Next Article in Journal
A Study on the Cause Analysis of Cyberbullying in Korean Adolescents
Previous Article in Journal
Estimation of Soil Organic Carbon Storage in Palustrine Wetlands, China
Previous Article in Special Issue
Sleep Quality and Psychological Status in a Group of Italian Prisoners
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Shift Work and Neurocognitive Function among Firefighters in South Korea: A Prospective before–after Study

1
Department of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, Korea University Ansan Hospital, Ansan 15355, Korea
2
Department of Environmental Sciences, Seoul National University Graduate School of Public Health, Seoul 08826, Korea
3
Department of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, Daewoo Hospital, Geoje 53317, Korea
4
Department of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, Hanyang University College of Medicine, Seoul 04763, Korea
5
Department of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, Ulsan University Hospital, University of Ulsan College of Medicine, Ulsan 44033, Korea
6
Department of Preventive Medicine, Yonsei University Wonju College of Medicine, Wonju 26426, Korea
7
Institute of Genomic Cohort, Yonsei University Wonju College of Medicine, Wonju 26426, Korea
8
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Eulji University School of Medicine, Daejeon 34824, Korea
9
Department of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, Wonju Severance Christian Hospital, Wonju 26426, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(13), 4647; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph17134647
Submission received: 20 May 2020 / Revised: 19 June 2020 / Accepted: 22 June 2020 / Published: 28 June 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Cognitive and Emotional Effects of Sleep Deprivation)

Abstract

:
Background: Recent research indicates that shift work is associated with neurocognitive function. However, studies that examine the association between shift work and neurocognitive function in firefighters have not yet been performed. We examined the effect of shift work on neurocognitive function in firefighters by measuring and comparing neurocognitive function before and after night shift. Methods: 352 firefighters from eight fire stations in South Korea were included in this study. We performed neurocognitive function test using central nervous system vital signs (CNSVS) during daytime work and on the next day after night work. We performed paired t-tests to assess differences between neurocognitive function before and after night work. We also compared neurocognitive function in insomnia and depression. We used a general linear model to analyze the associations between shiftwork schedule and the changes in neurocognitive function. Results: The neurocognitive function significantly decreased in six domains (composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, complex attention, psychomotor speed, and motor speed) as did the neurocognitive index on the next day after night work compared with during day work. These decreased domains were the same following night work regardless of the type of shift work. Conclusion: Night work in firefighters may cause neurocognitive decline.

1. Introduction

Firefighters in South Korea are responsible not only for fire suppression but also for various accident-related rescue and emergency activities and preparation for large-scale disasters and on-site response [1]. Firefighters have an increased risk of exposure to toxic gases (e.g., carbon monoxide and phosgene) and heat stress at fire sites [2]. Firefighters also suffer from high levels of job stress due to the mental tension caused by waiting time in the field and the sleep chronic deprivation caused by shift work. In fact, firefighters have higher risks of psychological problems, such as depression [3] and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [4,5,6] compared with the general population. Many firefighters suffer from sleep disorders due to exposure to shift work [7,8].
These risk factors in firefighters may also be related to neurocognitive function. Both the heat stress in a hot environment [9,10,11] and the exposure to higher stress levels [12,13] can affect neurocognitive function. Firefighters are at high-risk of PTSD [14] and depression [15,16,17], and study results indicate that these conditions are associated with cognitive impairment. In particular, results of recent studies suggest that shift work can also affect neurocognitive function [18,19,20,21]. Shift work-associated disruptions in circadian rhythms are associated with neurodegeneration [22,23]. In support of this hypothesis, one animal model experiment found that deletion of the master clock gene Bmal1 in the mouse brain results in an increase in neuronal oxidative damage [24]. A firefighter who is engaged in long-term shift work may be at risk of cognitive decline. An experimental study performed by Rodrigues et al. found that cognitive performance in firefighters decreases after exposure to increased levels of stress [25]. However, the effects of shift work on neurocognitive function in firefighters have not yet been studied.
This study aimed to investigate the effects of shift work on neurocognitive function in firefighters by measuring and comparing cognitive functions before and after they worked the night shift.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Subjects

This study was included in the SLEep Panel Study (SLEPS). The purpose of SLEPS was to investigate sleep problems among Korean firefighters. Using the two means formula for continuous variables, we calculated the sample size before conducting the SLEPS. The calculation was carried out using G*power version 3.1.9.7. When the α error was set to 0.05 and the statistical power (1–β error) to 80%, the calculated sample size was 124. In order to perform a stratified analysis in respect to sex, age group, department, job position, and shift work schedule, a sample size of 8928 was required; however, the final targeted sample size needed to be determined considering the budget constraint and time needed to construct the panel. A final target sample size of 500 was decided upon, and 516 firefighters were recruited to participate in the SLEPS. From 2017 to 2018, we conducted the questionnaire survey on a panel of 516 firefighters from eight fire stations in South Korea and performed neurocognitive function tests. Among the 516 firefighters in the panel, 88 who worked only in the daytime (i.e., no night shift work) were excluded from the analysis. We also excluded 68 firefighters who missed at least one of the neurocognitive function tests during daytime work or after nighttime work. Two firefighters being treated for insomnia by a physician were excluded because this condition could affect the results of neurocognitive function tests. Six firefighters who did not complete the insomnia or depression questionnaires were also excluded from the study. Finally, the data from 352 subjects who were currently working on a night shift schedule were included in the analysis (Figure 1).

2.2. Type of Shift Work

Firefighters in South Korea are typically subjected to 3-, 6-, 9-, or 21-day cycle shift work. In this study, the 3-day cycle shift work schedule consisted of a full day (24 h) of work, followed by 2 days off-duty. The 6-day cycle shift work schedule consisted of 2 days of daytime work (9:00 a.m.–6:00 p.m.), followed by 2 days of nighttime work (6:00 p.m.–9:00 a.m.) and 2 days off-duty. The 9-day cycle shift work schedule consisted of 3 days of daytime work (9:00 a.m.–6:00 p.m.), followed by three consecutive sets of nighttime work and off-duty. The 21-day schedule consisted of 5 days of daytime work (9:00 a.m.–6:00 p.m.), followed in order by 2 days off-duty, three consecutive sets of nighttime work and off-duty, a full day (24 h) of work, two consecutive sets of off-duty and nighttime work, 1 day off-duty, a full day (24 h) of work, and 1 day off-duty (Figure 2).

2.3. Questionnaire

During the panel survey, each subject was asked to complete a self-reported questionnaire. Data on age, sex, department and job position, monthly income, education level, task, type of shift work, history of illness, family medical history, history of smoking and drinking, physical activity, and symptoms related to the target organs were collected.

2.4. Sleep Disorder and Depression Evaluation Tools

The Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) [26,27] was used to measure insomnia. The ISI is a self-reported questionnaire that consists of seven questions used to evaluate degrees of difficult sleep onset and sleep maintenance, satisfaction with current sleep patterns, interference with daily functioning, noticeability of impairments attributed to sleep problems, and degrees of distress or concern caused by the sleep problem [28]. Each question was scored between 0 and 4. A higher score indicated a more severe status. An ISI score ≥8 indicated the presence of a mild sleep disorder and an ISI score ≥15 indicated the presence of sleep disorder [29]. Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) [30] was used to evaluate each subject’s depressive symptoms. Each item scores between 0 and 3 points for 9 depressive symptoms during the last 2 weeks. A PHQ-9 score ≥5 indicated the presence of mild depression and a score ≥10 indicated the presence of depression [31]. The Korean versions of the ISI and PHQ-9 have been validated by past studies [32,33].

2.5. Neurocognitive Function Testing

Neurocognitive function testing was based on the use of central nervous system vital signs (CNSVS). The CNSVS is a computerized neurocognitive test battery that was developed as a screening instrument for neurocognitive impairment; test validity and reliability have been verified [34]. CNSVS calculates standardized scores for the domains of composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, complex attention, psychomotor speed, motor speed, processing speed, reaction time, cognitive flexibility, executive functioning, and neurocognitive index [35]. The standardized CNSVS score is calculated via a process of data standardization; the data are stratified by 10 age groups, and an average value of 100 and standard deviation of 15 are used.
To evaluate the change in scores due to shift work, the CNSVS assessments were performed during daytime work and on the next day after nighttime work. The measurement of daytime work was performed on the last day of the daytime work; it was measured as firefighters went to work. Measurement of the nighttime work was performed once on the next day after the second or third nighttime work shift, and on the next day after the full day of work. Since the measurement of the nighttime work occurred as night shift was over, both measurements of the daytime and nighttime work were taken between 9 a.m. and 10 a.m.
The CNSVS test was designed to be given within a 30-min period to increase the probability of subject compliance. To avoid the possible effects of differences between computers, all tests were performed using the same kind of computers.

2.6. Study Endpoints

The primary endpoint was a change in neurocognitive function after night shift; the secondary endpoint was a change in neurocognitive function after night shift according to status of depression and insomnia. The exploratory endpoint was whether there was a difference in cognitive function changes depending on the type of shift work.

2.7. Statistical Analysis

We performed descriptive statistical analyses for demographic and socioeconomic characteristics, and health characteristics (lifestyle habits, and status of insomnia and depression symptoms). Among the socioeconomic characteristics, monthly income was divided into high, middle, and low with 3 million KRW (2,500 USD) and 5 million KRW (4,167 USD), respectively, separating the categories. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) and chi-square tests were used to assess differences in these general characteristics between types of shift work. Paired t-tests were used to assess differences between the standard scores measured during daytime work and on the next day after nighttime work for each category of the CNSVS assessment. Paired t-tests were also performed using stratification according to the subcategories within the insomnia and depressive symptoms categories. In the stratification analysis for depressive symptoms, mild depression was included in the depression category because the number of firefighters classified as having depression was small. We used a general linear model (GLM) to analyze the association between type of shift work and the change in CNSVS score and identified whether there were differences in CNSVS score changes during daytime work and on the next day after nighttime work, according to type of shift work. To adjust for confounding variables, we used four GLM models with different variables and calculated the least square mean (LSmean) value for each type of shift work for each category of CNSVS. Model 1 did not adjust other variables. Model 2 adjusted sex, age, and educational level. Model 3 included the Model 2 adjusted variables and further adjusted the job and income variables, and the ISI/PHQ-9 scores. Multiple comparisons were also performed to determine whether the LSmean values among the types of shift work were significantly different. All analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA).

2.8. Ethics Statement

This study was performed after obtaining approval from the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of Yonsei University Wonju Severance Christian Hospital (IRB no. CR318031).

3. Results

3.1. General Characteristics of Subjects

The 3-day cycle shift group was the oldest; a significant difference in the proportion of age groups was present across different shift groups. There were no between-group differences in sex ratios. There were statistically significant differences between the groups in socioeconomic status, such as income, highest level of education, and proportion of fire suppression work. Coffee consumption was also significantly different between groups. Health behaviors such as smoking, drinking, and exercise did not significantly differ between groups. There were no statistically significant between-group differences in mean scores of ISI and PHQ-9 scores for insomnia and depression. The ISI and PHQ-9 results indicated that there were no statistically significant differences in the proportion of insomnia and depression status among the groups of different shift work schedules (Table 1).

3.2. Neurocognitive Function

The results of neurocognitive function testing measured using CNSVS assessment indicated that the neurocognitive scores decreased on the next day after nighttime work, compared with during the daytime work, for all domains except reaction time and executive function. The scores for composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, complex attention, psychomotor speed, motor speed, and neurocognitive index were significantly decreased on the next day after nighttime work (Table 2).

3.3. Stratifying Analysis Based on the ISI

We compared neurocognitive function during daytime work and on the next day after nighttime work by stratifying according to the ISI questionnaire results for insomnia. The composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, and neurocognitive index in the normal group were significantly decreased on the next day after nighttime work. In the mild insomnia group, the composite memory, verbal memory, complex attention, psychomotor speed, motor speed, and neurocognitive index were significantly decreased on the day after night shift work. In the insomnia group, composite memory, verbal memory, and motor speed were significantly decreased on the next day after nighttime work. Composite memory and verbal memory were significantly decreased in all groups, regardless of insomnia status. Complex attention, psychomotor speed, and motor speed were not significantly different in the normal group but were significantly decreased on the next day after nighttime work in the mild insomnia group (Table 3).

3.4. Stratifying Analysis Based on the PHQ-9

We compared neurocognitive function during daytime work and on the next day after nighttime work; we stratified according to the PHQ-9 results for symptoms of depression. In both the normal group and the depression group including those with mild depressive symptoms, composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, complex attention, psychomotor speed, motor speed, and neurocognitive index were significantly decreased on the next day after nighttime work. There was no difference in the domain in which the neurocognitive functions were significantly decreased between normal and depression group (Table 4).

3.5. Multivariate Analysis for the Changes of Neurocognitive Function

The GLM analysis found that in the unadjusted model, type of shift work was significantly related to the domains of verbal memory, processing speed, cognitive flexibility, and executive functioning. However, the analyses using Model 2 and Model 3, which were adjusted for confounding variables, found that type of shift work was not significantly associated with verbal memory domain. The statistical significance of all domains, except verbal memory, was maintained after adjusting for confounding variables. The composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, complex attention, psychomotor speed, motor speed, and neurocognitive index domains, which showed statistically significant neurocognitive function decreases before and after nighttime work, were not significantly associated with type of shift work (Table 5).
Among the variables that were used as covariates other than the type of shift work, age was significantly related to neurocognitive function after nighttime work in the domain of retention time. The older firefighters had less change in neurocognitive function. The PHQ-9 score was significantly associated with neurocognitive function after nighttime work for the psychomotor speed and processing speed domains. As PHQ-9 scores increased, neurocognitive function after nighttime work was significantly more decreased. ISI score was significantly associated with changes in neurocognitive function after nighttime work for the psychomotor speed domain. However, as the ISI score increased, the change in neurocognitive function after nighttime work was significantly decreased contrary to the PHQ-9 score.

4. Discussion

The results of the neurocognitive function testing using the CNSVS assessment showed a significant decrease on the next day after nighttime work compared with during daytime work in the scores for six domains (composite memory, verbal memory, visual memory, complex attention, psychomotor speed, and motor speed) and the score of neurocognitive index, respectively. There was no statistically significant improvement for any domain after nighttime work.
In the multivariate analysis using GLM, the changes in neurocognitive function were associated with the type of shift work for the processing speed, cognitive flexibility, and executive functioning domains. The neurocognitive index and the domains with significant decreases after nighttime work did not show significant associations between the type of shift work and neurocognitive function change. In other words, the results confirmed that the decreases in significant neurocognitive function scores after nighttime work were the same regardless of the type of shift work.
When the data were stratified according to degree of insomnia, we found that the domains of complex attention, psychomotor speed, and motor speed score, which were not significantly decreased after nighttime work in the normal group, were significantly decreased in the mild insomnia group. Because there were differences in only some domains and there were no differences in the insomnia group except for the domain of motor speed, the generalizability of the results is limited. However, there were only 32 firefighters classified as having insomnia, so the statistical power required to identify differences was lacking. Therefore, taken together, these results suggest that even mild insomnia made the firefighters more vulnerable to the effects of nighttime work. Greater reductions in neurocognitive function were likely possible in these firefighters, compared with those without insomnia.
A firefighter’s sleep during the night shift can be disturbed by fire and emergency calls. These events may lead to sleep deprivation, which in turn can lead to poor short-term memory, slow response times, lapses in attention or concentration, and mood changes [36]. Several studies comparing the results of functional imaging of the brain for the sleep-deprived brain versus the well-rested brain have consistently found decreases in working memory in the sleep-deprived brain [37]. The results of cognitive and motor performance tests of participants with moderate sleep deprivation are affected not only by cognitive function but also by motor speed, accuracy, coordination, and attention [38]. In this study, the functions of memory domains such as composite memory, verbal memory, and visual memory, and domains in complex attention, psychomotor speed, and motor speed, were decreased after nighttime work. These changes may have been due to the sleep deprivation caused by nighttime work, resulting in a decrease in attention, and its effects on working memory, short-term memory, and motor function.
Firefighters repeatedly experience this sleep deprivation during long and repeated shift work cycles. Chronic sleep deprivation affects neurobehavioral function and shows a dose–response relationship [39]. Chronic sleep deprivation also induces changes in brain metabolism and neural activation [40]. Therefore, the deterioration in cognitive function due to the sleep deprivation of firefighters is not a one-time change; it constantly affects the brain and nervous system of firefighters and decreases the individual’s neurobehavioral function. This change increases the risk of accidents for firefighters during fire suppression activities [41] and might be a risk factor for the development of central neurological diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease in the long term [42].
In firefighters, sleep deprivation due to night work increased level of stress. A study on Dutch police officers revealed that the level of salivary cortisol was significantly increased between 4–14 months after the transition from regular day work to rotating shift work [43]. In addition, a study on German physicians reported that the level of salivary cortisol was significantly increased compared to that of who did not work night shifts [44]. Since a cortisol response is known to be associated with work stress, it can be interpreted that stress level has increased due to shift work [45,46]. Stressful conditions can induce the activation of the neuroendocrine stress system and epigenetic changes in brain, which can drive neuroplastic changes in emotional and cognitive functions [47]. Thus, it could be understood that stress induced by sleep deprivation might have influenced neurocognitive decline in firefighters after a night shift.
Previous studies of neurocognitive function changes in firefighters mainly assessed the effects of high temperatures. There is a concern that firefighters involved in fire suppression activities are exposed to high temperature environments for long periods and that the physical factors could affect neurocognitive function. Recently, an experimental study of U.S. firefighters responsible for wildland fire suppression found that there is no significant difference in the results of cognitive function assessments in very hot (45 °C) conditions compared to temperate environment (18 °C) conditions [48]. Morley et al. [49] performed a neurocognitive function test immediately after giving a heat stress similar to that of a fire suppression; they exposed the firefighters to a 50-min continuous treadmill exercise while wearing thermal protective clothing. The neurocognitive test scores did not change immediately after exercise. Our study, and these previous studies, found that firefighters in charge of fire suppression did not differ in changes in neurocognitive function test scores compared to other firefighters.
Depression, as well as sleep, can also affect a firefighter’s neurocognitive function. In previous studies, depression has been consistently associated with cognitive decline [50,51]. Firefighters also have many risk factors for depression, such as physical environment, organizational culture, and job stress [3]. Although the prevalence of depression in firefighters is not precisely known, several epidemiological studies reported high rates of depressive symptoms among firefighters [52,53]. In this study, after the other factors were adjusted for using multivariate analysis, depression was significantly related to the score changes after nighttime work for the psychomotor speed and motor speed domains. There was no difference in cognitive function before and after nighttime work between the normal and depression groups. However, only 54 firefighters were classified as having depression by PHQ-9, even if mild depression was included in the depression category. Therefore, the number of subjects was too small to confirm the difference, and further studies are needed.
In addition, measures to prevent and manage a cognitive decline due to shift work are needed. Prevention and treatment for sleep disorders are essential in reducing cognitive decline. Administrative management including the adjustment of a shift work cycle and rotating work with day shifts helps to prevent sleep disorders caused by shift work. In addition, maintenance of a sound personal sleep hygiene is also crucial at the individual level. Non-pharmacological therapies are also useful, especially for firefighters with sleep disorders. It has been reported that melatonin improved sleep efficiency [54,55]; taking a melatonin supplement or eating foods rich in melatonin might assist sleeping in firefighters on a night shift. In addition, cognitive behavioral therapy could improve sleep quality in firefighters with sleep disorders. A proper use of these non-pharmacological therapies will help to prevent cognitive decline due to sleep deprivation [56,57,58].
This study had several limitations. The CNSVS testing was conducted directly during the daytime and immediately after the night shift, but most of the tests were performed only once or twice. It was not followed up longitudinally. In addition, the specific firefighting activities at the nighttime fire scenes, which occurred before measurements were taken, are important factors that affect cognitive function, but no detailed records and observations of these activities were available. These activities likely acted as unmeasured confounders and resulted in bias. Furthermore, although this study included firefighters involved in various occupations and from many fire stations, the numbers of participants available for the stratified analyses were relatively small. The statistical power was low when the effects of stratification were analyzed according to PHQ-9 category because only a small number of firefighters was classified as having depression. To overcome these limitations, a longitudinal follow-up study involving more participants is needed in the future.

5. Conclusions

We compared neurocognitive function using CNSVS testing before and after nighttime work performed by firefighters in South Korea who were working in shifts. We found that after nighttime work, neurocognitive functions were generally lower, compared with those during daytime work. In particular, neurocognitive function was decreased for the memory and attention domains; and psychomotor and motor function were also decreased. This result is consistent with the effects of sleep deprivation found by previous studies. Therefore, the results of this study suggested that working the night shift in firefighters is a risk factor for neurocognitive decline.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, K.S.J.; methodology, K.K., B.-K.K., C.S.S., Y.-S.A., K.-S.C., and K.S.J.; software, K.K.; validation, K.K. and T.-W.J.; formal analysis, K.K., B.-K.K., and K.S.J.; investigation, T.-W.J., C.S.S., Y.-S.A., K.-S.C., and K.S.J.; resources, Y.-S.A.; data curation, K.K.; writing—original draft preparation, K.K.; writing—review and editing, B.-K.K., T.-W.J., C.S.S., Y.-S.A., K.-S.C., and K.S.J.; visualization, K.-S.C.; supervision, K.S.J.; project administration, Y.-S.A.; funding acquisition, Y.-S.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Korean National Fire Agency, grant number 2017-NFA001-010-01010000-2020.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the firefighters who participated in this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Lee, N.; Kim, J.H.; Kim, J.Y.; Kim, S.S. Association between workplace discrimination and depressive symptoms among firefighters in South Korea. Am. J. Ind. Med. 2018, 61, 741–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Guidotti, T.L.; Clough, V.M. Occupational health concerns of firefighting. Annu. Rev. Public Health 1992, 13, 151–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. An, S.J.; Chung, Y.K.; Kim, B.H.; Kwak, K.M.; Son, J.S.; Koo, J.; Ju, Y.S.; Kwon, Y.J. The effect of organisational system on self-rated depression in a panel of male municipal firefighters. Ann. Occup. Envrion. Med. 2015, 27, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Sakuma, A.; Takahashi, Y.; Ueda, I.; Sato, H.; Katsura, M.; Abe, M.; Nagao, A.; Suzuki, Y.; Kakizaki, M.; Tsuji, I.; et al. Post-traumatic stress disorder and depression prevalence and associated risk factors among local disaster relief and reconstruction workers fourteen months after the Great East Japan Earthquake: A cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry 2015, 15, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Berninger, A.; Webber, M.P.; Niles, J.K.; Gustave, J.; Lee, R.; Cohen, H.W.; Kelly, K.; Corrigan, M.; Prezant, D.J. Longitudinal study of probable post-traumatic stress disorder in firefighters exposed to the World Trade Center disaster. Am. J. Ind. Med. 2010, 53, 1177–1185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Del Ben, K.S.; Scotti, J.R.; Chen, Y.C.; Fortson, B.L. Prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in firefighters. Work Stress 2006, 20, 37–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Carey, M.G.; Al-Zaiti, S.S.; Dean, G.E.; Sessanna, L.; Finnell, D.S. Sleep problems, depression, substance use, social bonding, and quality of life in professional firefighters. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2011, 53, 928–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Lim, D.K.; Baek, K.O.; Chung, I.S.; Lee, M.Y. Factors related to sleep disorders among male firefighters. Ann. Occup. Environ. Med. 2014, 26, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Mazloumi, A.; Golbabaei, F.; Khani, S.M.; Kazemi, Z.; Hosseini, M.; Abbasinia, M.; Dehghan, S.F. Evaluating effects of heat stress on cognitive function among workers in a hot industry. Health Promot. Perspect. 2014, 4, 240–246. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  10. Hancock, P.A.; Vasmatzidis, I. Effects of heat stress on cognitive performance: The current state of knowledge. Int. J. Hyperthermia 2003, 19, 355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Gaoua, A. Cognitive function in hot environments: A question of methodology. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2010, 20, 60–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. McEwen, B.S.; Sapolsky, R.M. Stress and cognitive function. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 1995, 5, 205–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Marin, M.F.; Lord, C.; Andrews, J.; Juster, R.P.; Sindi, S.; Arsenault-Lapierre, G.; Fiocco, A.J.; Lupien, S.J. Chronic stress, cognitive functioning and mental health. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2011, 96, 583–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Barrett, D.H.; Green, M.L.; Morris, R.; Giles, W.H.; Croft, J.B. Cognitive functioning and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 1996, 153, 1492–1494. [Google Scholar]
  15. Austin, M.P.; Ross, M.; Murray, C.; O’Caŕroll, R.E.; Ebmeier, K.P.; Goodwin, G.M. Cognitive function in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 1992, 25, 21–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. McDermott, L.M.; Ebmeier, K.P. A meta-analysis of depression severity and cognitive function. J. Affect. Disord. 2009, 119, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rock, P.L.; Roiser, J.P.; Riedel, W.J.; Blackwell, A.D. Cognitive impairment in depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 2014, 44, 2029–2040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Titova, O.E.; Lindberg, E.; Elmståhl, S.; Lind, L.; Schiöth, H.B.; Benedict, C. Association between shift work history and performance on the trail making test in middle-aged and elderly humans: The EpiHealth study. Neurobiol. Aging 2016, 45, 23–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ansiau, D.; Wild, P.; Niezborala, M.; Rouch, I.; Marquie, J.C. Effects of working conditions and sleep of the previous day on cognitive performance. Appl. Ergon. 2008, 39, 99–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Machi, M.S.; Staum, M.; Callaway, C.W.; Moore, C.; Jeong, K.; Suyama, J.; Patterson, P.D.; Hostler, D. The relationship between shift work, sleep, and cognition in career emergency physician. Acad. Emerg. Med. 2012, 19, 85–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Marquie, J.C.; Tucker, P.; Folkard, S.; Gentil, C.; Ansiau, D. Chronic effects of shift work on cognition: Findings from the VISAT longitudinal study. Occup. Environ. Med. 2015, 72, 258–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Kondratova, A.A.; Kondratov, R.V. The circadian clock and pathology of the ageing brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012, 13, 325–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Wulff, K.; Gatti, S.; Wettstein, J.G.; Foster, R.G. Sleep and circadian rhythm disruption in psychiatric and neurodegenerative disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 589–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Musiek, E.S.; Lim, M.M.; Yang, G.; Bauer, A.Q.; Qi, L.; Lee, Y.; Roh, J.H.; Gonzalez, X.O.; Dearborn, J.T.; Culver, J.P.; et al. Circadian clock proteins regulate neuronal redox homeostasis and neurodegeneration. J. Clin. Investig. 2013, 123, 5389–5400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Rodrigues, S.; Paiva, J.S.; Dias, D.; Pimentel, G.; Kaiseler, M.; Cunha, J.P.S. Wearable Biomonitoring Platform for the Assessment of Stress and its Impact on Cognitive Performance of Firefighters: An Experimental Study. Clin. Pract. Epidemiol. Ment. Health 2018, 14, 250–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Bastien, C.H.; Vallières, A.; Morin, C.M. Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2001, 2, 297–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ensrud, K.E.; Joffe, H.; Guthrie, K.A.; Larson, J.C.; Reed, S.D.; Newton, K.M.; Sternfeld, B.; LaCroix, A.Z.; Landis, C.A.; Woods, N.F.; et al. Effect of escitalopram on insomnia symptoms and subjective sleep quality in healthy menopausal women with hot flashes: A randomized controlled trial. Menopause 2012, 19, 848–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Kwak, K.M.; Ju, Y.S.; Kwon, Y.J.; Chung, Y.K.; Kim, B.K.; Kim, H.; Youn, K. The effect of aircraft noise on sleep disturbance among the residents near a civilian airport: A cross-sectional study. Ann. Occup. Environ. Med. 2016, 28, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Morin, C.M.; Colecchi, C.; Stone, J.; Sood, R.M.; Brink, D. Behavioral and pharmacological therapies for late-life insomnia: A randomised controlled trial. JAMA 1999, 281, 991–999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Spitzer, R.L.; Kroenke, K.; Williams, J.B.W. Patient Health Questionnaire Study Group. Validity and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD: The PHQ Primary Care Study. JAMA 1999, 282, 1737–1744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Kroenke, K.; Spitzer, R.L.; Williams, J.B. The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. J. Gen. Intern Med. 2001, 16, 606–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Cho, Y.W.; Song, M.L.; Morin, C.M. Validation of a Korean Version of the Insomnia Severity Index. J. Clin. Neurol. 2014, 10, 210–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Han, C.; Jo, S.A.; Kwak, J.H.; Pae, C.U.; Steffens, D.; Jo, I.; Park, M.H. Validation of the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 Korean version in the elderly population: The Ansan Geriatric study. Compr. Psychiatry 2008, 49, 218–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Gualtieri, C.T.; Johnson, L.G. Reliability and validity of a computerized neurocognitive test battery, CNS Vital Signs. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2006, 21, 623–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. CNS Vital Signs. CNS Vital Signs Brief Interpretation Guide. Available online: https://www.cnsvs.com/WhitePapers/CNSVS-BriefInterpretationGuide.pdf (accessed on 19 July 2019).
  36. Bonnet, M.; Arand, D. Clinical effects of sleep fragmentation versus sleep deprivation. Sleep Med. Rev. 2003, 7, 297–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Chee, M.W.L.; Chuah, L.Y.M. Functional neuroimaging insights into how sleep and sleep deprivation affect memory and cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2008, 21, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Williamson, A.M.; Feyer, A.M. Moderate sleep deprivation produces impairments in cognitive and motor performance equivalent to legally prescribed levels of alcohol intoxication. Occup. Environ. Med. 2000, 57, 649–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Van Dongen, H.; Maislin, G.; Mullington, J.M.; Dinges, D.F. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: Dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep 2003, 26, 117–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Basner, M.; Rao, H.; Goel, N.; Dinges, D.F. Sleep deprivation and neurobehavioral dynamics. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2013, 23, 854–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Melamed, S.; Oksenberg, A. Excessive daytime sleepiness and risk of occupational injuries in non-shift daytime workers. Sleep 2002, 25, 315–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Geerlings, M.I.; Bouter, L.M.; Schoevers, R.A.; Beekman, A.T.F.; Jonker, C.; Deeg, D.J.H.; Van Tilburg, W.; Adèr, H.J.; Schmand, B. Depression and risk of cognitive decline and Alzheimer’s disease: Results of two prospective community-based studies in The Netherlands. Br. J. Psychiatry 2000, 176, 568–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Lammers-van der Holst, H.M.; Kerkhof, G.A. Individual differences in the cortisol-awakening response during the first two years of shift work: A longitudinal study in novice police officers. Chronobiol. Int. 2015, 32, 1162–1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Li, J.; Bidlingmaier, M.; Petru, R.; Gil, F.P.; Loerbroks, A.; Angerer, P. Impact of shift work on the diurnal cortisol rhythm: A one-year longitudinal study in junior physicians. J. Occup. Med. Toxicol. 2018, 13, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Chida, Y.; Steptoe, A. Cortisol awakening response and psychosocial factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol. Psychol. 2009, 80, 265–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Clow, A.; Hucklebridge, F.; Stalder, T.; Evans, P.; Thorn, L. The cortisol awakening response: More than a measure of HPA axis function. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2010, 35, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bottaccioli, A.G.; Bottaccioli, F.; Minelli, A. Stress and the psyche-brain-immune network in psychiatric diseases based on psychoneuroendocrineimmunology: A concise review. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018, 1437, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Williams-Bell, F.M.; Aisbett, B.; Murphy, B.A.; Larsen, B. The Effects of Simulated Wildland Firefighting Tasks on Core Temperature and Cognitive Function under Very Hot Conditions. Front. Physiol. 2017, 8, 815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Morley, J.; Beauchamp, G.; Suyama, J.; Guyette, F.X.; Reis, S.E.; Clifton, W.; Callaway, C.W.; Hostler, D. Cognitive function following treadmill exercise in thermal protective clothing. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2012, 112, 1733–1740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Christensen, H.; Griffiths, K.; Mackinnon, A.; Jacomb, P.A. A quantitative review of cognitive deficits in depression and Alzheimer-type dementia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1997, 3, 631–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Jorm, A.F. Is depression a risk factor for dementia or cognitive decline? Gerontology 2000, 46, 219–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Saijo, Y.; Ueno, T.; Hashimoto, Y. Twenty-four-hour shift work, depressive symptoms, and job dissatisfaction among Japanese firefighters. Am. J. Ind. Med. 2008, 51, 380–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Saijo, Y.; Ueno, T.; Hashimoto, Y. Job stress and depressive symptoms among Japanese fire fighters. Am. J. Ind. Med. 2007, 50, 470–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Brzezinski, A.; Vangel, M.G.; Wurtman, R.J.; Norrie, G.; Zhdanova, I.; Ben-Shushan, A.; Ford, I. Effects of exogenous melatonin on sleep: A meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 2005, 9, 41–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Ferracioli-Oda, E.; Qawasmi, A.; Bloch, M.H. Meta-analysis: Melatonin for the treatment of primary sleep disorders. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e63773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. van der Zweerde, T.; Bisdounis, L.; Kyle, S.D.; Lancee, J.; van Straten, A. Cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia: A meta-analysis of long-term effects in controlled studies. Sleep Med. Rev. 2019, 48, 101208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Eidelman, P.; Talbot, L.; Ivers, H.; Bélanger, L.; Morin, C.M.; Harvey, A.G. Change in dysfunctional beliefs about sleep in behavior therapy, cognitive therapy, and cognitive-behavioral therapy for insomnia. Behav. Ther. 2016, 47, 102–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Small, G.W. Detection and prevention of cognitive decline. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiat. 2016, 24, 1142–1150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study subjects.
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study subjects.
Ijerph 17 04647 g001
Figure 2. Shift work cycles of firefighters in South Korea.
Figure 2. Shift work cycles of firefighters in South Korea.
Ijerph 17 04647 g002
Table 1. General characteristics of the subjects.
Table 1. General characteristics of the subjects.
CharacteristicsShift Work ScheduleAll Subjects
N = 352
p-Value
3-Day Cycle
N = 71
6-Day Cycle
N = 77
9-Day Cycle
N = 83
21-Day Cycle
N = 121
Age (years) 0.043 3–6
20–294 (5.6%)11 (14.3%)11 (13.3%)14 (11.6%)40 (11.4%)
30–3921 (29.6%)35 (45.5%)29 (34.9%)47 (38.8%)132 (37.5%)
40–4930 (42.3%)19 (24.7%)24 (28.9%)48 (39.7%)121 (34.4%)
50–5916 (22.5%)12 (15.6%)19 (22.9%)12 (9.9%)59 (16.8%)
Mean ± SD42.5 ± 8.138.4 ± 9.341.2 ± 9.039.1 ± 8.040.1 ± 8.70.012 3–6,3–21
Sex 0.051
Men61 (85.9%)73 (94.8%)80 (96.4%)114 (94.2%)328 (93.2%)
Women10 (14.1%)4 (5.2%)3 (3.6%)7 (5.8%)24 (6.8%)
Job 0.009 3–21,6–21
Fire suppression14 (19.7%)21 (27.3%)25 (30.1%)51 (42.1%)111 (31.5%)
Others57 (80.3%)56 (72.7%)58 (69.9%)70 (57.9%)241 (68.5%)
Education 0.046 3–6,6–21
High school10 (14.1%)24 (31.2%)18 (21.7%)18 (14.9%)70 (19.9%)
2-year degree23 (32.4%)23 (29.9%)18 (21.7%)36 (29.7%)100 (28.4%)
4-year degree38 (53.5%)30 (39.0%)47 (56.6%)67 (55.4%)182 (51.7%)
Monthly income <0.001 3–6,3–21,6–9,9–21
Missing00202
Low 11 (15.5%)33 (42.9%)12 (14.8%)37 (30.6%)93 (26.6%)
Middle 32 (45.1%)32 (41.6%)42 (51.9%)62 (51.2%)168 (48.0%)
High 28 (39.4%)12 (15.6%)27 (33.3%)22 (18.2%)89 (25.4%)
Smoking 0.576
Missing00011
Never29 (40.8%)37 (48.0%)33 (39.8%)40 (33.3%)139 (39.6%)
Past smoker24 (33.8%)22 (28.6%)21 (25.3%)45 (37.5%)112 (31.9%)
Current light smoker10 (14.1%)10 (13.0%)17 (20.5%)21 (17.5%)58 (16.5%)
Current heavy smoker8 (11.3%)8 (10.4%)12 (14.5%)14 (11.7%)42 (12.0%)
Alcohol 0.996
Missing23128
No15 (21.7%)17 (23.0%)16 (19.5%)24 (20.2%)72 (20.9%)
Normal drinking41 (59.4%)41 (55.4%)48 (58.5%)69 (58.0%)199 (57.9%)
Heavy drinking13 (18.8%)16 (21.6%)18 (22.0%)26 (21.8%)73 (21.2%)
Regular exercise 0.636
No33 (46.5%)36 (46.8%)44 (53.0%)53 (43.8%)166 (47.2%)
Yes38 (53.5%)41 (53.2%)39 (47.0%)68 (56.2%)186 (52.8%)
Caffeine 0.045 3–6,3–9
Missing572418
No8 (12.1%)16 (22.9%)15 (18.5%)18 (15.4%)57 (17.1%)
Light coffee drinking25 (37.9%)35 (50.0%)45 (55.6%)61 (52.1%)166 (49.7%)
Moderate to heavy coffee drinking33 (50.0%)19 (27.1%)21 (25.9%)38 (32.5%)111 (33.2%)
Insomnia 0.148
Normal (≤ 7)43 (60.6%)31 (40.3%)44 (53.0%)68 (56.2%)186 (52.8%)
Mild Insomnia (8–14)21 (29.6%)40 (51.9%)32 (38.6%)41 (33.9%)134 (38.1%)
Insomnia (≥ 15)7 (9.9%)6 (7.8%)7 (8.4%)12 (9.9%)32 (9.1%)
Mean ± SD7.3 ± 2.28.8 ± 2.47.7 ± 4.77.2 ± 5.37.7 ± 5.00.151
Depression 0.896
Normal (≤ 4)60 (84.5%)64 (83.1%)71 (85.5%)103 (85.1%)298 (84.7%)
Mild depression (5–9)9 (12.7%)9 (11.7%)11 (13.3%)14 (11.6%)43 (12.2%)
Depression (≥ 10)2 (2.8%)4 (5.2%)1 (1.2%)4 (3.3%)11 (3.1%)
Mean ± SD2.2 ± 2.82.4 ± 3.12.1 ± 2.32.0 ± 2.92.1 ± 2.80.771
Values are presented as means ± standard deviation (SD) or number (%). Analyzed by ANOVA or chi-square test. Types of shift work with significant differences in multiple comparisons are listed with a superscript (3: 3-day cycle, 6: 6-day cycle, 9: 9-day cycle, 21: 21-day cycle). Heavy smoker: smoking of ≥15 cigarettes per day. Heavy drinking: consumption of ≥7 glasses per day at least two times per week in men, ≥5 glasses per day at least two times per week in women. Moderate to heavy coffee drinking: consumption of ≥3 cups per day (moderate: 3–4 cups, heavy: ≥5 cups). Regular exercise: exercise above moderate intensity for more than 1 hour a week.
Table 2. Comparison of the neurocognitive function during the daytime work and after the night shift work of firefighters.
Table 2. Comparison of the neurocognitive function during the daytime work and after the night shift work of firefighters.
DomainMean ± SD (N = 352)p-Value
During Day Work After Night Work
Composite memory90.6 ± 19.184.7 ± 19.7<0.001
Verbal memory87.7 ± 20.081.3 ± 21.9<0.001
Visual memory97.1 ± 16.394.0 ± 16.60.001
Complex attention97.8 ± 18.293.3 ± 32.40.007
Psychomotor speed112.4 ± 15.4110.1 ± 15.2<0.001
Motor speed111.0 ± 15.1108.7 ± 14.1<0.001
Processing speed107.6 ± 15.7107.4 ± 17.10.860
Reaction time92.4 ± 15.092.7 ± 17.10.711
Cognitive flexibility106.2 ± 16.9105.8 ± 19.10.671
Executive functioning107.0 ± 16.5107.2 ± 18.50.799
Neurocognitive index99.9 ± 11.697.4 ± 13.4<0.001
All subgroups were analyzed by paired t-test.
Table 3. Comparison of the neurocognitive function during the daytime work and after the night shift work of firefighters according to the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) category.
Table 3. Comparison of the neurocognitive function during the daytime work and after the night shift work of firefighters according to the Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) category.
DomainISI CategoryNMean ± SD (n = 352)p-Value
During Daytime WorkPost Nighttime Work
Composite memoryNormal18690.2 ± 20.184.8 ± 19.5<0.001
Mild insomnia13490.6 ± 17.285.4 ± 19.40.002
Insomnia3292.8 ± 21.681.5 ± 21.90.012
Verbal memoryNormal18687.7 ± 20.682.7 ± 22.5<0.001
Mild insomnia13487.4 ± 19.180.4 ± 20.3<0.001
Insomnia3289.7 ± 20.177.2 ± 24.60.001
Visual memoryNormal18696.6 ± 16.892.8 ± 15.80.006
Mild insomnia13497.6 ± 14.995.8 ± 17.10.269
Insomnia3298.3 ± 18.992.8 ± 18.60.110
Complex attentionNormal18696.1 ± 20.394.3 ± 23.70.260
Mild insomnia134100.1 ± 15.092.2 ± 43.30.027
Insomnia3298.2 ± 17.092.7 ± 22.40.227
Psychomotor speedNormal186111.7 ± 16.5110.2 ± 15.90.056
Mild insomnia134114.1 ± 14.5111.5 ± 14.50.008
Insomnia32109.3 ± 11.3104.3 ± 13.50.069
Motor speedNormal186110.1 ± 15.8108.6 ± 14.70.053
Mild insomnia134112.2 ± 14.8108.3 ± 13.50.001
Insomnia32111.4 ± 12.1104.8 ± 13.60.007
Processing speedNormal186107.7 ± 15.9106.5 ± 16.90.298
Mild insomnia134109.1 ± 15.9110.2 ± 17.40.341
Insomnia32100.8 ± 11.3101.4 ± 15.00.845
Reaction timeNormal18692.4 ± 15.292.3 ± 16.80.931
Mild insomnia13493.5 ± 15.293.3 ± 17.30.872
Insomnia3288.5 ± 12.393.0 ± 19.20.190
Cognitive flexibilityNormal186104.8 ± 18.8105.1 ± 18.90.809
Mild insomnia134108.4 ± 14.0106.8 ± 20.10.306
Insomnia32105.3 ± 15.4106.0 ± 15.80.821
Executive functioningNormal186105.6 ± 18.3106.6 ± 18.30.426
Mild insomnia134109.2 ± 13.8108.0 ± 19.80.414
Insomnia32105.8 ± 15.2108.0 ± 14.70.397
Neurocognitive indexNormal18699.0 ± 13.097.3 ± 12.70.009
Mild insomnia134101.4 ± 9.697.9 ± 14.90.001
Insomnia3298.9 ± 10.995.4 ± 10.30.089
All subgroups were analyzed by paired t-test.
Table 4. Comparison of the neurocognitive function during the daytime work and after the night shift work of firefighters according to the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) category.
Table 4. Comparison of the neurocognitive function during the daytime work and after the night shift work of firefighters according to the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) category.
DomainPHQ-9 CategoryNMean ± SD (n = 352)p-Value
During Daytime WorkPost Nighttime Work
Composite memoryNormal29890.1 ± 19.184.6 ± 19.7<0.001
Depression 5493.3 ± 19.485.3 ± 19.70.003
Verbal memoryNormal29887.3 ± 19.881.1 ± 22.0<0.001
Depression 5490.1 ± 20.982.7 ± 21.3<0.001
Visual memoryNormal29896.8 ± 16.394.0 ± 16.30.010
Depression 5499.1 ± 16.493.8 ± 18.40.040
Complex attentionNormal29897.2 ± 19.093.1 ± 34.00.030
Depression 54101.0 ± 12.794.5 ± 22.10.017
Psychomotor speedNormal298112.3 ± 15.8111.0 ± 15.00.035
Depression 54112.9 ± 13.5105.4 ± 15.9<0.001
Motor speedNormal298110.6 ± 15.5108.6 ± 14.10.002
Depression 54113.2 ± 12.7105.8 ± 14.3<0.001
Processing speedNormal298108.1 ± 15.9108.6 ± 16.90.626
Depression 54104.5 ± 14.2101.2 ± 16.80.143
Reaction timeNormal29892.7 ± 15.192.5 ± 17.30.808
Depression 5490.8 ± 14.593.7 ± 16.40.142
Cognitive flexibilityNormal298106.0 ± 17.4105.9 ± 19.00.921
Depression 54107.6 ± 14.1105.6 ± 19.70.471
Executive functioningNormal298106.8 ± 16.9107.4 ± 18.40.545
Depression 54108.2 ± 13.7106.6 ± 19.40.544
Neurocognitive indexNormal29899.7 ± 12.097.4 ± 13.6<0.001
Depression 54101.1 ± 9.496.9 ± 12.10.003
All subgroups were analyzed by paired t-test.
Table 5. Comparison of the changes of neurocognitive function according to the type of shift work.
Table 5. Comparison of the changes of neurocognitive function according to the type of shift work.
DomainType of Shift WorkModel 1Model 2Model 3
LSmeansp-ValueLSmeansp-ValueLSmeansp-Value
Composite memory3-day cycle8.380.5039.260.4938.680.483
6-day cycle4.74 5.06 4.59
9-day cycle3.81 4.82 4.18
21-day cycle6.50 7.35 7.07
Verbal memory3-day cycle−20.170.030−15.780.060−15.920.071
6-day cycle−15.49 9 −12.13 −12.07
9-day cycle−25.95 6,21 −13.01 −21.21
21-day cycle−17.19 9 −15.78 −12.68
Visual memory3-day cycle6.200.3276.390.2465.340.313
6-day cycle1.30 0.61 0.12
9-day cycle1.49 1.30 0.69
21-day cycle3.79 3.59 3.29
Complex attention3-day cycle0.210.5630.290.499−0.150.547
6-day cycle7.03 7.75 6.86
9-day cycle4.11 4.53 4.29
21-day cycle5.63 6.50 6.32
Psychomotor speed 3-day cycle2.040.3072.400.2151.810.279
6-day cycle0.31 0.10 −0.41
9-day cycle3.66 3.96 3.15
21-day cycle2.71 2.68 2.15
Motor speed 3-day cycle4.210.1313.250.0753.200.112
6-day cycle3.14 1.92 1.69
9-day cycle4.31 3.29 2.81
21-day cycle1.00 −0.39 −0.50
Processing speed ††3-day cycle−2.66 21<0.001−0.23 21<0.001−1.47 21<0.001
6-day cycle−4.21 9,21 −2.37 9,21 −2.93 9,21
9-day cycle1.12 6 3.60 6 2.64 6
21-day cycle3.88 3,6 6.36 3,6 5.50 3,6
Reaction time3-day cycle−0.690.9861.640.9072.520.860
6-day cycle−0.56 0.71 1.47
9-day cycle0.04 2.41 3.50
21-day cycle−0.09 1.57 2.41
Cognitive flexibility3-day cycle−3.24 21<0.001−3.02 21<0.001−2.88 21<0.001
6-day cycle−3.74 21 −4.53 21 −4.62 21
9-day cycle0.20 21 −0.36 21 −0.05 21
21-day cycle5.25 3,6,9 4.90 3,6,9 5.18 3,6,9
Executive functioning3-day cycle−3.52 21<0.001−3.21 21<0.001−3.10 21<0.001
6-day cycle−4.78 21 −5.51 21 −5.39 21
9-day cycle−0.46 21 −0.83 21 −0.59 21
21-day cycle4.77 3,6,9 4.42 3,6,9 4.80 3,6,9
Neurocognitive index3-day cycle1.460.2912.190.2691.520.225
6-day cycle1.57 1.77 2.92
9-day cycle2.35 3.00 4.55
21-day cycle3.99 4.53 2.07
Analyzed by general linear model. Types of shift work with significant differences in multiple comparisons are listed with a superscript (3: 3-day cycle, 6: 6-day cycle, 9: 9-day cycle, 21: 21-day cycle). Model 1: unadjusted. Model 2: adjustment for age, sex, and education level. Model 3: Model 2 + adjustments for income, job, and ISI/PHQ-9 scores. LSmeans: least square means. PHQ-9 score (β: 1.05, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.54 to 1.56) was significantly associated with change in the domain of psychomotor speed. Age was significantly associated with change in the domain of reaction time in Model 2 (β: −0.30, 95% CI −0.48 to −0.12) and Model 3 (β: −0.26, 95% CI −0.48 to −0.03). †† ISI (β: −0.58, 95% CI −0.94 to −0.21) and PHQ-9 (β: 1.32, 95% CI 0.67 to −1.98) scores were significantly associated with change in the domain of processing speed.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kwak, K.; Kim, B.-K.; Jang, T.-W.; Sim, C.S.; Ahn, Y.-S.; Choi, K.-S.; Jeong, K.S. Association between Shift Work and Neurocognitive Function among Firefighters in South Korea: A Prospective before–after Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4647. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph17134647

AMA Style

Kwak K, Kim B-K, Jang T-W, Sim CS, Ahn Y-S, Choi K-S, Jeong KS. Association between Shift Work and Neurocognitive Function among Firefighters in South Korea: A Prospective before–after Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(13):4647. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph17134647

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kwak, Kyeongmin, Bong-Kyu Kim, Tae-Won Jang, Chang Sun Sim, Yeon-Soon Ahn, Kyeong-Sook Choi, and Kyoung Sook Jeong. 2020. "Association between Shift Work and Neurocognitive Function among Firefighters in South Korea: A Prospective before–after Study" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 13: 4647. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph17134647

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop