Next Article in Journal
You Can’t Find Healthy Food in the Bush: Poor Accessibility, Availability and Adequacy of Food in Rural Australia
Next Article in Special Issue
Determinants of Spatial Heterogeneity of Functional Illiteracy among School-Aged Children in the Philippines: An Ecological Study
Previous Article in Journal
Current Status and Policy Planning for Promoting Age-Friendly Cities in Taitung County: Dialogue Between Older Adults and Service Providers
Previous Article in Special Issue
Perspectives on Inclusive Education of Preschool Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders and Other Developmental Disabilities in Iran
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Participation in Social Activities on Cognitive Function Among Middle-Aged and Older Adults in Korea

1
Division of Social Welfare and Health Administration, Wonkwang University, Iksan 54538, Korea
2
Department of Family Medicine & Biomedical Sciences, Seoul National University, Seoul 03080, Korea
3
Department of Health Care Policy Research, Korea Institute for Health and Social Affairs, Sejong 30147, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15(10), 2315; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15102315
Submission received: 21 July 2018 / Revised: 10 October 2018 / Accepted: 18 October 2018 / Published: 21 October 2018

Abstract

:
Cognitive function is a critical health issue in later life, the decline of which disrupts well-being and daily life function. Cognitive decline in older ages can also be understood in the context of the social environment such as social connectedness and engagement in personal life. This study aimed to examine: (1) whether participation in social activities contributes to preventing cognitive decline, and (2) what type of social activities are beneficial to maintaining cognitive function. Data from the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (KLOSA) 2006–2014, a longitudinal survey of the household-dwelling population aged 45 and older in Korea were used. The results revealed that Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores decreased with increasing age, at a rate of approximately 0.18 units across all age-gender groups, and the decrease was steeper for adults aged 65 and over. Participation in social gatherings was likely to delay the decline in cognitive function after the age of 65. In a gender-stratified model, social activity may not have an impact on the decline of cognitive function for men, whereas participation in social gatherings was negatively related to the decline of MMSE scores in women. This study suggests the need for a gender-stratified policy for preventing the decline of cognitive function while promoting engagement in social activities in Korean older adults.

1. Introduction

Cognitive function is a critical health issue in later life [1,2], and the decline of which disrupts well-being and daily life function [3]. Accumulative studies have documented age-related decrements in cognitive function, indicating that cognitive decline is a general part of brain aging [4,5]. However, biological aging is not entirely responsible for cognitive impairment among older adults. Cognitive function in older ages can also be understood in the context of their social environment, such as social connectedness and engagement in personal life [2,6,7]. A review of the association between cognitive function and social lifestyle reported that social relationships, including engagement, activity, and networking, exhibit a close relationship with cognitive function [8,9]. Individuals who are less socially active are more likely to experience cognitive declines compared with those who lead more active social lifestyles [10]. These findings suggest that more social engagement in later life may help delay or prevent cognitive decline [11]. In fact, a burgeoning body of literature highlights the fact that greater participation in a wide range of social activities in later life contributes to the prevention of cognitive impairment [12]. It has been suggested that active participation in various activities may protect cognitive function by providing stimulation. A number of studies have reported that engaging in social activities is beneficial to maintaining better physical and mental health outcomes, including better cognitive function, ultimately leading to a reduced risk of dementia, particularly among older adults [13,14]. Participation in social activities offers important social roles, self-esteem, and social competence, which may protect against neuropathology (i.e., reducing the stress response) [15,16,17]. For instance, a community-based prospective study suggested that participation in senior citizen clubs or senior centers may produce beneficial effects in reducing cognitive decline in later life [18]. In studies of older American and Swedish adults, participating in social and leisure activities helped prevent cognitive decline [19,20]. Furthermore, a recent population-based prospective study reported that emotional exchanges in social situations and satisfaction in relationships are both protective factors against cognitive decline among older adults [21].
Participation in social activities is part of a socially-oriented sharing of individual resources, which includes a wide range of activities involving interactions with family, friends, and other social groups [22]. It has been suggested that social activities related to economic security and spiritual well-being may contribute to improving, or at least maintaining, physical and mental health, ultimately associated with longevity in older adults [11]. With respect to cognitive function, it is hypothesized that engagement with cognitively stimulating activity benefits its function, reducing the possibility of cognitive decline [23]. However, social activities do not have a consistently positive impact if participation in social groups brings about interpersonal conflict, which may lead to psychological distress, adversely impacting health [24,25]. In this sense, it could be understood that various social activities differently affect cognitive function, given that each activity has its own purpose. In addition to the potentially differential impacts of various activities, the benefits of social activities may differ by gender. For example, one cross-sectional study among Chinese older adults demonstrated that volunteer work was associated with better cognitive function among only women, whereas participating in hobby groups was associated with better cognitive function among both men and women [8]. Other studies suggested that older women might have more beneficial effects from involvement in social activity compared with men, because older men are more likely to receive social and emotional support from their spouses [18,26,27,28]. Thus, older women tend to gain noteworthy caring support from other social network, such as friends, colleagues, and neighborhoods, rather than their spouses [18,26,27,28].
Prevention, or at least the delay, of cognitive decline in later life is a prominent issue in the field of health policy. Although an increasing number of studies seek to understand the relationship between cognitive function and participation in social activities [6,11,13,29], it is still not clear whether different social activities have different impacts on cognitive function among middle-aged and older individuals in addition to different impacts based on gender, particularly the Korean population. Recent studies using data from the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (KLOSA) attempted to assess the relationship between different types of social activities and cognitive function in older adults, indicating that the role of social activity should receive more attention given the close association between participation in social activity and cognitive function [17,29]. Although their results may provide some insight into the relationship, these studies failed to accurately scrutinize the impact of social activities on cognitive function and overlooked gender differences in the relationship between social activity and cognitive decline. This could have been because of the study design, whether it was a cross-sectional or a comparison of certain points. To reduce the knowledge gap, this study aimed to examine: (1) whether participation in social activities contributes to preventing cognitive decline in older adults, and (2) what type of social activities are beneficial to maintaining cognitive function at older ages using nationally representative longitudinal survey data.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Population

Data were derived from the KLOSA, which was conducted by the Korean Employment Information Service. The KLOSA is a nationally representative longitudinal survey of the household-dwelling population aged 45 and older in Korea using multi-stage, stratified probability sampling [30]. A total of 10,254 individuals completed interviews at baseline (2006). Interviews were conducted every even-numbered year up to 2014, which resulted in five survey waves. At each wave, information regarding education, income, assets, employment, lifestyle, health status, and cognitive function was collected. The follow-up rate for each wave was 86.6%, 80.3%, 76.2%, and 72.8% for the 2008, 2010, 2012, and 2014 surveys, respectively [30]. The current study selected 7299 adults with normal cognitive function at baseline, which was defined as a Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (K-MMSE) score ≥24. Given missing covariates at baseline, 593 participants were excluded from the study. Of 6706 eligible respondents, 5948 (88.7%) provided at least one memory score and 4453 (66.4%) provided four memory scores at all assessments during the follow-up period (after the baseline survey).

2.2. Assessment of Cognitive Function

Cognitive function measured by K-MMSE scores was used as a dependent variable. The K-MMSE, a structured questionnaire to measure global cognitive performance and screen for dementia, consists of 11 items in seven categories including orientation for time, place, and person, registration of three objects, attention and calculation, recall of three words, language, and visual construction [31]. Total K-MMSE scores range from 0 to 30, with normal cognitive function defined as score of 24 or greater [31,32]. The K-MMSE was validated elsewhere [32]. The present study used K-MMSE scores over the eight-year follow-up as outcome measures.

2.3. Assessment of Social Activity

Social activity was assessed at baseline using four questions: (1) “Do you participate in religious meetings?” (Religious parties), (2) “Do you participate in social clubs (Private savings clubs, senior citizens’ clubs), or leisure, cultural, or sports groups?” (Social gatherings), (3) “Do you participate in alumni societies, societies for people from the same hometown, or family councils?” (Alumni/clan gatherings), and (4) “Do you participate in volunteer groups, political parties, non-government organizations, or interest groups?” (Volunteer work). This study assessed the relationship between each social activity and cognitive performance.

2.4. Assessment of Covariates

Sociodemographic covariates included gender (male/female), age, educational status (elementary school, middle school, high school, or college and above), household income quartile (first quartile as the poorest), working status (yes/no), marital status (single/married), and residence (urban/rural). Based on the self-reported information, health-related covariates included physical activity, smoking status (ever/never smoked), alcohol use (non-drinkers/current drinkers), limited activities of daily living (0, ≥1), depression (10-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies–Depression Scale ≥4, <4), and comorbidity (at least one of hypertension, diabetes, cardiovascular disease, and/or cerebrovascular disease, 0). All covariates mentioned above were measured at baseline (2006).

2.5. Statistical Analysis

We used multi-level mixed models to test the hypothesis that individuals with higher baseline social activity would experience a slower rate of decline in cognitive performance during the follow-up period. The multi-level models allowed us to examine the trajectory of cognitive performance based on the level of baseline social activity, the average rate of cognitive performance over time.
First, to assess the overall change of K-MMSE scores related to increasing age, the difference between actual age and an age of 45 (age-45) was included. However, even if baseline social activity did not relate to the change of cognitive function in middle age, it could affect the change of cognitive function in old age (older than 65). Therefore, a spline at the age of 64 was introduced to assess the change of K-MMSE scores related to increasing age in adults over the age of 65, indicating that the difference between actual age and the age of 65 for adults older than 65 (adults under 64 had a value of 0) was included. In the models, the changes over time for adults over the age of 65 was assessed by the sum of two coefficients of age-45 and age-65. Second, to assess the difference based on the level of social activity in rate of change of cognitive performance over the years, the interactions of social activities with the variables of age-45 and the variable of age-65 were introduced. The decline (or increasing) rate in MMSE scores in adults over the age of 65 was assessed as the sum of between coefficients of two interaction terms. The present study analyzed the pooled sample and the sample stratified by gender, which resulted in three regression analytic models. In addition, we used the Wald test to examine whether the association of social activity with cognition depended on gender. Given that correlations between social activities were weak, ranging between 0.023 and 0.207, the association of four types of social activity with cognition was assessed in the same analytic model. All analyses were conducted with SAS 9.0 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). The PROC MIXED procedure was used for multi-level linear mixed models.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

The baseline demographics of participants are depicted in Table 1. Of the total adults aged 45 years or older (n = 6706), 3347 (49.9%) were male and 3359 (50.1%) were female. The proportion of respondents who participated in religious meetings was 24.7%. The proportion of respondents who participated in social clubs or leisure, cultural, or sports groups was 62.6%. The proportion of respondents who participated in alumni societies, societies for people from the same hometown, or family councils was 25.8%. The proportion of respondents who participated in volunteer groups, political parties, non-government organizations, or interest groups was 4.6%.

3.2. Relationship between Social Activities and Cognitive Decline

Among all participants, MMSE scores decreased with increasing age, at an approximate rate of 0.18 per year between the ages of 45 and 65, as shown in all four columns (Table 2). The decrease in MMSE scores was steeper at up to 0.22 per year for adults aged 65+, indicating that MMSE scores decreased at an approximate rate of 0.39 per year over the age of 65. Baseline social activity (all kinds of social activity) was not related to the slope of changes of MMSE scores when age was less than 65, revealing insignificant coefficients of interaction terms between age-45 and each social activity. Baseline social gatherings (but not religious parties, alumni/clan gatherings, and volunteer work) were positively related to the slope of changes of MMSE scores when age was greater than 65 (p = 0.01). In other words, social gatherings were likely to delay the decline of cognitive function when individuals were older than 65, by approximately 0.08 units (Table 2).
Among men, MMSE scores decreased with increasing age, at a rate of approximately 0.18 units. The decrease in MMSE scores was steeper for those aged older than 65 compared with younger participants. At baseline, social activity of any kind was not related to the MMSE scores. However, baseline social activity did not appear to delay the decline of MMSE scores. Social activity may not impact the decline of cognitive function for men (Table 2).
Among women, MMSE scores decreased with increasing age at a rate of approximately 0.18 units. The decline in scores was steeper for those aged older than 65. At baseline, social activity of any type was not related to MMSE score. In addition, baseline social activity was not related to the decline in MMSE score in individuals younger than 65. Participation in social gatherings contributed to preserving the decline of MMSE scores (β = 0.118, p = 0.011), whereas religious activities, alumni/clan gatherings, and volunteer work did not exhibit significant relationships with the decline of MMSE scores in individuals older than 65. In other words, social gatherings were likely to delay the decline of cognitive function among women older than 65 (Table 2). Further, Wald tests on the interaction terms in the pooled samples revealed significant interaction effects based on gender (p = 0.027). Figure 1 presents the change in K-MMSE scores based on age among women who participated in social gatherings, which was estimated using the regression model.

4. Discussion

Using nationally representative longitudinal data for middle-aged and older Koreans, this study investigated the relationship between participation in different social activities and cognitive function by gender. The results indicate that social gatherings had a positive impact on preventing or delaying the decline of cognitive function, particularly among the older adults (over 65). One of the key findings is that the effect of social activities on cognitive function varied by gender, where women who participated in social gatherings were less likely to experience the decline of cognitive function when older than 65, whereas no effect of social gatherings on cognitive function was observed in men. Additionally, participation in social activities of any type had no impact on cognitive function in men, suggesting the need for a gender-stratified policy for preventing the decline of cognitive function while promoting engagement of social activities in the Korean older adults.
The findings from our study are consistent with previous studies suggesting that engaging in social activities in later life is more likely to prevent the decline of cognitive function at some level, and eventually reduce the risk of mental health problems and its related mortality [33,34,35]. Although the positive effects of greater participation in social activities on cognitive impairment in later life have mostly been observed, there are mixed findings on how different social activities impact cognitive function [8,17,34,36]. For instance, a previous study found that active volunteer work has a protective effect on cognitive function [36], whereas other studies suggested that active engagement in religious or social group activities contributes to preventing cognitive decline in both middle-aged and older adult populations [8,37,38]. In addition, a cross-sectional study examining the relationship between participation in social activities and MMSE decline among Korean middle-aged and older adult populations found that only religious and family-oriented activities were likely to delay cognitive function among individuals over 65, suggesting a community-based policy approach as one of the strategies to prevent and manage the increasing prevalence of dementia [37]. Studies from other Asian countries, such as China, Taiwan, and Japan, have also suggested that participation in activities associated with intellectually challenging activities and active interpersonal exchanges have positive effects on the prevention of cognitive decline regardless of biological aging [8,17,34]. Although the mechanism of how such activities decrease the risk of cognitive decline among the older adults is complex, ample evidence suggests that environments that provide social or cognitive interaction, resulting from greater participation in social activities, could be conducive to increasing cognitive reserve, thereby delaying the decline of cognitive function, regardless of biological aging [9,39]. Given the different nature of various social activities and cultures, one must be cautious in concluding that all types of social activities could be equally beneficial to cognitive function in the older adults.
Although there is no consensus on the possible mechanisms, there are possible explanations for the positive effects of social gatherings on cognitive decline in the Korean older adults. It has been suggested that psychological pathways might be involved in the beneficial effect on cognitive function [18]. Participation in friendship or leisure activity fulfills meaningful social roles, which could potentially sustain a person’s self-concept of worth and competence [40]. In this sense, our results, indicating social gatherings had beneficial effects on cognitive function, could be interpreted to mean that the older adults find socially meaningful roles or strengthen their self-efficacy through networking with friends and similar age group in community senior or social clubs.
In addition, older people who actively engage with social-tie activities might exhibit higher positive emotional states and less stressful daily life. Frequent interaction with close social ties generally leads to stress reduction and increased emotional stability, which is ultimately beneficial in preserving cognitive abilities compared with individuals with no interaction with others [41]. From a theoretical perspective, it is also possible that participation in social gatherings is associated with cognitive stimulations, contributing to the building of a cognitive reserve that ultimately optimizes its function [9,41]. For instance, activities involved with acquiring and organizing information, such as attending organization meetings and practicing an artistic or lecture activity, are associated with a reduced risk of dementia [9]. Therefore, continuous cognitive stimulation through involvement with such an activity might contribute to preserving cognitive decline, even enhancing cognitive reserve [9,41].
It is important to note why participation in other activities did not contribute to preserving cognitive decline. In fact, some studies suggest religious and volunteer activities also have positive relations with cognitive function. It is plausible that this conflicting result is attributed to the generally low levels of importance placed on religion in Korea compared with other countries, such as the U.S., where most studies on the relationship between religion and cognitive function were conducted. In Korea, religious activity is typically associated with personal belief and value, so religiosity may have smaller health effects than in countries where religious activity remains an important part of life [29,42]. Regarding volunteer work, the quality of volunteer work in Korea may not be sufficient to influence cognitive function for older-aged individuals [43]. Despite the increasing demand for volunteer work for older adults, systematic support and a formal compensation schemes are still lacking [29]. These limitations may hinder older adults in their attempt to engage more often in such activities and results in no effect on cognition. Several cross-sectional studies indicate that alumni and clan gathers are also beneficial to delaying cognitive decline; however, our results from a longitudinal analysis reveal no association. Most alumni and clan gatherings may be less cognitively demanding compared with other activities [44]. The frequency of social activity may also help to explain the conflicting results. For instance, participation in social gatherings (e.g., private saving or senior clubs), but alumni/clan gatherings are generally occasional events (e.g., semiannual or annual), which offer very limited influence on older adults’ cognitive function.
The effect of greater participation in such activities varied by gender. Although none of the social activities were associated with cognitive function, senior center-oriented activities were statistically associated with women only. Regarding the type of social participation, a previous study demonstrated that significant associations vary according to gender. Among females, participation in all social groups except for senior citizen clubs was inversely associated with a decline in efficacy, whereas among males, a beneficial effect on efficacy was restricted to hobby and volunteer groups [45]. Some studies have reported that social participation produces greater benefits for the health of males than females [46,47]. As they are more likely to achieve close relationships with their large and diverse networks compared with males, females tend to receive positive benefits from social participation, demonstrating that a constructive influence of social participation on efficacy is stronger for females compared with males [45]. One plausible explanation is that men are less likely to be involved in social interaction in later life given that marriage constitutes a large portion of their social life [48]. In contrast, women are often responsible for maintaining and fostering social activities after their counterparts retire and lose their social contact [49]. This given role in later life may lead women to participate in more activities, resulting in more benefits on cognitive function. Along with findings from previous studies, our study could help conclude that women of older ages need to be encouraged to be involved in certain types of social activities as part of healthy aging strategies. In addition, it is interesting to highlight that participation in social gatherings was effective to prevent cognitive function in older women, but not in younger women (under age 65). Because age is known to be a strong predictor of cognitive decline, it is possible that women under age 65 were less likely to experience cognitive decline compared to older women (over age 65). This helps explain why younger women may have no effect or limited effect of participation in social gatherings on cognitive preservation [17].
Our results indicate that social and health policy should be centered on encouraging social activities, including investing in physical and social infrastructure as part of older adults’ health policy intervention in terms of preventing cognitive decline and eventually preventing dementia. Although this study provides clear policy implications for the prevention of cognitive decline in Koreans older adults using a population-based longitudinal survey, there are some limitations that we have identified. The limitations of this study included the measures of social activity and health. Our measure of social activity did not contain information about the quality of these activities. In addition, data on health conditions were self-reported; therefore, it is possible that unmeasured health experiences potentially affected levels of memory and integration. We addressed this possibility by excluding baseline respondents with poor memory scores, but further studies with more intensive data collection on health status are required. We also identified the strengths of this study. Previous studies have linked social activities to lower cognitive abilities in later life [17,29,50]. These findings are mainly based on studies with a cross-sectional design or studies with short-term follow-up. These features limit the possibility of evaluating the longer-term associations between social activities and cognitive change and identifying some indication of the direction of the association, which results in excluding individuals interviewed at all data points, such as the deceased or dropouts, from the analysis. As these are the individuals most likely to experience cognitive impairment, this may lead to an overestimation of the beneficial impact of social activities. To overcome this identified limitation, our study adopted a longitudinal design with up to five measures of cognitive abilities over an eight-year period using a multi-level linear mixed model.

5. Conclusions

This study builds upon previous research by applying a more statistically sound approach, a multi-level linear mixed model, to examine how participation in social activities affects cognitive function in older Korean adults. This study found that MMSE scores decreased with increasing age, at a rate of approximately 0.18 units across all age-gender groups, and the decrease was steeper for adults aged 65 and over. Social gatherings were likely to delay the decline of cognitive function after the age of 65 by 0.079 units. In a gender-stratified model, the results demonstrated that social activity might not impact the decline of cognitive function in men. In women, participation in social gatherings was negatively related to the decline in MMSE scores, whereas baseline religious parties, alumni/clan gatherings, and volunteer work did not show significant relationships with the decline in MMSE scores. This finding could be interpreted as indicating that social gatherings are likely to delay the decline in cognitive function among women older than 65. This study suggests the need for a gender-stratified policy for preventing the decline in cognitive function while promoting engagement of social activities in the Korean older adults given that the effects of social gatherings on cognitive function differ based on gender.

Author Contributions

J.H., S.P., and S.K. conceptualized and designed the research; S.K. conducted data analysis; J.H., S.P., and S.K. wrote the manuscript; J.H. and S.K. edited and finalized the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Basic Science Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea funded by the Ministry of Education, Science and Technology [2016R1D1A1B03935971].

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. National Research Council Committee on Future Directions for Cognitive Research. The national academies collection: Reports funded by national institutes of health. In The Aging Mind: Opportunities in Cognitive Research; Stern, P.C., Carstensen, L.L., Eds.; National Academies Press (US)/National Academy of Sciences: Washington, DC, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  2. Wu, Y.T.; Prina, A.M.; Jones, A.P.; Barnes, L.E.; Matthews, F.E.; Brayne, C. Community environment, cognitive impairment and dementia in later life: Results from the cognitive function and ageing study. Age Ageing 2015, 44, 1005–1011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Johansson, M.M.; Marcusson, J.; Wressle, E. Cognitive impairment and its consequences in everyday life: Experiences of people with mild cognitive impairment or mild dementia and their relatives. Int. Psychogeriatrics 2015, 27, 949–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Deary, I.J.; Corley, J.; Gow, A.J.; Harris, S.E.; Houlihan, L.M.; Marioni, R.E.; Penke, L.; Rafnsson, S.B.; Starr, J.M. Age-associated cognitive decline. Br. Med. Bull. 2009, 92, 135–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Brayne, C. The elephant in the room—Healthy brains in later life, epidemiology and public health. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007, 8, 233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Marioni, R.E.; Proust-Lima, C.; Amieva, H.; Brayne, C.; Matthews, F.E.; Dartigues, J.-F.; Jacqmin-Gadda, H. Social activity, cognitive decline and dementia risk: A 20-year prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health 2015, 15, 1089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Bourassa, K.J.; Memel, M.; Woolverton, C.; Sbarra, D.A. Social participation predicts cognitive functioning in aging adults over time: Comparisons with physical health, depression, and physical activity. Aging Ment. Health 2017, 21, 133–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Fu, C.; Li, Z.; Mao, Z. Association between social activities and cognitive function among the elderly in china: A cross-sectional study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Kelly, M.E.; Duff, H.; Kelly, S.; McHugh Power, J.E.; Brennan, S.; Lawlor, B.A.; Loughrey, D.G. The impact of social activities, social networks, social support and social relationships on the cognitive functioning of healthy older adults: A systematic review. Syst. Rev. 2017, 6, 259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Charles, S.; Carstensen, L.L. Social and emotional aging. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2010, 61, 383–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Hsu, H.C. Does social participation by the elderly reduce mortality and cognitive impairment? Aging Ment. Health 2007, 11, 699–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. James, B.D.; Wilson, R.S.; Barnes, L.L.; Bennett, D.A. Late-life social activity and cognitive decline in old age. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 998–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Hughes, T.F. Promotion of cognitive health through cognitive activity in the aging population. Aging Health 2010, 6, 111–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Williams, K.; Kemper, S. Exploring interventions to reduce cognitive decline in aging. J. Psychosoc. Nurs. Ment. Health Serv. 2010, 48, 42–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Holtzman, R.E.; Rebok, G.W.; Saczynski, J.S.; Kouzis, A.C.; Wilcox Doyle, K.; Eaton, W.W. Social network characteristics and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2004, 59, 278–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Young Bum, K. Type of social activity and morale in later life : A comparison between male and female. Asian Women 2009, 25, 69–85. [Google Scholar]
  17. Lee, S.H.; Kim, Y.B. Which type of social activities may reduce cognitive decline in the elderly?: A longitudinal population-based study. BMC Geriatr. 2016, 16, 165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Tomioka, K.; Kurumatani, N.; Hosoi, H. Association between social participation and instrumental activities of daily living among community-dwelling older adults. J. Epidemiol. 2016, 26, 553–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Kåreholt, I.; Lennartsson, C.; Gatz, M.; Parker, M.G. Baseline leisure time activity and cognition more than two decades later. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2011, 26, 65–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Green, A.F.; Rebok, G.; Lyketsos, C.G. Influence of social network characteristics on cognition and functional status with aging. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008, 23, 972–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Amieva, H.; Stoykova, R.; Matharan, F.; Helmer, C.; Antonucci, T.C.; Dartigues, J.-F. What aspects of social network are protective for dementia? Not the quantity but the quality of social interactions is protective up to 15 years later. Psychosom. Med. 2010, 72, 905–911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Levasseur, M.; Richard, L.; Gauvin, L.; Raymond, É. Inventory and analysis of definitions of social participation found in the aging literature: Proposed taxonomy of social activities. Soc. Sci. Med. 2010, 71, 2141–2149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Hu, Y.; Lei, X.; Smith, J.; Zhao, Y. Effects of social activities on cognitive functions: Evidence from charls. RAND Work. Pap. Ser. 2012, 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Turner, A.D.; James, B.D.; Capuano, A.W.; Aggarwal, N.T.; Barnes, L.L. Perceived stress and cognitive decline in different cognitive domains in a cohort of older african americans. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2017, 25, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Cohen, S. Social relationships and health. Am. Psychol. 2004, 59, 676–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Fuhrer, R.; Stansfeld, S.A. How gender affects patterns of social relations and their impact on health: A comparison of one or multiple sources of support from “close persons”. Soc. Sci. Med. 2002, 54, 811–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Aida, J.; Kondo, K.; Kawachi, I.; Subramanian, S.; Ichida, Y.; Hirai, H.; Kondo, N.; Osaka, K.; Sheiham, A.; Tsakos, G.; et al. Does social capital affect the incidence of functional disability in older japanese? A prospective population-based cohort study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2013, 67, 42–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Norton, M.C.; Skoog, I.; Franklin, L.M.; Corcoran, C.; Tschanz, J.T.; Zandi, P.P.; Breitner, J.C.; Welsh-Bohmer, K.A.; Steffens, D.C. Gender differences in the association between religious involvement and depression: The cache county (utah) study. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2006, 61, 129–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. DaeHyun, K.; Hidenori, A.; SungHi, K. Social activities are associated with cognitive decline in older koreans. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2017, 17, 1191–1196. [Google Scholar]
  30. Korea Employment Information Service. User Guideline for Korean Logitudinal Study of Ageing Survey; Korea Employment Information Service: Chungchenogbuk-do, Korea, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  31. Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kang, Y.N.; Na, D.L.; Hahn, S.H. A validity study on the korean mini-mental state examination (k-mmse) in dementia patients. J. Korean Neurol. Assoc. 1997, 15, 300–308. [Google Scholar]
  33. Valenzuela, M.J.; Sachdev, P. Brain reserve and cognitive decline: A non-parametric systematic review. Psychol. Med. 2006, 36, 1065–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Valenzuela, M.; Brayne, C.; Sachdev, P.; Wilcock, G.; Fiona Matthews on Behalf of the Medical Research Council Cognitive, F.; Ageing, S. Cognitive lifestyle and long-term risk of dementia and survival after diagnosis in a multicenter population-based cohort. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2011, 173, 1004–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Marioni, R.E.; Valenzuela, M.J.; van den Hout, A.; Brayne, C.; Matthews, F.E.; Function, M.R.C.C.; Ageing, S. Active cognitive lifestyle is associated with positive cognitive health transitions and compression of morbidity from age sixty-five. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e50940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Griep, Y.; Hanson, L.M.; Vantilborgh, T.; Janssens, L.; Jones, S.K.; Hyde, M. Can volunteering in later life reduce the risk of dementia? A 5-year longitudinal study among volunteering and non-volunteering retired seniors. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0173885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Hill, T.D.; Burdette, A.M.; Angel, J.L.; Angel, R.J. Religious attendance and cognitive functioning among older mexican americans. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 2006, 61, 3–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Glei, D.A.; Landau, D.A.; Goldman, N.; Chuang, Y.-L.; Rodríguez, G.; Weinstein, M. Participating in social activities helps preserve cognitive function: An analysis of a longitudinal, population-based study of the elderly. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2005, 34, 864–871. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Hughes, T.F.; Ganguli, M. Modifiable midlife risk factors for late-life cognitive impairment and dementia. Curr. Psychiatry Rev. 2009, 5, 73–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Wang, H.-X.; Karp, A.; Winblad, B.; Fratiglioni, L. Late-life engagement in social and leisure activities is associated with a decreased risk of dementia: A longitudinal study from the kungsholmen project. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2002, 155, 1081–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Sakamoto, A.; Ukawa, S.; Okada, E.; Sasaki, S.; Zhao, W.; Kishi, T.; Kondo, K.; Tamakoshi, A. The association between social participation and cognitive function in community-dwelling older populations: Japan gerontological evaluation study at taisetsu community hokkaido. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2017, 32, 1131–1140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Ritchie, S.J.; Gow, A.J.; Deary, I.J. Religiosity is negatively associated with later-life intelligence, but not with age-related cognitive decline. Intelligence 2014, 46, 9–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Choi, Y.; Park, S.; Cho, K.H.; Chun, S.Y.; Park, E.C. A change in social activity affect cognitive function in middle-aged and older koreans: Analysis of a korean longitudinal study on aging (2006–2012). Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2016, 31, 912–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Su, X.; Huang, X.; Jin, Y.; Wan, S.; Han, Z. The relationship of individual social activity and cognitive function of community chinese elderly: A cross-sectional study. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2018, 14, 2149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Tomioka, K.; Kurumatani, N.; Hosoi, H. Social participation and the prevention of decline in effectance among community-dwelling elderly: A population-based cohort study. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0139065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Takagi, D.; Kondo, K.; Kawachi, I. Social participation and mental health: Moderating effects of gender, social role and rurality. BMC Public Health 2013, 13, 701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Kavanagh, A.M.; Bentley, R.; Turrell, G.; Broom, D.H.; Subramanian, S.V. Does gender modify associations between self rated health and the social and economic characteristics of local environments? J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2006, 60, 490–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Dykstra, P.A.; Fokkema, T. Social and emotional loneliness among divorced and married men and women: Comparing the deficit and cognitive perspectives. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 29, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Finkel, D.; Andel, R.; Pedersen, N.L. Gender differences in longitudinal trajectories of change in physical, social, and cognitive/sedentary leisure activities. J. Gerontol. Sér. B 2018, 73, 1491–1500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Hwang, J.; Kwon, S. The relation between the participation in social activity and cognitive function among middle-aged and elderly population. J. Korea Gerontol. Soc. 2009, 29, 971–986. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Change in K-MMSE over age based on participation in social gatherings among women.
Figure 1. Change in K-MMSE over age based on participation in social gatherings among women.
Ijerph 15 02315 g001
Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the sample from the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (KLOSA).
Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the sample from the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (KLOSA).
VariablesAllMaleFemale
Mean (SE) or %nMean (SE) or %nMean (SE) or %n
100670649.9334750.13359
Age 58.1 (0.12) 59.2 (0.17) 57.0 (0.16)
Social activityReligious partiesNo75.3504782.1274768.52300
Yes24.7165917.960031.51059
Social gatheringsNo37.4251035.3118039.61330
Yes62.6419664.7216760.42029
Alumni/clan gatheringsNo74.2497465217483.42800
Yes25.8173235117316.6559
Volunteer workNo95.4639795.5319595.33202
Yes4.63094.51524.7157
Marital statusMarried 86.2578192.9310979.52672
Single 13.89257.123820.5687
Working statusNo52.1349435.8119968.32295
Yes47.9321264.2214831.71064
Residence areaUrban 79.5533177.8260581.22726
Rural 20.5137522.274218.8633
EducationElementary school 7.65084.916310.3345
Middle school 25.316962066830.61028
High school 52.9354553.8180251.91743
College or above 14.395721.37147.2243
IncomeQ1 24.9166923.478226.4887
Q2 24.6165124.983324.4818
Q3 26.3176627.290925.5857
Q4 24.2162024.682323.7797
Physical activityYes44.4297945.5152443.31455
No55.6372754.5182356.71904
DrinkingNon-drinker 56.8380834.6115878.92650
Current drinker 43.2289865.4218921.1709
SmokingNever smoked 67.5452837.9126797.13261
Ever smoked 32.5217862.120802.998
Activities of daily living (ADL)None99663698.7330599.23331
1+1701.3420.828
DepressionNo76.1510179.4265772.82444
Yes23.9160520.669027.2915
ComorbidityNone69.3464968.5229270.22357
1+30.7205731.5105529..81002
K-MMSE Score2006 27.4 (0.02)670627.5 (0.03)334727.2 (0.03)3359
2008 25.7 (0.02)578625.8 (0.10)287625.7 (0.09)2910
2010 24.2 (0.02)536324.3 (0.12)264224.0 (0.12)2722
2012 25.1 (0.02)515925.3 (0.13)253025.0 (0.12)2629
2014 25.6 (0.02)472526.0 (0.08)227325.3 (0.09)2452
Table 2. Relationship between social activity and cognitive performance from the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (KLOSA), 2006–2014 (n = 6706).
Table 2. Relationship between social activity and cognitive performance from the Korean Longitudinal Study of Aging (KLOSA), 2006–2014 (n = 6706).
Variables *All (n = 6706)Male (n = 3359)Female (n = 3347)
Coeff.S.E.p-ValueCoeff.S.E.p-ValueCoeff.S.E.p-Value
Age-45−0.1770.014<0.001−0.1800.021<0.001−0.1740.019<0.001
Age-65−0.2190.026<0.001−0.1690.035<0.001−0.2700.038<0.001
Religious parties (RP)−0.0100.1790.957−0.0390.3160.902−0.0440.2180.841
(Age-45)·RP−0.0110.0140.435−0.0150.0230.507−0.0060.0170.718
(Age-65)·RP0.0040.0350.9200.0270.0530.6130.0000.0480.999
Social gatherings (SG)0.2920.1620.0720.2880.2560.2600.3050.2080.143
(Age-45)·SG−0.0190.0120.125−0.0060.0190.772−0.0320.0170.052
(Age-65)·SG0.0790.0310.0100.0400.0420.3410.1180.0460.011
Alumni/clan gatherings (AG)−0.1570.1670.346−0.2470.2440.311−0.0390.2430.874
(Age-45)·AG0.0010.0130.9660.0080.0190.681−0.0100.0220.637
(Age-65)·AG0.0490.0380.1920.0170.0450.7110.0890.0790.258
Volunteer work (VW)0.1720.3340.6070.0820.5210.8760.1880.4330.665
(Age-45)·VW0.0160.0270.5560.0240.0400.5560.0140.0370.703
(Age-65)·VW0.0230.0840.782−0.0400.1100.7180.1270.1310.333
Sex0.1570.1040.131
Age0.1740.010<0.0010.1630.015<0.0010.1800.014<0.001
Marital Status0.2060.1130.0670.1510.2060.4650.2900.1380.035
Work Status0.0010.0800.992−0.0170.1310.8970.0250.1040.808
Residence (Rural)0.1560.0880.0760.1320.1270.2970.1800.1230.144
Education-Elementary−1.9240.191<0.001−2.0490.310<0.001−1.7050.266<0.001
Education-Middle school−0.9550.132<0.001−1.0070.181<0.001−0.8210.208<0.001
Education-High school−0.2880.1050.006−0.4070.1320.002−0.1060.1820.559
Income-Q1−0.4480.105<0.001−0.4760.1590.003−0.4200.1400.003
Income-Q2−0.3190.1020.002−0.2610.1510.083−0.3630.1400.010
Income-Q3−0.0140.0940.882−0.0440.1360.7450.0290.1300.826
Physical Activity (No)0.0000.0720.9990.0820.1050.438−0.0930.1000.353
Drinking (Current drinkers)−0.0240.0790.7600.0160.1110.882−0.0760.1140.503
Smoking (Ever smoked)−0.0150.0980.8770.0300.1060.775−0.5190.2930.077
ADL−0.5180.117<0.001−0.5470.127<0.001−0.3210.3070.297
Depression (Yes)−0.4230.085<0.001−0.5320.133<0.001−0.3320.1120.003
Comorbidity−0.2400.0800.003−0.2690.1140.018−0.1960.1140.086
* All covariates except for Age-45 and Age-65 were measured at baseline. Dependent variable (K-MMSE), Age-45, and Age-65 were measured at each wave.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hwang, J.; Park, S.; Kim, S. Effects of Participation in Social Activities on Cognitive Function Among Middle-Aged and Older Adults in Korea. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 2315. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15102315

AMA Style

Hwang J, Park S, Kim S. Effects of Participation in Social Activities on Cognitive Function Among Middle-Aged and Older Adults in Korea. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018; 15(10):2315. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15102315

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hwang, Jongnam, Sangmin Park, and Sujin Kim. 2018. "Effects of Participation in Social Activities on Cognitive Function Among Middle-Aged and Older Adults in Korea" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 15, no. 10: 2315. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph15102315

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop