (PET), and single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT); (5) PET and SPECT studies not investigating glucose metabolism and regional cerebral blood flow; (6) MRI studies using visual rating; (7) magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging; (8) pharmacological studies; (9) case studies (except for fMRI during VH); (10) review and theoretical articles; (11) non-English articles; and (12) non-peer reviewed articles.
Figure 1. Flow chart describing the selection process of the studies included (adapted from Moher et al. [14]). 1 Exclusion criteria: (1) pathologies other than dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), Parkinson’s disease (PD), or Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD); (2) neuroimaging analysis not related to visual hallucinations (VH); (3) patients with medication-induced VH; (4) studies not using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), functional MRI (fMRI), diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), positron emission tomography (PET), and single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT); (5) PET and SPECT studies not investigating glucose metabolism and regional cerebral blood flow; (6) MRI studies using visual rating; (7) magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging; (8) pharmacological studies; (9) case studies (except for fMRI during VH); (10) review and theoretical articles; (11) non-English articles; and (12) non-peer reviewed articles.
Brainsci 07 00084 g001
Table 1. Summary of the most consistent findings associated with VHs in LBD.
Table 1. Summary of the most consistent findings associated with VHs in LBD.
Brain RegionsGM VolumeFunctional Connectivity Task-Related BOLD ActivationGlucose MetabolismBrain Perfusion
Frontal↑↓↓↑
Parietal ↓↑
Temporal ↓↑
Occipito- temporal ↓↑
Occipital ↓↑
BOLD: blood-oxygenation level-dependent; GM: grey matter; LBD: Lewy body disease; VH: visual hallucinations; ↓: decrease; ↑: increase.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pezzoli, S.; Cagnin, A.; Bandmann, O.; Venneri, A. Structural and Functional Neuroimaging of Visual Hallucinations in Lewy Body Disease: A Systematic Literature Review. Brain Sci. 2017, 7, 84. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci7070084

AMA Style

Pezzoli S, Cagnin A, Bandmann O, Venneri A. Structural and Functional Neuroimaging of Visual Hallucinations in Lewy Body Disease: A Systematic Literature Review. Brain Sciences. 2017; 7(7):84. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci7070084

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pezzoli, Stefania, Annachiara Cagnin, Oliver Bandmann, and Annalena Venneri. 2017. "Structural and Functional Neuroimaging of Visual Hallucinations in Lewy Body Disease: A Systematic Literature Review" Brain Sciences 7, no. 7: 84. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/brainsci7070084

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop