Next Article in Journal
Frequency Analysis of Failure Scenarios from Shale Gas Development
Next Article in Special Issue
Metabolic Syndrome in First Episode Schizophrenia, Based on the National Mental Health Registry of Schizophrenia (NMHR) in a General Hospital in Malaysia: A 10-Year Retrospective Cohort Study
Previous Article in Journal
Health Seeking Behavior among Rural Left-Behind Children: Evidence from Shaanxi and Gansu Provinces in China
Previous Article in Special Issue
Differentiating Medicated Patients Suffering from Major Depressive Disorder from Healthy Controls by Spot Urine Measurement of Monoamines and Steroid Hormones
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Electrocardiographic and Electrooculographic Responses to External Emotions and Their Transitions in Bipolar I and II Disorders

1
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychiatry/School of Public Health, Zhejiang University College of Medicine, Hangzhou 310058, China
2
Department of Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, Zhejiang University College of Science, Hangzhou 310058, China
3
Department of Psychiatry, Second Affiliated Hospital, Zhejiang University College of Medicine, Hangzhou 310058, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15(5), 884; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15050884
Submission received: 23 February 2018 / Revised: 11 April 2018 / Accepted: 27 April 2018 / Published: 28 April 2018
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Adult Psychiatry)

Abstract

:
Bipolar disorder has two main types, bipolar I (BD I) and II (BD II), which present different affective states and personality characteristics, they might present different modes of emotional regulation. We hypothesized that the electrocardiogram and electrooculogram to external emotions are different in BD I and BD II. We asked 69 BD I and 54 BD II patients, and 139 healthy volunteers to undergo these tests in response to disgust, erotica, fear, happiness, neutral, and sadness, and their transitions. Their affective states were also measured. The heart rate in BD I was significantly higher under background fear after target neutral. The eyeball movement was quicker in BD I under target happiness after background disgust; in BD I under target sadness after background disgust; and in BD I under background disgust after target neutral. Some electrocardiographic and electrooculographic changes were correlated with affective states in patients. BD I and BD II had different physiological responses to external emotions and their transitions, indicating different pathophysiologies and suggesting different emotional-therapies for BD I and BD II.

1. Introduction

Emotion plays a vital role in our lives, and inability to regulate or inhibit emotional responses efficiently contributes to various psychiatric disorders [1]. Bipolar disorder, which has two main types—bipolar I (BD I) and II (BD II)—is particularly characterized by emotional variations and difficulties in emotion regulation [2,3]. Besides the high risk of suicide, bipolar disorder also displays public health or public safety significance [4,5,6], for instance, with an increased risk of offending in every category of justice involvement [7]. In clinics, BD I presents mania and depression and has a higher prevalence of reckless activity, distractibility, and increased self-esteem [8], while BD II presents hypomania and depression and has higher episode frequency and comorbidity, more suicidal behavior, and rapid cycling [9]. Clinical managements of BD I and BD II are similar, but studies have shown that these patients responded differently to medication or other non-pharmacological treatments [10,11]. Investigators thus are looking for more evidence regarding the different pathophysiologies of BD I and BD II, especially those relating to emotional processing [12].
In healthy individuals, processing of an emotional stimulus includes an early perception, and a later response which includes emotional expressing and regulating [13]. The underlying mechanisms of these stages include attentional and cognitive resource mobilization and behavioral activation [14]. In different psychiatric disorders (e.g., BD I and BD II), the patterns of recognition and response to external emotion might be different. For instance, BD I had a lowered emotional recognition and tended to interpret sadness as fear [15]. BD I also showed a worsened recognition of fear, disgust [16,17], and happiness [17]. Neuroimaging techniques have shown some cerebral responsiveness when processing facial emotions in BD I and BD II patients, for instance, they displayed hippocampal over-activation to fear [18]. Compared to BD II, BD I showed more activation in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and accumbens, and lower functional connectivity between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and amygdala in response to fear [19].
Besides the above-mentioned studies which present no clear delineation between BD I and BD II, there might be some differences regarding the peripheral responses to an external emotion between them. The outcome of bipolar disorder can be shaped by environmental variables, such as psychosocial events or external emotions [20]. For instance, positive life events predict manic symptoms of BD I [21], while negative ones such as increasing arguments with the spouse, business failure, and serious illness of a family member are significantly associated with subsequent severity of both manic/hypomanic and depressive symptoms [22]. These affective variations might eventually turn into threats to patients themselves and the public [7]. Regarding physiological measures, specifically for automatic or implicit emotional response, one candidate parameter is the heart rate (the RR interval in electrocardiogram, ECG). A heart rate increase was observed in response to anger, fear, and sadness, and a decrease to disgust [23,24]. Some scholars also reported a temporary decrease of heart rate to negative emotional-stimulation, while an unchanged one to positive stimulation [25,26]. For the voluntary or explicit emotional response, eyeball movement as reflected in the electrooculogram (EOG) is another measurement candidate. Emotional responses are associated with eyeball movement, for example, exposure to pictures evoking aggression or erotica (erotism) was associated with reduced eye fixation [27].
On the other hand, the regulation of an emotion might be accomplished by voluntary effort such as exaggerating or suppressing a response of an individual [28], or by situation selecting such as choosing or avoiding specific environments [29]. In addition, emotional transitions were shown to lead to consistently different outcomes than their corresponding steady-state emotions during social interactions [30]. Characterization of peripheral physiological responses when an individual is emerged in an external emotion or during the transition from one external emotion to another would identify more differences between BD I and BD II. When referring to the inhibitory brainstem reflex, the duration of the second exteroceptive suppression of temporalis muscle activity was more shortened under sadness in BD II than that in BD I, and the latency of the second exteroceptive suppression under erotica was more prolonged in BD I, while more prolonged under happiness and sadness in BD II [31]. When referring to the excitatory brainstem reflex, blink reflex R2 components were more delayed under disgust, erotica, fear, and happiness in BD I, and were more decreased under disgust, erotica, fear, happiness, and sadness in BD II [32]. However, to date, there has been no report about the ECG/EOG responses to different external emotions and their transitions in BD I or BD II.
Based on the above-mentioned neurophysiological changes during the external emotions in BD I and BD II, we might speculate that the peripheral physiological-responses during the external emotions and their transitions from positive to negative ones or vice versa are different in these two types of bipolar disorder. BD I patients with manic and mixed phases reported an elevated arousal level when viewing all types of pictures [33], and BD I patients with manic phase showed a worsened overall recognition of facial emotion [16]. The search for the associations between physiological measures and concurrent affective states would help to clarify phaseological characteristics in these two types of bipolar disorder. We thus hypothesized that: (1) BD I more actively reacts to positive emotions, such as erotica, happiness, or their transitions to other emotions, with an accelerated heart rate and a quicker eyeball movement; and BD II more actively reacts to negative emotions, such as disgust, fear, sadness, or their transitions to other emotions, with an accelerated heart rate and a quicker eyeball movement; and (2) the ECG and EOG changes to external emotions and their transitions are correlated with their ongoing mania/hypomania or depression. Therefore, we have invited BD I and BD II patients and healthy volunteers to undergo ECG and EOG tests to the externally positive (erotica and happiness), negative (disgust, fear, and sadness), and neutral emotions and their transitions. Participants were also invited to answer questionnaires targeting their own ongoing affective states.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Altogether 267 participants were invited to attend the current study, five participants were excluded due to their inattention during the test or to their missing one or both physiological measures. Finally, there were 139 healthy volunteers (62 females and 77 males; mean age: 24.18 years with 7.78 S.D.; range: 17–59 years) who were recruited from the university, hospital staff or community; 69 outpatients with BD I (35 females and 34 males; mean age: 22.49 ± 5.92; range: 18–50), and 54 BD II (30 females and 24 males; mean age: 24.56 ± 5.93; range: 16–40) who were recruited from a psychiatric clinic. Patients were firstly interviewed by junior psychiatrists or clinicians and then diagnosed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth edition (DSM-5) [2] criteria by an experienced psychiatrist (WW). There was no significant difference when regarding either age (one-way ANOVA, main effect, F (2, 259) = 1.74, p = 0.18, mean square effect (MSE) = 84.38), or gender (Pearson’s chi-square with Yates correction, χ2 = 2.06, p = 0.36) among the three groups. All participants had to be medication-free for at least a month, were confirmed to have no other confounding factors including schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, nor prior history of head injury, alcohol or tobacco abuse, psychoactive substance abuse, central nervous system inflammation, nor other neurocognitive disorders through a semi-structured clinical interview. Two co-authors (G.M. and C.W.) were available to aid participants (including the hyperactive BD I patients) in the proper filling of the required demographic information, questionnaires and the informed consents, and to ensure corrective feedbacks. The study protocol was approved by a local ethics committee (no. ZGL201404-2-1) and all participants had given their written informed consents (the informed consents of the young adolescents were signed by their guardians).

2.2. Questionnaires

Participants were asked to complete the following three questionnaires in a quiet room.
  • The Mood Disorder Questionnaire (MDQ) [34], which consists of three parts, including 13 forced-choice (yes or no) questions to assess the presence of symptoms and behaviors related to mania or hypomania, one question to determine whether two or more symptoms have been experienced at the same time, and one question to determine the extent to which symptoms have caused functional impairment on a scale ranging from “no problems” to “serious problems”. Its internal reliability was 0.79 according to a recent study in a Chinese sample [35].
  • The Hypomania Checklist-32 (HCL-32) [36], which is a self-assessment instrument comprising 32 items for detecting hypomanic symptoms. Individuals were instructed to answer the forced-choice (yes or no) questions about emotions, thoughts, or behaviors, and to answer questions regarding the duration, the impact on family, social and work life, or people’s reactions. According to a recent study [37], its internal reliability was 0.88 in a Chinese sample.
  • The Plutchik-van Praag Depression Inventory (PVP) [38], which consists of 34 items. Each item has three scale points (0, 1, 2) corresponding with increasing depressive tendencies. Subjects have “possible depression” if they score between 20 and 25, or “depression” if they score above 25. According to a recent study [39], the internal reliability of the inventory was 0.94 in a Chinese sample.

2.3. External Emotional Stimuli

The emotional stimuli were six scenes of distinct emotions (or epochs) composed of high arousal and valence pictures and sounds of the same domain. Pictures were selected from the International Affective Picture System [40] and sounds from the International Affective Digitized Sounds [41]. The six external emotional epochs were namely disgust (picture code: 9301; sound code: 700), erotica (4611; 201), fear (3001; 275), happiness (2045; 110), neutral (7175; 152), and sadness (2799; 293). In each emotional epoch, a color picture was presented horizontally (768 × 512 pixels), sustaining about 19.8° × 13.5° of visual angles. Meanwhile via headphones, a sound of 6 s in duration and 40–50 dB in intensity, was repeatedly delivered and lasted for the same time duration as for the picture. Considering that hypersexuality is frequently reported in bipolar disorder [42,43,44], and happiness and sadness are prominent affections in this disorder [2], we chose erotica, happiness, neutral, and sadness as target emotions. Each target emotion was paired with the other remaining (five) emotions individually, resulting in a total of 20 emotional combinations. Within each combination, one silent white screen (blank) was presented first for 10 s, then one emotion (background) was presented for 6 s, then another emotion (Target) was presented for 8 s, then the previous background emotion was presented again for 10 s. For the sake of brevity, designations were used for each emotional epoch within different emotional combinations (Table 1).

2.4. Physiological Test Procedure

After 10 min rest, participants were led to a dimly lit room and seated 100 cm in front of a computer screen. They were equipped with a headphone and were attached with electrodes. Twenty emotional combinations were presented in a randomized order for each participant. Physiological responses measured during the five seconds before presentations of each emotional combination were treated as the baseline (blank) data. During blank epochs and during the presentation of each emotional combination, participants were instructed to keep their head and body motionless. After each presentation, participants were asked to report their perceived intensity of the target emotion on a visual analogue scale (VAS) which ranged from 0 (none) to 8 (most intense).

2.5. Recordings

The biosignals were recorded with a two-channel bio-amplifier (Model MP150, Biopac Inc., USA) which were connected with Ag-AgCl disposable electrodes. ECG was recorded using an ECG100C amplifier and three Ag-AgCl electrodes. Two electrodes were affixed on the right arm and the left leg, and ground electrode was placed on the right leg. ECG signals were amplified using the following hardware setting: Amplifier Gain: 2000; Mode: Normal; Notch: 50 Hz; and Band-pass: 0.5–100 Hz. EOG recordings were obtained using an EOG100C amplifier, by placement of two Ag-AgC1 electrodes 1.5 cm from the outer canthus of each eye. EOG signals were amplified using the following hardware setting: Amplifier Gain: 2000; Mode: Normal; Notch: 50 Hz; and Band-pass: 0.05–100 Hz. Analog data of both ECG and EOG were sampled at 1 kHz using an MP150 analog/digital converter and recorded online with AcqKnowledge (version 4.2.0, BIOPAC Systems, Inc., Goleta, CA, USA) software for Windows.

2.6. Physiological Data Reduction and Normalization

Off-line processing of the digitized physiological data was conducted with the AcqKnowledge software. The five seconds ECG during blank before background emotion, and the first five seconds ECG during each background or target emotion epoch (see Table 1) were identified, and their respective mean RR intervals (unit:s) were calculated. The five seconds EOG during blank before background emotion, and the first five seconds EOG during each background or target emotion epoch were identified, and their EOG derivatives (dp/dt, 1000 samples/s) were calculated. The absolute values of the derivative were used to globally represent the velocity of horizontal eyeball movement [45]. Figure 1 shows examples of the rectification and quantification of the ECG and EOG signals in a healthy volunteer under the stimulation sequence of blank-b1sadness-THappiness-b2sadness.
In order to minimize the individual differences among participants in terms of their VAS scores as well as physiological responses while experiencing a specific emotion, we normalized the data acquired through quantification to [0, 1] by
x’(t) = [x(t) − min]/(max − min)
where x’(t) is the normalized data, x(t) is the original data, min is the minimum of all the original data, and max is the maximum of all the original data [46]. Normalized data were administered into further analyses.

2.7. Statistical Analyses

Two-way ANOVA (group X emotion) was applied to the ECG, EOG, and VAS data in the three groups of participant. One-way ANOVA was applied to the scale scores of MDQ, HCL-32, and PVP in the three groups of participant. Whenever a significant main effect was found, post hoc analysis by the Bonferroni test was employed to evaluate between-group differences. A p-value less than 0.05 was considered as significant for these group comparisons. The relationships between ECG/EOG data and MDQ, HCL-32, and PVP scale scores were assessed by the Pearson correlation test. In order to reduce the risk of Type I error, we took a p-value less than 0.01 and |r| ≥ 0.35 as significant regarding correlations.

3. Results

3.1. Measures of Concurent Affective States

The mean PVP scores were significantly different among the three groups of participant (F (2, 259) = 50.83, p < 0.001, MSE = 5165.71), with that in BD II higher than those in BD I (p < 0.001, 95% confidence interval (CI = 3.55~12.38) and healthy controls (p < 0.001, 95% CI = 11.93~19.72). The mean PVP score in BD I was also higher than that in controls (p < 0.001, 95% CI = 4.28~11.43). The mean MDQ scores were significantly different among the three groups of participant (F (2, 259) = 300.10, p < 0.001, MSE = 1065.90), with that in BD I higher than those in BD II (p < 0.001, 95% CI = 5.23~6.88) and controls (p < 0.001, 95% CI = 5.95~7.28). The mean HCL-32 scores were also significantly different among the three groups of participant (F (2, 259) = 89.44, p < 0.001, MSE = 1622.74), with those in BD I (p < 0.001, 95% CI = 6.30~9.32) and BD II (p < 0.001, 95% CI = 4.12~7.41) higher than that in controls, and the score in BD I higher than that in BD II (p = 0.03, 95% CI = 0.19~3.92) (Table 2).

3.2. Perceived Intensity of Target Emotions (VAS)

Regarding VAS scores, a significant difference (F (2, 248) =3.31, p = 0.04, MSE = 0.34) was found for TNeutral after b1fear (Table 3). Specifically, BD I rated higher than BD II did on the target emotion (p = 0.04, 95% CI = 0.007~0.30). For the sake of brevity, detailed statistical results were omitted.

3.3. Electrocardiographic Changes

The mean RR intervals before normalization during blank were 0.79 s ± 0.10 S.D. for healthy controls, 0.80 ± 0.11 for BD I, and 0.79 ± 0.09 for BD II respectively; and they were not significantly different from each other (F (2, 259) = 0.53, p = 0.59, MSE = 0.006). With normalized data, we identified a significant group effect in b2fear after TNeutral (group effect, F (2, 259) = 4.68, p = 0.01, MSE = 0.32; emotion effect, F (3, 777) = 6.45, p < 0.001, MSE = 0.18; group X emotion interaction effect, F (6, 777) = 0.91, p = 0.48, MSE = 0.03), with the mean RR interval in controls (p = 0.008, 95% CI = 0.02~0.16) and BD II (p = 0.005, 95% CI = 0.03~0.20) longer than that in BD I (Table 4). No significant group effect was found regarding other emotional combinations. For the sake of brevity, the detailed statistical results were not reported.

3.4. Electrooculographic Changes

The mean eyeball movement before normalization during blank were 0.78 (mV/s) ± 0.32 S.D. for healthy controls, 0.87 ± 0.57 for BD I, and 0.83 ± 0.58 for BD II respectively, and they were not significantly different from each other either (F (2, 259) = 1.03, p = 0.36, MSE = 0.22). With normalized data, we identified a significant group effect in THappiness and TSadness after b1disgust (group effect, F (2, 259) = 4.44, p = 0.01, MSE = 0.44; emotion effect, F (3, 777) = 6.98, p < 0.001, MSE = 0.20; group X emotion interaction effect, F (6, 777) = 0.95, p = 0.46, MSE = 0.03), with the mean eyeball movement in BD I higher than that in controls (p = 0.045, 95% CI = 0.001~0.17) during THappiness, and the movement in BD I higher than that in BD II (p = 0.02, 95% CI = 0.01~0.19) during TSadness. Another significant group effect was identified for b2disgust after TNeutral (group effect, F (2, 259) = 3.56, p = 0.03, MSE = 0.31; emotion effect, F (3, 777) = 6.23, p < 0.001, MSE = 0.21; group X emotion interaction effect, F (6, 777) = 0.39, p = 0.89, MSE = 0.01), with the movement in BD I (p = 0.02, 95% CI = 0.01~0.17) higher than that in controls (Table 5). No significant group effect was found regarding other emotional combinations. Again, for the sake of brevity, detailed statistical results were not reported.

3.5. Relationships between Physiological Measures and Affective States

In BD I, RR interval during TSadness after b1erotica was negatively correlated with MDQ (n = 69, r = −0.35, p < 0.01). The eyeball movement during TSadness after b1neutral was positively correlated with MDQ (r = 0.36, p < 0.01). In BD II, RR interval during TNeutral after b1erotica was negatively correlated with HCL-32 (n = 54, r = −0.38, p < 0.01), and during b2neutral after TSadness was positively correlated with MDQ (r = 0.39, p < 0.01). The RR interval during b1sadness before THappiness was positively correlated with MDQ (r = 0.41, p < 0.01).

3.6. Summary of Differences between BD I and BD II

Compared to healthy controls, BD I and BD II were characterized differently in regard to the affective states, perceived intensity of the target emotion, and the electrocardiographic and electrooculographic changes (Table 6).

4. Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first trial of physiological responses to external emotions and their transitions in bipolar disorder. The RR interval in BD I was shorter (i.e., the heart rate was higher) than that in BD II during b2fear after TNeutral. The eyeball movement was quicker in BD I compared with controls during THappiness after b1disgust, and compared with BD II during TSadness after b1disgust. During b2disgust after TNeutral, BD I showed quicker eyeball movement than controls did. In BD I, the RR interval during TSadness after b1erotica was negatively correlated with MDQ. The eyeball movement during TSadness after b1neutral was positively correlated with MDQ. In BD II, the RR interval during TNeutral after b1erotica was negatively correlated with HCL-32, and that during b2neutral after TSadness was positively correlated with MDQ. The RR interval during b1sadness before THappiness was positively correlated with MDQ. Moreover, we found higher PVP in BD II than those in BD I and controls, higher MDQ in BD I than those in BD II and controls, and higher HCL-32 in both BD I and BD II than that in controls, which were consistent with previous reports [47,48,49]. The higher score of HCL-32 in BD I than that in BD II was consistent with what we have found previously [31,32,49], which might be due to the coexistence of mania and hypomania in bipolar disorder [50] and to the sufficiency of one mania episode to diagnose BD I [2].
Our first hypothesis was partly supported regarding exaggerated response to positive stimulations in BD I, but we failed to detect an increased response to negative emotions in BD II. Based on previous findings that patients with manic and mixed states considered neutral pictures pleasant [33,51,52], the accelerated heart rate in BD I compared to BD II in response to emotional transition from TNeutral to b2fear might imply that BD I patients were sympathetically more reactive when facing negative emotion fear than BD II patients did. Consistently, a higher subjective intensity of TNeutral after b1fear was found in our BD I than that in BD II.
During THappiness after b1disgust, BD I showed higher speed of eyeball movement than healthy controls did, which was in line with that the manic individuals exhibited more motoric activity in a novel environment [53]. The increased speed of eyeball movement in BD I compared with BD II during TSadness after b1disgust might be due to the depressive tendency in BD II, i.e., TSadness presented after b1disgust might initiate the depressive response, since depression is associated with diminished responses to the external emotional-stimuli [54,55]. The increased speed of eyeball movement during b2disgust after TNeutral in BD I, might again imply that BD I patients need more peace (TNeutral), and that they use more active strategies to cope with negative situations. These outcomes were consistent with that BD I relied more on active coping strategies, such as seeking professional help and stimulus reduction, while BD II relied more on negative ones, such as denying and blaming [56].
The correlation results of the current study support our second hypothesis regarding the effects of affective state on physiological parameters. In BD I, RR interval during TSadness after b1erotica was negatively correlated with MDQ, which was in line with previous reports that patients with manic and mixed states reported a higher arousal when viewing pictures [33]. It might be due to the hypersexual tendencies in BD I [43,44]. However, it remains unanswered why the correlation was specific to b1erotica. Also in BD I, the positive correlation between MDQ and eyeball movement during TSadness after b1neutral was supported by a previous study showing that the recognition of sad faces was negatively correlated with mania [16].
In BD II, the negative correlation between HCL-32 and RR interval during TNeutral after b1erotica and the positive correlation between MDQ and RR interval during b2neutral after TSadness might be connected with the attempts observed in BD II where individuals would elevate their mood by involving more sexual activities or other endeavors such as intake of food, alcohol, or drugs [57]. There was also a positive correlation between MDQ and RR interval during b1sadness before THappiness, which might be linked with a preparatory action, a phenomenon found in the movement-related cerebral potential study [58], for the anticipated THappiness. It might also imply a deficit in the processing of sadness in mania [16].
The present study suffers from several design limitations. Firstly, we used only two peripheral physiological parameters in response to the external emotions and their transitions, whether other physiological parameters were more sensitive to them remains to be seen. Secondly, each emotional combination was presented only once for one participant, repetitive presentations would help to build more reliable physiological responses. Thirdly, besides disgust, erotica, fear, happiness, neutral, and sadness, there are other emotions such as anger and surprise, which might exhibit other effects on these responses in BD I and BD II. Fourthly, although the external emotions we used were composed of pictures from the International Affective Picture System and of sounds from the International Affective Digital Sounds, the pictures were without scrambled image control [59], which might bias our results.

5. Conclusions

In the present study, we have demonstrated that BD I and BD II displayed different physiological responses to external emotions and their transitions, and some responses were affective state-associated. Future designs might be targeted at the cortical information processing of these external emotions and their transitions, and at the clinical management of emotion in bipolar disorder, which might eventually benefit the public safety.

Author Contributions

W.W. conceived the study; G.M., C.W., Y.J., J.W., B.Z., C.S., H.F., and B.P. collected the data; G.M. and C.W. analyzed the data; G.M. and W.W. drafted the paper.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the Natural Science Foundation of China (nos. 81571336 and 81771575) to Wei Wang.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. Regarding research work described in the paper, each one of our co-authors declares that there is no conflict of interest, and conformed to the Helsinki Declaration concerning human rights and informed consent, and followed correct procedures concerning treatment of humans in research.

References

  1. Leppänen, J.M. Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2006, 19, 34–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Publishing: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. Van Rheenen, T.E.; Murray, G.; Rossell, S.L. Emotion regulation in bipolar disorder: Profile and utility in predicting trait mania and depression propensity. Psychiatry Res. 2015, 225, 425–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Carta, M.; Angst, J. Screening for bipolar disorders: A public health issue. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 205, 139–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Conus, P.; Macneil, C.; McGorry, P.D. Public health significance of bipolar disorder: Implications for early intervention and prevention. Bipolar Disord. 2014, 16, 548–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Swanson, J.W.; McGinty, E.E.; Fazel, S.; Mays, V.M. Mental illness and reduction of gun violence and suicide: Bringing epidemiologic research to policy. Ann. Epidemiol. 2015, 25, 366–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Robertson, A.; Swanson, J.; Frisman, L.; Lin, H.; Swartz, M. Patterns of justice involvement among adults with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: Key risk factors. Psychiatr. Serv. 2014, 65, 931–938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Serretti, A.; Olgiati, P. Profiles of “manic” symptoms in bipolar I, bipolar II and major depressive disorders. J. Affect. Disord. 2005, 84, 159–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Vieta, E.; Suppes, T. Bipolar II disorder: arguments for and against a distinct diagnostic entity. Bipolar Disord. 2008, 10, 163–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Benazzi, F. Bipolar disorder-focus on bipolar II disorder and mixed depression. Lancet 2007, 369, 935–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Medda, P.; Perugi, G.; Zanello, S.; Ciuffa, M.; Cassano, G.B. Response to ECT in bipolar I, bipolar II and unipolar depression. J. Affect. Disord. 2009, 118, 55–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Schloesser, R.J.; Huang, J.; Klein, P.S.; Manji, H.K. Cellular plasticity cascades in the pathophysiology and treatment of bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008, 33, 110–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Gross, J.J. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 1998, 2, 271–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Wessa, M.; Linke, J. Emotional processing in bipolar disorder: behavioural and neuroimaging findings. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2009, 21, 357–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Derntl, B.; Seidel, E.; Kryspin-Exner, I.; Hasmann, A.; Dobmeier, M. Facial emotion recognition in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 2009, 48, 363–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Lembke, A.; Ketter, T.A. Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania. Am. J. Psychiatry 2002, 159, 302–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Hoertnagl, C.M.; Muehlbacher, M.; Biedermann, F.; Yalcin, N.; Baumgartner, S.; Schwitzer, G.; Deisenhammer, E.A.; Hausmann, A.; Kemmler, G.; Benecke, C.; et al. Facial emotion recognition and its relationship to subjective and functional outcomes in remitted patients with bipolar I disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2011, 13, 537–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Malhi, G.S.; Lagopoulos, J.; Sachdev, P.S.; Ivanovski, B.; Shnier, R.; Ketter, T. Is a lack of disgust something to fear? A functional magnetic resonance imaging facial emotion recognition study in euthymic bipolar disorder patients. Bipolar Disord. 2007, 9, 345–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Caseras, X.; Murphy, K.; Lawrence, N.S.; Fuentes-Claramonte, P.; Watts, J.; Jones, D.K.; Phillips, M.L. Emotion regulation deficits in euthymic bipolar I versus bipolar II disorder: A functional and diffusion-tensor imaging study. Bipolar Disord. 2015, 17, 461–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Johnson, S.L. Life events in bipolar disorder: Towards more specific models. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 25, 1008–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Johnson, S.L.; Sandrow, D.; Meyer, B.; Winters, R.; Miller, I.; Solomon, D.; Keitner, G. Increases in manic symptoms after life events involving goal attainment. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2000, 109, 721–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Koenders, M.A.; Giltay, E.J.; Spijker, A.T.; Hoencamp, E.; Spinhoven, P.; Elzinga, B.M. Stressful life events in bipolar I and II disorder: Cause or consequence of mood symptoms? J. Affect. Disord. 2014, 161, 55–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Ekman, P.; Levenson, R.W.; Friesen, W.V. Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science 1983, 221, 1208–1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Levenson, R.W.; Ekman, P.; Friesen, W.V. Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system activity. Psychophysiology 1990, 27, 363–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Hubert, W.; de Jong-Meyer, R. Psychophysiological response patterns to positive and negative film stimuli. Biol. Psychol. 1990, 31, 73–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hubert, W.; de Jong-Meyer, R. Autonomic, neuroendocrine, and subjective responses to emotion-inducing film stimuli. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1991, 11, 131–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Luborsky, L.; Blinder, B.; Mackworth, N. Eye fixation and recall of pictures as a function of GSR responsivity. Percept. Mot. Skills 1963, 16, 469–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Kim, S.H.; Hamann, S. Neural Correlates of Positive and Negative Emotion Regulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2007, 19, 776–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Hayward, R.M.; Tuckey, M.R. Emotional boundary management: a new adaptive approach to emotion regulation at work. Res. Occup. Stress Well Being 2013, 11, 35–74. [Google Scholar]
  30. Filipowicz, A.; Barsade, S.; Melwani, S. Understanding emotional transitions: the interpersonal consequences of changing emotions in negotiations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 101, 541–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Zhu, Q.; Wang, J.; Shen, C.; Fan, H.; Zhang, B.; Ma, G.; Lu, Y.; Wang, W. Inhibitory brainstem reflexes under external emotional-stimuli in bipolar I and II disorders. BMC Psychiatry 2017, 17, 224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Zhu, Q.; Wang, J.; Fan, H.; Ma, G.; Zhang, B.; Shen, C.; Wang, W. Blink reflex under external emotional-stimuli in bipolar I and II disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2018, 259, 520–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. M’Bailara, K.; Atzeni, T.; Colom, F.; Swendsen, J.; Gard, S.; Desage, A.; Henry, C. Emotional hyperreactivity as a core dimension of manic and mixed states. Psychiatry Res. 2012, 197, 227–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Hirschfeld, R.M.; Williams, J.B.; Spitzer, R.L.; Calabrese, J.R.; Flynn, L.; Keck, P.E., Jr.; Lydia Lewis, B.A.; McElroy, S.L.; Post, R.M.; Rapport, D.J.; et al. Development and validation of a screening instrument for bipolar spectrum disorder: the Mood Disorder Questionnaire. Am. J. Psychiatry 2000, 157, 1873–1875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Yang, H.C.; Yuan, C.M.; Liu, T.B.; Li, L.J.; Peng, H.J.; Rong, H.; Liao, C.P.; Shen, Q.J.; Fang, Y.R. Validity of the Chinese version Mood Disorder Questionnaire (MDQ) and the optimal cutoff screening bipolar disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2011, 189, 446–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Angst, J.; Adolfsson, R.; Benazzi, F.; Gamma, A.; Hantouche, E.; Meyer, T.D.; Skeppar, P.; Vieta, E.; Scott, J. The HCL-32: towards a self-assessment tool for hypomanic symptoms in outpatients. J. Affect. Disord. 2005, 88, 217–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Yang, H.C.; Yuan, C.M.; Liu, T.B.; Li, L.J.; Peng, H.J.; Liao, C.P.; Rong, H.; Fang, Y.R.; Angst, J. Validity of the 32-item Hypomania Checklist (HCL-32) in a clinical sample with mood disorders in China. BMC Psychiatry 2011, 11, 84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Plutchik, R.; Van Praag, H.M. Interconvertability of five self-report measures of depression. Psychiatry Res. 1987, 22, 243–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Wang, W.; Cao, M.; Zhu, S.; Gu, J.; Liu, J.; Wang, Y. Zuckerman-Kuhlman’s Personality Questionnaire in patients with major depression. Soc. Behav. Pers. 2002, 30, 757–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Lang, P.J.; Bradley, M.M.; Cuthbert, B.N. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual; Technical Report A-8; NIMH, Center for the Study of Emotion & Attention: Gainesville, FL, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  41. Bradley, M.M.; Lang, P.J. The International Affective Digitized Sounds (IADS-2): Affective Ratings of Sounds and Instruction Manual; Technical Report B-3; University of Florida: Gainesville, FL, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  42. Dell’Osso, L.; Carmassi, C.; Carlini, M.; Rucci, P.; Torri, P.; Cesari, D.; Landi, P.; Ciapparelli, A.; Maggi, M. Sexual dysfunctions and suicidality in patients with bipolar disorder and unipolar depression. J. Sex. Med. 2009, 6, 3063–3070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Mazza, M.; Mazza, S.; Harnic, D.; Catalano, V.; Di Nicola, M.; Bruschi, A.; Bria, P.; Daniele, A.; Mazza, S. Sexual behavior in women with bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2011, 131, 364–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Kopeykina, I.; Kim, H.; Khatun, T.; Boland, J.; Haeri, S.; Cohen, L.J.; Galynker, I.I. Hypersexuality and couple relationships in bipolar disorder: a review. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 195, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Itakura, N.; Sakamoto, K. A new method for calculating eye movement displacement from AC coupled electro-oculographic signals in head mounted eye–gaze input interfaces. Biomed. Signal. Process. Control. 2010, 5, 142–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Chang, C.Y.; Chang, C.W.; Zheng, J.Y.; Chung, P.C. Physiological emotion analysis using support vector regression. Neurocomputing 2013, 122, 79–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Xu, S.; Gao, Q.; Ma, L.; Fan, H.; Mao, H.; Liu, J.; Wang, W. The Zuckerman-Kuhlman personality questionnaire in bipolar I and II disorders: a preliminary report. Psychiatry Res. 2015, 226, 357–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Yao, J.; Xu, Y.; Qin, Y.; Liu, J.; Shen, Y.; Wang, W.; Chen, W. Relationship between personality disorder functioning styles and the emotional states in bipolar I and II disorders. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0117353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Yu, E.; Li, H.; Fan, H.; Gao, Q.; Tan, Y.; Lou, J.; Lou, J.; Wang, W. Relationship between Chinese Adjective Descriptors of Personality and emotional symptoms in young Chinese patients with bipolar disorders. J. Int. Med. Res. 2015, 43, 790–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Kupka, R.W.; Altshuler, L.L.; Nolen, W.A.; Suppes, T.; Luckenbaugh, D.A.; Leverich, G.S.; Frye, M.A.; Keck, P.E.; McElroy, S.L.; Grunze, H.; et al. Three times more days depressed than manic or hypomanic in both bipolar I and bipolar II disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2007, 9, 531–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. M’Bailara, K.; Demotesmainard, J.; Swendsen, J.; Mathieu, F.; Leboyer, M.; Henry, C. Emotional hyper-reactivity in normothymic bipolar patients. Bipolar Disord. 2009, 11, 63–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Lemaire, M.; El-Hage, W.; Frangou, S. Increased affective reactivity to neutral stimuli and decreased maintenance of affective responses in bipolar disorder. Eur. Psychiatry 2015, 30, 852–860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Minassian, A.; Henry, B.L.; Geyer, M.A.; Paulus, M.P.; Young, J.W.; Perry, W. The quantitative assessment of motor activity in mania and schizophrenia. J. Affect. Disord. 2010, 120, 200–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Lawrence, N.S.; Williams, A.M.; Surguladze, S.; Giampietro, V.; Brammer, M.J.; Andrew, C.; Frangou, S.; Ecker, C.; Phillips, M.L. Subcortical and ventral prefrontal cortical neural responses to facial expressions distinguish patients with bipolar disorder and major depression. Biol. Psychiatry 2004, 55, 578–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Bylsma, L.M.; Morris, B.H.; Rottenberg, J. A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2008, 28, 676–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Parikh, S.V.; Velyvis, V.; Yatham, L.; Beaulieu, S.; Cervantes, P.; MacQueen, G.; Siotis, I.; Streiner, D.; Zaretsky, A. Coping styles in prodromes of bipolar mania. Bipolar Disord. 2007, 9, 589–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Simpson, S.G.; Al-Mufti, R.; Andersen, A.E.; DePaulo, J.R. Bipolar II affective disorder in eating disorder inpatients. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1992, 180, 719–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Cunnington, R.; Iansek, R.; Bradshaw, J.L.; Phillips, J.G. Movement-related potentials associated with movement preparation and motor imagery. Exp. Brain Res. 1996, 111, 429–436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Raila, H.; Scholl, B.J.; Gruber, J. Seeing the world through rose-colored glasses: People who are happy and satisfied with life preferentially attend to positive stimuli. Emotion 2015, 15, 449–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Illustrations of electrocardiogram and electrooculogram and their rectifications and quantifications in a healthy volunteer under blank-b1sadness-THappiness-b2sadness emotional combination: (I) electrocardiogram raw; (II) RR interval; (III) electrooculogram raw; (IV) dp/dt of electrooculogram; (V) absolute value of the (IV). See Table 1 for explanations of the emotional epochs.
Figure 1. Illustrations of electrocardiogram and electrooculogram and their rectifications and quantifications in a healthy volunteer under blank-b1sadness-THappiness-b2sadness emotional combination: (I) electrocardiogram raw; (II) RR interval; (III) electrooculogram raw; (IV) dp/dt of electrooculogram; (V) absolute value of the (IV). See Table 1 for explanations of the emotional epochs.
Ijerph 15 00884 g001
Table 1. Combination and permutation of external emotional stimulations after blank (lasting for 10 s).
Table 1. Combination and permutation of external emotional stimulations after blank (lasting for 10 s).
Emotional CombinationEmotional Permutation
Target emotionBackground emotionbackground(b1, lasting for 6 s)Target(T, lasting for 8 s)same background again(b2, lasting for 10 s)
EroticaDisgustb1disgustTEroticab2disgust
Fearb1fearTEroticab2fear
Happinessb1happinessTEroticab2happiness
Neutralb1neutralTEroticab2neutral
Sadnessb1sadnessTEroticab2sadness
HappinessDisgustb1disgustTHappinessb2disgust
Eroticab1eroticaTHappinessb2erotica
Fearb1fearTHappinessb2fear
Neutralb1neutralTHappinessb2neutral
Sadnessb1sadnessTHappinessb2sadness
NeutralDisgustb1disgustTNeutralb2disgust
Eroticab1eroticaTNeutralb2erotica
Fearb1fearTNeutralb2fear
Happinessb1happinessTNeutralb2happiness
Sadnessb1sadnessTNeutralb2sadness
SadnessDisgustb1disgustTSadnessb2disgust
Eroticab1eroticaTSadnessb2erotica
Fearb1fearTSadnessb2fear
Happinessb1happinessTSadnessb2happiness
Neutralb1neutralTSadnessb2neutral
Table 2. Questionnaire scale scores (mean ± S.D.) in healthy volunteers (controls, n = 139) and in patients with bipolar I (BD I, n = 69) and II (BD II, n = 54) disorders.
Table 2. Questionnaire scale scores (mean ± S.D.) in healthy volunteers (controls, n = 139) and in patients with bipolar I (BD I, n = 69) and II (BD II, n = 54) disorders.
ScaleControlsBD IBD II
Mood Disorder Questionnaire2.79 ± 2.049.41 ± 1.53 a3.35 ± 1.88 b
Hypomanic Checklist-3214.2 ± 5.1622.01 ± 2.96 a19.96 ± 2.84 a,b
Plutchik-van Praag Depression Inventory8.83 ± 4.8416.68 ± 12.13 a24.65 ± 15.71 a,b
Note: a, p < 0.05 vs. controls; b, p < 0.05 vs. BD I.
Table 3. Intensity ratings of target emotions (mean ± S.D.; normalized, with raw data in brackets) in healthy volunteers (controls, n = 139) and in patients with bipolar I (BD I, n = 69) and II (BD II, n = 54) disorders.
Table 3. Intensity ratings of target emotions (mean ± S.D.; normalized, with raw data in brackets) in healthy volunteers (controls, n = 139) and in patients with bipolar I (BD I, n = 69) and II (BD II, n = 54) disorders.
Emotional EpochControlsBD IBD II
(b1disgust) Terotica0.66 ± 0.32 (4.99 ± 2.29)0.71 ± 0.27 (5.29 ± 2.02)0.70 ± 0.31 (5.29 ± 2.56)
(b1disgust) Thappiness0.69 ± 0.31 (5.14 ± 2.13)0.73 ± 0.32 (5.41 ± 2.27)0.79 ± 0.29 (5.73 ± 2.47)
(b1disgust) Tneutral0.69 ± 0.28 (5.08 ± 2.12)0.73 ± 0.31 (5.33 ± 2.21)0.66 ± 0.34 (5.18 ± 2.68)
(b1disgust) TSadness0.70 ± 0.29 (5.11 ± 2.11)0.76 ± 0.25 (5.42 ± 1.98)0.65 ± 0.32 (4.86 ± 2.51)
(b1erotica) THappiness0.53 ± 0.29 (4.11 ± 2.03)0.54 ± 0.30 (4.00 ± 2.15)0.52 ± 0.32 (4.14 ± 2.49)
(b1erotica) TNeutral0.51 ± 0.30 (4.07 ± 1.95)0.58 ± 0.23 (4.36 ± 1.63)0.50 ± 0.34 (3.94 ± 2.43)
(b1erotica) TSadness0.48 ± 0.30 (3.90 ± 1.98)0.49 ± 0.29 (3.94 ± 2.08)0.52 ± 0.32 (4.06 ± 2.47)
(b1fear) TErotica0.61 ± 0.32 (4.77 ± 2.34)0.66 ± 0.32 (5.03 ± 2.34)0.54 ± 0.33 (4.29 ± 2.54)
(b1fear) THappiness0.63 ± 0.35 (4.83 ± 2.45)0.69 ± 0.29 (5.23 ± 2.29)0.62 ± 0.35 (4.67 ± 2.73)
(b1fear) TNeutral0.60 ± 0.32 (4.73 ± 2.33)0.69 ± 0.31 (5.24 ± 2.33)0.54 ± 0.36 b (4.20 ± 2.79)
(b1fear) TSadness0.62 ± 0.35 (4.73 ± 2.40)0.68 ± 0.32 (5.23 ± 2.35)0.56 ± 0.37 (4.45 ± 2.61)
(b1happiness) TErotica0.49 ± 0.30 (3.89 ± 2.05)0.48 ± 0.30 (3.91 ± 2.17)0.51 ± 0.33 (4.04 ± 2.45)
(b1happiness) TNeutral0.45 ± 0.33 (3.73 ± 2.09)0.44 ± 0.30 (3.70 ± 2.08)0.50 ± 0.33 (4.02 ± 2.39)
(b1happiness) TSadness0.41 ± 0.33 (3.48 ± 2.15)0.42 ± 0.31 (3.50 ± 2.23)0.43 ± 0.36 (3.49 ± 2.45)
(b1neutral) TErotica0.24 ± 0.28 (2.43 ± 1.88)0.27 ± 0.27 (2.62 ± 1.86)0.23 ± 0.25 (2.25 ± 1.91)
(b1neutral) THappiness0.19 ± 0.26 (2.18 ± 1.86)0.15 ± 0.20 (2.06 ± 1.71)0.21 ± 0.25 (2.04 ± 1.81)
(b1neutral) TSadness0.23 ± 0.27 (2.36 ± 1.90)0.20 ± 0.25 (2.32 ± 2.02)0.22 ± 0.32 (2.18 ± 2.30)
(b1sadness) TErotica0.37 ± 0.27 (3.33 ± 1.92)0.42 ± 0.23 (3.61 ± 1.82)0.42 ± 0.34 (3.39 ± 2.52)
(b1sadness) THappiness0.41 ± 0.30 (3.48 ± 2.07)0.49 ± 0.28 (3.97 ± 2.10)0.47 ± 0.35 (3.84 ± 2.65)
(b1sadness) TNeutral0.38 ± 0.28 (3.38 ± 2.04)0.43 ± 0.25 (3.62 ± 1.90)0.47 ± 0.32 (3.71 ± 2.49)
Note: b, p < 0.05 vs. BD I; see Table 1 for explanations of the emotional epochs.
Table 4. Electrocardiogram data (mean ± S.D.; normalized) during different emotional-epochs in healthy volunteers (controls, n = 139) and patients with bipolar I (BD I, n = 69) and II (BD II, n = 54) disorders.
Table 4. Electrocardiogram data (mean ± S.D.; normalized) during different emotional-epochs in healthy volunteers (controls, n = 139) and patients with bipolar I (BD I, n = 69) and II (BD II, n = 54) disorders.
Emotional EpochControlsBD IBD II
b1disgust (TErotica)0.56 ± 0.210.58 ± 0.210.59 ± 0.20
b1disgust (THappiness)0.51 ± 0.220.52 ± 0.200.51 ± 0.21
b1disgust (TNeutral)0.53 ± 0.220.51 ± 0.240.57 ± 0.18
b1disgust (TSadness)0.52 ± 0.210.56 ± 0.190.54 ± 0.21
b1erotica (THappiness)0.53 ± 0.210.52 ± 0.220.52 ± 0.21
b1erotica (TNeutral)0.55 ± 0.210.57 ± 0.210.53 ± 0.21
b1erotica (TSadness)0.56 ± 0.210.54 ± 0.200.60 ± 0.20
b1fear (TErotica)0.58 ± 0.220.61 ± 0.200.58 ± 0.22
b1fear (THappiness)0.53 ± 0.220.57 ± 0.230.53 ± 0.21
b1fear (TNeutral)0.52 ± 0.220.52 ± 0.200.55 ± 0.21
b1fear (TSadness)0.53 ± 0.200.53 ± 0.200.54 ± 0.19
b1happiness (TErotica)0.57 ± 0.210.60 ± 0.200.62 ± 0.19
b1happiness (TNeutral)0.57 ± 0.220.56 ± 0.230.55 ± 0.21
b1happiness (TSadness)0.50 ± 0.220.57 ± 0.200.48 ± 0.24
b1neutral (TErotica)0.55 ± 0.200.58 ± 0.230.54 ± 0.19
b1neutral (THappiness)0.54 ± 0.230.55 ± 0.210.52 ± 0.17
b1neutral (TSadness)0.55 ± 0.210.57 ± 0.200.54 ± 0.21
b1sadness (TErotica)0.58 ± 0.240.56 ± 0.230.60 ± 0.23
b1sadness (THappiness)0.51 ± 0.230.54 ± 0.230.52 ± 0.20
b1sadness (TNeutral)0.57 ± 0.220.55 ± 0.210.59 ± 0.24
(b1disgust) TErotica0.62 ± 0.180.61 ± 0.200.65 ± 0.19
(b1disgust) THappiness0.57 ± 0.200.53 ± 0.240.56 ± 0.22
(b1disgust) TNeutral0.57 ± 0.200.55 ± 0.230.63 ± 0.20
(b1disgust) TSadness0.59 ± 0.210.56 ± 0.220.61 ± 0.20
(b1erotica) THappiness0.58 ± 0.170.60 ± 0.210.58 ± 0.20
(b1erotica) TNeutral0.61 ± 0.200.61 ± 0.160.65 ± 0.19
(b1erotica) TSadness0.60 ± 0.200.62 ± 0.210.64 ± 0.23
(b1fear) TErotica0.69 ± 0.210.69 ± 0.180.67 ± 0.22
(b1fear) THappiness0.63 ± 0.210.64 ± 0.200.62 ± 0.21
(b1fear) TNeutral0.64 ± 0.200.60 ± 0.200.65 ± 0.21
(b1fear) TSadness0.65 ± 0.210.66 ± 0.210.66 ± 0.20
(b1happiness) TErotica0.57 ± 0.200.58 ± 0.170.62 ± 0.18
(b1happiness) TNeutral0.57 ± 0.190.55 ± 0.190.54 ± 0.16
(b1happiness) TSadness0.54 ± 0.180.54 ± 0.190.53 ± 0.21
(b1neutral) TErotica0.64 ± 0.180.61 ± 0.210.65 ± 0.18
(b1neutral) THappiness0.55 ± 0.200.63 ± 0.200.55 ± 0.16
(b1neutral) TSadness0.59 ± 0.200.58 ± 0.200.62 ± 0.20
(b1sadness) TErotica0.63 ± 0.210.57 ± 0.220.61 ± 0.21
(b1sadness) THappiness0.55 ± 0.200.53 ± 0.200.55 ± 0.19
(b1sadness) TNeutral0.59 ± 0.200.59 ± 0.200.57 ± 0.23
(TErotica) b2disgust0.54 ± 0.180.53 ± 0.210.58 ± 0.18
(THappiness) b2disgust0.50 ± 0.210.45 ± 0.220.52 ± 0.21
(TNeutral) b2disgust0.49 ± 0.200.49 ± 0.210.54 ± 0.18
(TSadness) b2disgust0.52 ± 0.210.51 ± 0.210.55 ± 0.19
(THappiness) b2erotica0.56 ± 0.180.54 ± 0.190.52 ± 0.18
(TNeutral) b2erotica0.61 ± 0.190.58 ± 0.190.62 ± 0.18
(TSadness) b2erotica0.57 ± 0.210.59 ± 0.200.60 ± 0.19
(TErotica) b2fear0.64 ± 0.190.58 ± 0.200.63 ± 0.19
(THappiness) b2fear0.56 ± 0.190.52 ± 0.200.56 ± 0.18
(TNeutral) b2fear0.59 ± 0.190.50 ± 0.21 a0.61 ± 0.17 b
(TSadness) b2fear0.57 ± 0.220.54 ± 0.170.59 ± 0.21
(TErotica) b2happiness0.58 ± 0.200.56 ± 0.200.61 ± 0.20
(TNeutral) b2happiness0.54 ± 0.190.53 ± 0.190.49 ± 0.21
(TSadness) b2happiness0.51 ± 0.200.51 ± 0.190.52 ± 0.21
(TErotica) b2neutral0.61 ± 0.180.60 ± 0.200.63 ± 0.20
(THappiness) b2neutral0.53 ± 0.200.55 ± 0.170.51 ± 0.18
(TSadness) b2neutral0.56 ± 0.190.51 ± 0.180.56 ± 0.17
(TErotica) b2sadness0.60 ± 0.200.59 ± 0.170.65 ± 0.21
(THappiness) b2sadness0.52 ± 0.200.48 ± 0.180.51 ± 0.18
(TNeutral) b2sadness0.55 ± 0.200.51 ± 0.220.54 ± 0.23
Note: a, p < 0.05 vs. controls; b, p < 0.05 vs. BD I; see Table 1 for explanations of the emotional epochs.
Table 5. Electrooculogram data (mean ± S.D.; normalized) during different emotional epochs in healthy volunteers (controls, n = 139) and patients with bipolar I (BD I, n = 69) and II (BD II, n = 54) disorders.
Table 5. Electrooculogram data (mean ± S.D.; normalized) during different emotional epochs in healthy volunteers (controls, n = 139) and patients with bipolar I (BD I, n = 69) and II (BD II, n = 54) disorders.
Emotion EpochControlsBD IBD II
b1disgust (TErotica)0.36 ± 0.210.34 ± 0.210.36 ± 0.20
b1disgust (THappiness)0.34 ± 0.230.36 ± 0.240.32 ± 0.19
b1disgust (TNeutral)0.22 ± 0.190.28 ± 0.220.23 ± 0.21
b1disgust (TSadness)0.36 ± 0.210.40 ± 0.200.38 ± 0.18
b1erotica (THappiness)0.30 ± 0.200.36 ± 0.190.35 ± 0.22
b1erotica (TNeutral)0.22 ± 0.170.26 ± 0.190.30 ± 0.22
b1erotica (TSadness)0.37 ± 0.220.34 ± 0.190.39 ± 0.21
b1fear (TErotica)0.34 ± 0.210.36 ± 0.210.42 ± 0.23
b1fear (THappiness)0.35 ± 0.240.34 ± 0.220.31 ± 0.19
b1fear (TNeutral)0.26 ± 0.200.24 ± 0.190.27 ± 0.19
b1fear (TSadness)0.36 ± 0.180.40 ± 0.200.42 ± 0.19
b1happiness (TErotica)0.37 ± 0.230.40 ± 0.250.40 ± 0.23
b1happiness (TNeutral)0.26 ± 0.230.27 ± 0.220.24 ± 0.16
b1happiness (TSadness)0.41 ± 0.200.39 ± 0.220.37 ± 0.24
b1neutral (TErotica)0.34 ± 0.230.37 ± 0.220.35 ± 0.26
b1neutral (THappiness)0.31 ± 0.200.34 ± 0.220.32 ± 0.20
b1neutral (TSadness)0.39 ± 0.220.35 ± 0.190.40 ± 0.22
b1sadness (TErotica)0.38 ± 0.220.38 ± 0.240.36 ± 0.19
b1sadness (THappiness)0.32 ± 0.210.34 ± 0.210.34 ± 0.18
b1sadness (TNeutral)0.22 ± 0.180.24 ± 0.200.31 ± 0.23
(b1disgust) TErotica0.27 ± 0.200.32 ± 0.260.28 ± 0.19
(b1disgust) THappiness0.30 ± 0.230.39 ± 0.26 a0.30 ± 0.21
(b1disgust) TNeutral0.25 ± 0.200.32 ± 0.240.27 ± 0.20
(b1disgust) TSadness0.26 ± 0.210.31 ± 0.230.21 ± 0.14 b
(b1erotica) THappiness0.33 ± 0.220.36 ± 0.220.36 ± 0.24
(b1erotica) TNeutral0.28 ± 0.200.32 ± 0.200.33 ± 0.21
(b1erotica) TSadness0.33 ± 0.210.31 ± 0.200.37 ± 0.23
(b1fear) TErotica0.34 ± 0.220.32 ± 0.210.38 ± 0.20
(b1fear) THappiness0.36 ± 0.220.38 ± 0.220.33 ± 0.18
(b1fear) TNeutral0.31 ± 0.190.33 ± 0.200.37 ± 0.22
(b1fear) TSadness0.33 ± 0.210.35 ± 0.200.41 ± 0.21
(b1happiness) TErotica0.30 ± 0.190.31 ± 0.210.27 ± 0.16
(b1happiness) TNeutral0.30 ± 0.220.28 ± 0.190.33 ± 0.25
(b1happiness) TSadness0.30 ± 0.200.31 ± 0.240.26 ± 0.17
(b1neutral) TErotica0.22 ± 0.190.21 ± 0.220.18 ± 0.18
(b1neutral) THappiness0.24 ± 0.190.23 ± 0.180.28 ± 0.21
(b1neutral) TSadness0.20 ± 0.180.17 ± 0.160.20 ± 0.15
(b1sadness) TErotica0.35 ± 0.200.35 ± 0.220.32 ± 0.20
(b1sadness) THappiness0.37 ± 0.210.34 ± 0.220.36 ± 0.20
(b1sadness) TNeutral0.32 ± 0.200.29 ± 0.180.38 ± 0.24
(TErotica) b2disgust0.33 ± 0.210.38 ± 0.220.33 ± 0.23
(THappiness) b2disgust0.35 ± 0.220.39 ± 0.220.34 ± 0.19
(TNeutral) b2disgust0.26 ± 0.210.35 ± 0.24 a0.29 ± 0.21
(TSadness) b2disgust0.35 ± 0.220.40 ± 0.230.35 ± 0.22
(THappiness) b2erotica0.29 ± 0.190.31 ± 0.220.32 ± 0.24
(TNeutral) b2erotica0.21 ± 0.160.24 ± 0.220.23 ± 0.17
(TSadness) b2erotica0.33 ± 0.210.31 ± 0.220.33 ± 0.17
(TErotica) b2fear0.32 ± 0.200.34 ± 0.240.38 ± 0.23
(THappiness) b2fear0.33 ± 0.220.35 ± 0.220.31 ± 0.19
(TNeutral) b2fear0.24 ± 0.180.30 ± 0.200.29 ± 0.18
(TSadness) b2fear0.36 ± 0.220.39 ± 0.230.37 ± 0.19
(TErotica) b2happiness0.34 ± 0.240.37 ± 0.190.33 ± 0.24
(TNeutral) b2happiness0.28 ± 0.210.30 ± 0.210.32 ± 0.22
(TSadness) b2happiness0.39 ± 0.200.38 ± 0.240.32 ± 0.22
(TErotica) b2neutral0.29 ± 0.210.32 ± 0.230.29 ± 0.21
(THappiness) b2neutral0.29 ± 0.200.30 ± 0.200.34 ± 0.22
(TSadness) b2neutral0.33 ± 0.200.31 ± 0.210.31 ± 0.21
(TErotica) b2sadness0.33 ± 0.220.29 ± 0.210.29 ± 0.18
(THappiness) b2sadness0.32 ± 0.230.27 ± 0.150.29 ± 0.21
(TNeutral) b2sadness0.21 ± 0.170.25 ± 0.220.28 ± 0.21
Note: a, p < 0.05 vs. controls; b, p < 0.05 vs. BD I; see Table 1 for explanations of the emotional epochs.
Table 6. Measurement results of affective states and perceived intensity, and of heart rate and eyeball movement in bipolar I (BD I) and II (BD II) disorders and healthy controls (controls).
Table 6. Measurement results of affective states and perceived intensity, and of heart rate and eyeball movement in bipolar I (BD I) and II (BD II) disorders and healthy controls (controls).
MeasuresScale/ConditionBD I vs. ControlsBD II vs. ControlsBD I vs. BD II
ScoreMood Disorder Questionnaire-
ScoreHypomanic Checklist-32
ScorePlutchik-van Praag Depression Inventory
Perceived intensity(b1fear) TNeutral--
Heart rate(TNeutral) b2fear-
Eyeball movement(b1disgust) THappiness--
Eyeball movement(b1disgust) TSadness--
Eyeball movement(TNeutral) b2disgust--
Note: see Table 1 for explanations of the emotional epochs.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ma, G.; Wang, C.; Jia, Y.; Wang, J.; Zhang, B.; Shen, C.; Fan, H.; Pan, B.; Wang, W. Electrocardiographic and Electrooculographic Responses to External Emotions and Their Transitions in Bipolar I and II Disorders. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 884. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15050884

AMA Style

Ma G, Wang C, Jia Y, Wang J, Zhang B, Shen C, Fan H, Pan B, Wang W. Electrocardiographic and Electrooculographic Responses to External Emotions and Their Transitions in Bipolar I and II Disorders. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018; 15(5):884. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15050884

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ma, Guorong, Chu Wang, Yanli Jia, Jiawei Wang, Bingren Zhang, Chanchan Shen, Hongying Fan, Bing Pan, and Wei Wang. 2018. "Electrocardiographic and Electrooculographic Responses to External Emotions and Their Transitions in Bipolar I and II Disorders" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 15, no. 5: 884. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15050884

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop