Next Article in Journal
Listening to Hospital Personnel’s Narratives during the COVID-19 Outbreak
Next Article in Special Issue
Impact of Age-Related Vision Changes on Driving
Previous Article in Journal
Prejudice and Feeling of Threat towards Syrian Refugees: The Moderating Effects of Precarious Employment and Perceived Low Outgroup Morality
Previous Article in Special Issue
Are Your Eyes “on the Road”? Findings from the 2019 National Study on Vision and Driving Safety in Spain
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Naturalistic Driving Study in Brazil: An Analysis of Mobile Phone Use Behavior while Driving

1
Department of Transportation, Federal University of Parana, 81530-000 Curitiba, Brazil
2
Academic Department of Civil Construction, Federal University of Technology-Parana, 81280-340 Curitiba, Brazil
3
National Observatory for Road Safety, 13333-070 Indaiatuba, Brazil
4
Centre for Accident Research and Road Safety, Queensland (CARRS-Q), Queensland University of Technology (QUT), Brisbane 5049, Australia
5
Department of Industrial Engineering, Universidad del Norte, Barranquilla, Colombia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17(17), 6412; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph17176412
Submission received: 5 August 2020 / Revised: 30 August 2020 / Accepted: 31 August 2020 / Published: 3 September 2020

Abstract

:
Mobile phone use (MPU) while driving is an important road safety challenge worldwide. Naturalistic driving studies (NDS) emerged as one of the most sophisticated methodologies to investigate driver behavior; however, NDS have not been implemented in low- or middle-income countries. The aim of this research is to investigate MPU while driving and compare the results to those reported in international studies. An analysis of 61.32 h and 1350 km driven in Curitiba (Brazil) showed that MPU lasted for an average of 28.51 s (n = 627) and occurred in 58.71% of trips (n = 201) with an average frequency of 8.37 interactions per hour (n = 201). The proportion of the trip time using a mobile phone was 7.03% (n = 201), and the average instantaneous speed was 12.77 km/h (n = 627) while using the phone. Generally, drivers spent less time on more complex interactions and selected a lower speed when using the phone. MPU was observed more during short duration than longer trips. Drivers in this study engaged in a larger number of MPU compared to drivers from Netherlands and the United States; and the percentage of trip time with MPU was between North American and European values.

1. Introduction

Road crashes are the eighth leading cause of death in the world, which corresponds to 1.35 million deaths. Of these deaths, 80% are concentrated in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs) [1]. Despite the large mortality in the Americas, road safety performance has remained stable during the last seven years, which highlights the challenge of reducing road trauma [2]. Among the risk factors for the occurrence of crashes, in-vehicle distractions caused by engaging in secondary tasks are an increasing road safety concern. Specifically, mobile phone use (MPU) while driving has been identified as one of the main types of distracted driving behaviors. Due to the functionality for everyday life that these devices offer, it is possible that this scenario will worsen as mobile phone devices become more popular and/or evolve technologically [3,4,5]. It is estimated that 87% of the Brazilian population has access to some type of mobile phone—a proportion comparable to that of countries like the United States (89%), Germany (92%) and the United Kingdom (93%)—and 54% of Brazilian users check their device in the first five minutes after waking up, which indicates addiction-like symptoms [6].
The use of mobile phones while driving is also present in LMICs such as Brazil, where the vehicle fleet has grown by more than 60% in the last 10 years [7] and the number of injuries remains high. According to the most recent data (for the year 2017), there were 35,375 traffic-related fatalities in Brazil, equivalent to a rate of 15 fatalities per 100,000 inhabitants [8]. Even though Brazil has laws that prohibit MPU while driving [1], national data on traffic violations within the federal highway system indicates an increase of approximately 40% in the number of fines for MPU while driving between 2015 and 2018 [9]. A survey conducted in 27 Brazilian cities showed that 19.4% of respondents engage in MPU while driving [10].
Understanding driver behavior associated with mobile phone distracted driving is challenging. Official records such as crash data can only provide limited insights regarding the occurrence of distraction, as they can rarely guarantee that a driver was distracted at a particular point [11]. In addition, methodologies such as questionnaires and on-road observations can only provide a limited picture because drivers might exhibit social desirability bias which can result in inaccurate responses to questionnaires or engage in efforts to conceal mobile phone use, which reduces the accuracy of observations. The lack of reliable data on MPU while driving imposes important barriers to evidence-based decision making in LMICs. To address this shortcoming, the prevalence and risks of MPU while driving have been increasingly studied using naturalistic methods in countries such as the United States [12,13,14,15], the Netherlands [16,17], Germany [18], Sweden [19,20], Finland [21], Australia [22] and China [23].
Naturalistic data, however, might present a couple of limitations. In an NDS, it is possible for a single driver to generate multiple MPU episodes, while other drivers generate no episodes, introducing some bias to further analysis [24]. Moreover, results about MPU conversations are not consistent across naturalistic, experimental and epidemiological studies; specifically, texting results are similar between naturalistic and experimental studies but different to epidemiological studies [24]. In addition, it is difficult to predict crash risks and real-world crash profiles considering only the safety critical events detected in NDS [24], even with confidence limits from crash databases being coherent with NDS crash rates [25].
The understanding of driver behavior in Brazil and other LMICs has been limited to survey-based studies [10,26,27], which are subject to desirability and memory biases [15], and driving simulator studies which might have limited external validity [28,29,30]. The aims of this research are to obtain and explore fundamental indicators about MPU while driving through a naturalistic driving study (NDS) and to compare the results to those reported in international studies. The need for this research is justified by the lack of knowledge—in terms of incidence, frequency and characterization—about behaviors that are capable of causing distraction while driving a vehicle, especially distraction from MPU. It has been designed to enable the future replication of procedures on a regional scale or even in other parts of Brazil. In addition, the focus of this research is to survey the fundamental indicators, seeking first to measure more general characteristics on the use of mobile phones while driving, with the expectation that this will raise the interest of researchers and institutions for future research.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. The First Naturalistic Driving Study (NDS) in Brazil

The NDS described in this investigation was designed following the principle of “minimum value prototype” with the non-intrusive instrumentation of vehicles, i.e., laptop (Dell Inc., Round Rock, TX, USA), voltage inverter (TechOne, Rio de Janeiro, Brazil), GPS device (Global Positioning System) (Inteform, Sao Paulo, Brazil) and three cameras (LogiTech, Lausanne, Switzerland), one internal and two facing the outside in order to collect images (Figure 1). The laptop, which was positioned in front of the passenger seat, was programmed to activate the cameras and GPS automatically and to collect images. Position and speed data were collected every 1 s. No sound was recorded in order to provide some level of privacy and to avoid inhibiting any conversation related behavior (e.g., having a mobile phone call).
A total of six drivers with a conventional-use vehicle (e.g., home-work trips, leisure trips and routine tasks) were recruited using social networks. The equipment was installed in their own private vehicle—all conventional passenger cars. The participants received an amount equivalent to 100 USD for participating in the NDS. Each driver received the appropriate training in the operation of the monitoring system that had been installed and signed a consent document for privacy and image use. Their ages ranged between 19 and 38 years old, mostly male (4), with driving experience from less than 1 year to 21 years. The oldest vehicle was manufactured in 2002 and the newest in 2013, all with manual gear-transmission and with power ranging from 76 to 165 HP (Table 1).
Each driver was monitored for two weeks, and the data collection period extended from August to November 2019. Participants made a total of 207 trips, with a total duration of 61.32 driving hours and a total distance of 1350 km. The study was undertaken in Curitiba and its metropolitan region, a predominantly urban jurisdiction located in southern Brazil. Data for each driver’s first trip was discarded in order to allow the driver to familiarize themselves with the monitoring system. Only trips that recorded both video and GPS data were considered valid. The periods when the driver was not actually driving were not included in any analysis, i.e., at the beginning of the trip, when the driver turned on the monitoring system without movement of the car; during the trip, when eventually the driver pulled over without turning off the monitoring system; and at the end of the trip, when the driver activated the hand brake but kept the monitoring system on before leaving the vehicle. There were six completely invalid trips, resulting in the total number of valid trips being reduced to 201, the valid trip time to 58.38 h and the total distance to 1303 km.

2.2. MPU Analysis

The analysis of the videos taken from the internal camera allowed the identification of visual-manual MPU while driving. The following six MPU behaviors were examined in this study:
  • Texting: starting when the driver moves their hand towards the device, then touches the screen with one or both hands several times in a row, and ending when the driver puts the device down and resumes eye contact with the route or engages in another secondary task;
  • Calling/voice message: starting when the driver moves their hand towards the device, then uses it for calls or to send/listen to audio in apps, and ending when the driver puts the device down or engages in another secondary task;
  • Holding: starting when the driver moves their hand towards the device, then keeps holding the mobile phone, while looking in a different direction than where the device is, and ending when the driver puts the device down or engages in another secondary task;
  • In-holder use: starting when the driver moves their hand towards the device, then uses the mobile phone while it is in a holder fixed to the vehicle’s panel/internal windshield screen, and ending when the driver ceases manual contact with the device and resumes eye contact with the route or engages in another secondary task;
  • Checking/browsing: starting when the driver moves their hand towards the device, then touches the screen, maintaining visual and/or manual contact with the mobile phone, in order to view information, and ending when the driver puts the device down and resumes eye contact with the route, or engages in another secondary task;
  • Other: starting when the driver moves their hand towards the device, then uses it for various purposes other than those described above, such as taking a photo or using the flashlight, and ending when the driver puts the device down and resumes eye contact with the route or engages in another secondary task.
Through manual coding of the videos by a team of trained researchers, a total of 627 MPU events were identified. Five behavioral indicators were extracted: average MPU time (in seconds), I1; percentage of trips with MPU (%), I2; percentage of time using the mobile phone (%), I3; frequency of MPU (use/hour), I4; and average instantaneous speed during MPU (km/h), I5. For I4, a mobile phone use was considered complete each time the driver ceased visual and manual contact with the device. Lastly, I5 was the average value of the instantaneous speeds (for every second) during the MPU time that occurred within a valid trip period.
To investigate speed adaptation due to MPU, the average speed was considered between 8 and 10 s before starting the MPU (speed S1), the average speed from 8 to 10 s after starting the use (speed S2), the average speed from 8 to 10 s before ending the use (speed S3) and the average speed from 8 to 10 s after ending the use (speed S4). Therefore, the analysis of speed adaptation involved comparing speeds S1 and S2 and speeds S3 and S4. Of the 627 MPU events while driving, those that lasted less than 10 s and occurring at a speed of less than 10 km/h (to avoid including MPU while stationary at traffic lights or due to congestion) were not considered in this analysis. Moreover, cases in which S1 and S4 had values below 10 km/h were not considered. For cases where MPU duration was 10 to 20 s long, the speeds S2 and S3 had been coincident and centralized over the total period of MPU. Events where the interval between use associated with the S4 speed overlapped with the interval associated with the S1 speed of the subsequent use were excluded from this analysis.
After applying these criteria, only 17 MPU events remained. The videos of these events were checked, and it was found that all occurred in an urban environment and were influenced by the car-following behavior (that is, by the speed of the vehicle ahead), turning maneuvers or by passing over speed bumps. Thus, there were no MPU events remaining in which speed adaptation could be assessed without the influence of extraneous variables. For this reason, this paper will not evaluate speed adaptation.
Data analysis was conducted using non-parametric methods for comparing the obtained indicator values in terms of categorical variables: in Section 3.2 for comparing I1 values according to different types of MPU; in Section 3.3 for comparing I4 values for different trip duration categories; in Section 3.4 for comparing I4 values according to speed intervals, as well as I5 values according to different types of MPU. The limited sample size did not allow for parametric methods.

2.3. Benchmarking with International NDS

Although previously published NDS focusing on the use of mobile phones while driving used different methods, the results of the NDS in Brazil were compared with international NDSs selected mainly for their use of indicators comparable to I1 to I5 (Table 2).
In the United States, data collected from 108 drivers for 6 weeks as part of the Integrated Vehicle-Based Safety Systems Field Operational Test was analyzed [13]. The Netherlands study [16] included data from 21 drivers for a period of 5 to 6 weeks using 5 instrumented vehicles. In Germany, a study was based on data from 49 drivers for a three-month period in Germany, as part of the EuroFOT project (European Field Operational Test) [18]. In Sweden, data collected also in the EuroFOT project from 100 drivers for one year was analyzed [19,20]. In Australia, data from the Australian Naturalistic Driving Study (ANDS) included 377 drivers monitored over 4 months [22]. In the SHRP 2 project (Second Strategic Highway Research Program Naturalistic Driving Study), data from 3262 drivers was examined in the United States [31]. More recently, in the UDRIVE project (European Naturalistic Driving Study), 28 participants were analyzed during a six-month driving period in the Netherlands [17]. In Finland, data from 30 drivers during a period from 63 to 84 days was used [21].

3. Results

3.1. Characteristics of the Trips Recorded in the Brazilian NDS

Drivers travelled a total of 1303 km during the monitoring period. 96.95% of this distance was on urban roads and 3.05% on officially rural roads; however, with a high level of urbanization. Figure 2 contains examples of typical urban main road (Figure 2a), local road (Figure 2b) and a rural road with high level of urbanization (Figure 2c). Table 3 shows the individual contribution of each driver to the trips analyzed in this study.
The heat map on Figure 3 shows the concentration of trips in the main area of Curitiba, which is a city with a highly urbanized road network structured following main roads and arterials. The heat map was based on geographical coordinate points collected every second for all drivers. Thus, the visualization represents both traveled distance and travel time densities. Red areas on the map usually consist of intersections between main roads, where drivers tend to spend quite a long time stationary in congestion or at traffic lights. The number of NDS trips on particular road segments ranged from 1 to 45. For scale reasons, Figure 3 visualization covers 98% of the traveled distance considering all drivers.
Table 4 contains information on the share of trips per duration and share of travel time per speed interval. Drivers presented different patterns on trip duration, probably as a function of their commuting distances; for example, while D2 had 71.43% of trips lasting more than 30 min, D4 and D6 did not travel for more than 30 min. According to speed intervals, share of travel time showed more similar distributions between drivers: 63.91% average of the trip time on speed up to 30 km/h, 31.53% average on speed between 30 and 60 km/h and 4.46% average on speed above 60 km/h.
The average number of trips on weekdays per driver was 2.78. Of all the trips, 84.08% were from Monday to Friday, while on weekends the average number of trips per driver was 1.28 (53.96% lower than on weekdays). The start times of trips were concentrated in the afternoon period, from 5 to 6 p.m. (20.71% of the trips), from 7 to 8 p.m. (10.65%) and from 6 to 7 p.m. (8.28%). Morning had lower concentrations, i.e., 8.28% between 7 and 8 a.m., 7.69% between 9 and 10 a.m., and 4.73% between 8 and 9 a.m. The incidence of MPU according to the trip duration showed that 61.98% of the trips with up to 15 min duration included MPU; however, for trips lasting between 15 and 30 min, this proportion decreased to 10.64%, and for trips lasting more than 30 min the proportion was 9.09%.

3.2. Average Time That Driver Spent Using Mobile Phone and Type of Use

The average time spent engaged in visual-manual MPU, I1, was 28.46 s (SD = 47.71 s) based on the 627 events. In the present study, the range of time engaged in MPU varied from 2 s to 10.48 min. While usage duration less than 10 s represents 38.76% of the events (Figure 4a), durations that were between 10 and 50 s represented 48.17% of events. All drivers used their mobile phones, with an average time ranging from 10 s, for D2 (driver 2), and 38 s for D5 (Figure 4b). In order to present the boxplot of Figure 4b on an appropriate scale, four outliers of MPU duration were removed, namely, 629 s for D5, 367 s for D4, 344 s for D3 and 297 s for D1.
The most common type of use was checking/browsing the device at 46.11% of the time spent, followed by simply holding the mobile phone (without eye contact) for 21.92% and 14.46% for making/receiving calls, including listening to and recording audio messages (Table 5). The use of the mobile phone to type messages corresponded to 7.39% of the total time of use. Calling/voice message demanded an average time of 63.05 s (SD = 73.50 s), the longest among the types of use; the average texting task duration was 20.03 s (SD = 20.15 s) and the in-holder use 11.07 s (SD = 14.90). In addition, Wilcoxon signed rank tests indicated that calling/voice message duration is greater than checking/browsing (W = 13,525.0, p < 0.001), in-holder use (W = 6168.5, p < 0.001) and texting durations (W = 2902.0, p < 0.001); checking/browsing duration is greater than in-holder use duration (W = 123,885.5, p < 0.001); and texting duration is greater than in-holder use duration (W = 13,511.0, p < 0.001).

3.3. Average Percentage of Trips with MPU, Average Percentage of Trip Time with MPU and Frequency of Use

The average percentage of trips with MPU while driving (I2) was 58.71% (SD = 25.80%), varying between 29.73% (D6) and 93.10% (D1). Table 6 contains the values of means, standard deviation, minimum, maximum and quartiles for I3 (mobile phone usage time percentage) and I4 (frequency of use) considering the 201 valid trips and considering the 627 MPU events for I5 (instantaneous average speed during MPU).
In general, low I3 values (<5%) are more common since they correspond to 63.18% of the trips (Figure 5a). For higher values of I3, the share of participation reduces, for instance, to 20.40% for I3 between 5 and 15% of the time and to 8.46% for I3 between 15 and 25%. On average, 8.37 occurrences of MPU per hour (SD = 10.69 uses/h) were identified (I4), equivalent to one MPU event every 7.17 min. The histogram in Figure 5b shows the distribution of I4 values, where an amount of up to 5 uses/h is observed in 49.25% of trips and an I4 value between 5 and 15 uses/h in 28.86% of trips. The comparison between frequencies of MPU in different trip durations showed that there is some influence of trip duration on engagement in MPU while driving. Wilcoxon signed rank tests showed that the frequency of MPU in trips between 0 and 15 min was lower than in trips between 15 and 30 min (W = 8743.0, p < 0.001), 30 and 45 min (W = 7909.5, p < 0.001), 45 and 60 min (W = 7607.0, p = 0.002) and longer than 60 min (W = 7587.0, p = 0.015).

3.4. Average Instantaneous Speed While Using the Mobile Phone

The average instantaneous speed during MPU (I5) is 12.77 km/h (SD = 16.38 km/h). Based on the association between the indicators of frequency of use and percentage of time using the mobile phone at a given speed, confidence interval graphs were constructed showing the means (Figure 6). In Figure 6a it can be seen that the average MPU time was lower for the speed range 0 to 10 km/h (when there is a greater number of cases), while it increases for the speed range 10 to 20 km/h, and it starts to reduce continuously as speed increases.
When it comes to the frequency of MPU, a higher mean (but with a wide standard deviation) was identified for the speed range 0 to 10 km/h (Figure 6b), for which the number of cases using the mobile phone is substantially greater. It can be noted that the average values of I5 and the variation around the mean reduce to speed ranges between 10 and 20 km/h and between 20 and 30 km/h, when the number of MPU cases reduces. Wilcoxon signed rank tests show statistically significant differences between frequencies of MPU according to speed intervals: Frequencies of MPU are higher for speeds between 0 and 10 km/h in comparison to speeds between 10 and 20 km/h (W = 253.0, p = 0.018), between 20 and 30 km/h (W = 327.5, p = 0.005) and higher than 30 km/h (W = 113.0, p = 0.004).
The types of use that tend to demand greater visual-manual contact, which causes a higher level of distraction (such as typing messages), were performed at a lower average speed of 10.57 km/h (SD = 12.30 km/h). On the other hand, the average speed while engaged in in-holder use (16.45 km/h, SD = 15.39 km/h) and calling/voice message (17.28 km/h, SD = 14.80 km/h) secondary tasks was higher. Wilcoxon signed rank tests confirms these assumptions, since speed while calling/voice message is greater than checking/browsing (W = 11,413.0, p = 0.002) and texting speeds (W = 2642.0, p = 0.003); checking browsing speed is lower than use on-holder speed (W = 108,223.0, p < 0.001); and texting speed is lower than in-holder use speed (W = 16,908.0, p = 0.009).

4. Discussion

This paper describes the first NDS performed in Brazil, including six drivers below 40 years old resulting in 58.38 driving hours and a total distance of 1303 km available for analysis, mostly traveled in urban area. In this study, MPU while driving lasted 28.46 s on average with 46.11% of that time being used to “check/browse” on the phone. MPU occurred in 58.71% of trips, the average frequency of use was 8.37 uses/h, the percentage of trip time using the mobile phone was 7.03%, and the average instantaneous speed was 12.77 km/h while engaged in MPU. The confirmation of the large prevalence of MPU while driving in Brazil is very concerning because previous research has consistently shown that visual-manual interactions with a mobile phone while driving such as texting or browsing impairs performance of the driving task [30,31,32,33,34]. Consequently, MPU while driving can be associated with an increased risk of crashing [31,35,36]. The findings from this study and the associated risks of MPU while driving highlight the need to urgently develop interventions to reduce MPU while driving in Brazil.

4.1. MPU Duration, Trip Length and Driving Speed Analysis

Comparisons between the duration of different MPU behaviors were conducted. The results showed a difference in average duration of MPU and average speed using the phone while driving according to the type of use (texting, calling/voice message, in-holder use and checking/browsing). Drivers tend to spend less time using the mobile phone for complex MPU such as texting, which involved looking at the screen of the device and typing, as opposed to browsing/checking which does not require typing. The large difference in average duration of in-holder MPU (11.07 s) and calling/voice message MPU (63.05 s) is consistent with the assumptions that (i) drivers perceive that talking and voice messaging do not deviate much attention needed for safe driving, so they tend to spend more time on these activities; and (ii) in-holder use tends to be more uncomfortable, thus mobile phone use tends to be brief. These behaviors can be interpreted as a risk-compensatory strategy of drivers to prevent excessive workload from the secondary task. Previous experimental research [37] has demonstrated that drivers actively seek to reduce or manage the additional workload produced by mobile phone interactions. Specifically, drivers make tactical decisions to reduce workload by selecting types of MPU that are shorter and easier to self-pace in case the driver needs to suddenly focus all of their attention on the driving task again.
The results from this study also show that MPU while driving increases during longer trips and lower average driving speeds. The associations between length of the trip and MPU further confirm associations between driver states such as boredom with engagement in mobile phone distracted driving. Previous research has shown that mobile phones are utilized by drivers as a source of stimuli to overcome boredom [38]. Drivers have generally also been observed reducing their speed in order to facilitate MPU. This finding is well established in the scientific literature through self-reports [39,40], experiments [41,42,43] and naturalistic studies [15,44]. It has been demonstrated that this change in the driving speed is used by drivers to reduce crash risk [45].

4.2. Comparisons with International Studies

The values found in this study resemble those of other international NDS, highlighting the seriousness of MPU. International comparisons indicate the average time spent engaged in visual-manual MPU (I1) of 28.46 s (SD = 47.71 s) is similar to the average time of 31 s found in a Netherlands naturalistic study [16] and the average time of 30 s found in a North American naturalistic study carried out in Detroit, MI [13]. For the German study, the I1 value was 46 s [18]. This result is not surprising since research has consistently showed that drivers in LMICs show the same or more propensity to engage in risky driving behaviors due to a wide number of systematic factors such as insufficient legislation, lack of effective police enforcement, poor education and training, etc. [46]. The average percentage of trips with MPU while driving (I2) of 58.71% (SD = 25.80%) was much higher than the value of 13% found in a Swedish NDS [20], higher than that found in two studies in the Netherlands, an older one with 40% [16] and a more recent one with 32% [17]. However, the present Brazilian result (I2) is similar to the results of the German NDS that found MPU in 56% of the trips [18].
The Brazilian value of 7.03% of time using the mobile phone (I3) is similar in comparison to the Australian value of 7% [22]. Although the average value found in this research for I3 is lower than the most recent Netherlands study, 9.2% [17], it is higher than the Swedish value, 0.63% [19], the North American values, 2.3% [13] and 6% [31], as well as the older Netherlands study with 4% [16]. On average, 8.37 MPU occurrences of use per hour (SD = 10.69 uses/h) were identified (I4), equivalent to one MPU event every 7.17 min. The 8.37 MPU occurrences of use per hour (SD = 10.69 uses/h) identified (I4) is nearly twice the frequency of 4.2 uses/h calculated for the Netherlands study [16] and more than three times the value of 2.5 uses/h from the North American study [13]. Finally, although driving speed is strongly influenced by the operational characteristics of the environment driven and a direct comparison is very difficult [47,48], the average instantaneous speed during MPU of 12.77 km/h (SD = 16.38 km/h) is much lower than the value (34 km/h) obtained in the Finnish study [21].

4.3. Limitations of the Study

The first NDS in Brazil had limited scope with a small sample size, since drivers (all under the age of 40) from one city in Brazil may not represent all Brazilian drivers or even less all drivers in LMICs. Another potential limitation is selection bias; as just certain types of drivers would have been attracted to participate in the study due to the financial rewards or to privacy concerns. Future research is needed to strengthen the generalizability of the findings and to overcome limitations. Increasing the geographical coverage and sample size of the current study is one option to increase generalizability of the findings. In addition, a more diverse group of participants would enable identification of human factors and age-specific determinants of driver behavior. Moreover, evaluation of speed variation due to MPU might be possible in a larger scale NDS.
The participants were not told that mobile phone use while driving was going to be specifically registered. However, it was clear to drivers that they were participating in driver behavior research, in which one would probably expect that mobile phone use would be monitored. Drivers were told to behave exactly how they would behave if they were in their natural driving conditions. It was possible to identify that drivers were performing typically private behaviors (for example, scratching or applying make-up), that is, actions usually performed alone or in a very familiar environment.

5. Conclusions

The current study offers unique insights into driver behavior that have not been reported in previous research in Brazil. The development of low-cost solutions for conducting behavioral research, like the one presented in this manuscript, contributes to the development of road safety knowledge and traffic behavior in LMICs. Naturalistic driving studies are a viable alternative for the survey of road user behavior in LMICs. Overall, these findings show that the results from this investigation resemble previous research findings, which in turn provides evidence for the validity of this Brazilian NDS. Therefore, the Brazilian NDS is a low-cost and valid methodology to investigate MPU while driving.
The present research identified evidence of the strategies used for drivers in Brazil to compensate for excessive workload due to mobile phone use while driving. Drivers spent less time on the mobile phone for complex MPU, such as texting, as opposed to less complex tasks like browsing/checking. Moreover, it was observed that MPU when driving increases during longer trips and lower average driving speeds. Drivers in this study also showed a larger number of engagements in MPU than values from countries such as the Netherlands and the United States. On the other hand, the percentage of MPU time obtained in this research is between North American and European values. These findings may be the result of cultural, legislative and enforcement differences between high-income versus low- and middle-income countries.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.T.B., J.A.R. and O.O.-T.; methodology, J.T.B. and O.O.-T.; software, J.T.B., P.A.B.d.S. and O.O.-T.; validation, J.T.B. and O.O.-T.; formal analysis, J.T.B., P.A.B.d.S., E.C.A. and O.O.-T.; investigation, J.T.B., P.A.B.d.S. and E.C.A.; resources, J.T.B., T.M.C.G. and J.A.R.; data curation, J.T.B., P.A.B.d.S., E.C.A. and J.A.R.; writing—original draft preparation, J.T.B., P.A.B.d.S. and E.C.A.; writing—review and editing, J.T.B., T.M.C.G., M.J.K. and O.O.-T.; visualization, J.T.B., P.A.B.d.S. and E.C.A.; supervision, J.T.B. and M.J.K.; project administration, J.T.B.; funding acquisition, J.T.B. and J.A.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq, Brazil)/MCTIC/CNPq N°. 28/2018, Universal/Faixa A, by the Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (CAPES)/Finance Code 001 and by the National Observatory for Road Safety (ONSV) under the Technical Cooperation Agreement with the Federal University of Parana. Dr. Oscar Oviedo-Trespalacios is the recipient of an Australian Research Council Discovery Early Career Researcher Award [DE200101079] funded by the Australian Government.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the video monitoring team for the detailed information provided, Magaly Natália Pazzian Vasconcellos Romão for leading the focus group session and Anabela dos Santos Aleixo Simões for the participation during the research project conception.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. WHO. Global Status Report on Road Safety. 2018. Available online: https://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/road_safety_status/2018/en/ (accessed on 13 January 2020).
  2. PAHO. Status of Road Safety in the Region of the Americas. 2019. Available online: http://iris.paho.org/xmlui/handle/123456789/51088 (accessed on 13 January 2020).
  3. Tison, J.; Chaudhary, N.; Cosgrove, L. National Phone Survey on Distracted Driving Attitudes and Behaviors. 2011. Available online: https://www.nhtsa.gov/sites/nhtsa.dot.gov/files/811555.pdf (accessed on 13 January 2020).
  4. WHO. Mobile Phone Use: A Growing Problem of Driver Distraction; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2011; ISBN 978 92 4 150089 0. [Google Scholar]
  5. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; Nandavar, S.; Newton, J.D.A.; Demant, D.; Phillips, J.G. Problematic use of mobile phones in Australia…Is it getting worse? Front. Psychiatry 2019, 10, 105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Wigginton, C.; Curran, M.; Brodeur, C. Global Mobile Consumer Trends, 2nd ed.; Delloite: London, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ministry of Infrastructure. Registred Vehicle Fleet. 2020. Available online: https://infraestrutura.gov.br/component/content/article/115-portal-denatran/8552-estatísticas-frota-de-veículos-denatran.html (accessed on 14 January 2020).
  8. Ministry of Health. Number of Traffic Fatalities in Brazil. 2020. Available online: http://tabnet.datasus.gov.br/cgi/tabcgi.exe?sim/cnv/ext10uf.def (accessed on 19 December 2019).
  9. Federal Highway Police Department Traffic Offenses on Federal Highways. 2020. Available online: https://portal.prf.gov.br/dados-abertos-infracoes (accessed on 13 January 2020).
  10. Ministry of Health. Vigitel Brasil 2018: Vigilância de Fatores de Risco e Proteção para Doenças Crônicas por Inquerito Telefônico. 2019. Available online: http://bvsms.saude.gov.br/bvs/publicacoes/vigitel_brasil_2011_fatores_risco_doencas_cronicas.pdf (accessed on 13 January 2020).
  11. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; Truelove, V.; Watson, B.; Hinton, J.A. The impact of road advertising signs on driver behaviour and implications for road safety: A critical systematic review. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2019, 122, 85–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Schneidereit, T.; Petzoldt, T.; Keinath, A.; Krems, J.F. Using SHRP 2 naturalistic driving data to assess drivers’ speed choice while being engaged in different secondary tasks. J. Saf. Res. 2017, 62, 33–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Funkhouser, D.; Sayer, J. Naturalistic Census of Cell Phone Use. Transp. Res. Rec. J. Transp. Res. Board 2012, 2321, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Atwood, J.; Guo, F.; Fitch, G.; Dingus, T.A. The driver-level crash risk associated with daily cellphone use and cellphone use while driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2018, 119, 149–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Dozza, M.; Flannagan, C.A.C.; Sayer, R. Real-world effects of using a phone while driving on lateral and longitudinal control of vehicles. J. Saf. Res. 2015, 55, 81–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Christoph, M.; van Nes, N.; Knapper, A. Naturalistic Driving Observations of Manual and Visual-Manual Interactions with Navigation Systems and Mobile Phones While Driving. Transp. Res. Rec. J. Transp. Res. Board 2013, 2365, 31–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Christoph, M.; Wesseling, S.; van Nes, N. Self-regulation of drivers’ mobile phone use: The influence of driving context. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2019, 66, 262–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Metz, B.; Landau, A.; Just, M. Frequency of secondary tasks in driving—Results from naturalistic driving data. Saf. Sci. 2014, 68, 195–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Tivesten, E.; Dozza, M. Driving context influences drivers’ decision to engage in visual-manual phone tasks: Evidence from a naturalistic driving study. J. Safety Res. 2015, 53, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Tivesten, E.; Dozza, M. Driving context and visual-manual phone tasks influence glance behavior in naturalistic driving. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2014, 26, 258–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Kujala, T.; Mäkelä, J. Naturalistic study on the usage of smartphone applications among Finnish drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2018, 115, 53–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Young, K.L.; Osborne, R.; Koppel, S.; Charlton, J.L.; Grzebieta, R.; Williamson, A.; Haworth, N.; Woolley, J.; Senserrick, T. What contextual and demographic factors predict drivers’ decision to engage in secondary tasks? IET Intell. Transp. Syst. 2019, 13, 1218–1223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Zhang, L.; Cui, B.; Yang, M.; Guo, F.; Wang, J. Effect of using mobile phones on driver’s control behavior based on naturalistic driving data. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Simmons, S.M.; Hicks, A.; Caird, J.K. Safety-critical event risk associated with cell phone tasks as measured in naturalistic driving studies: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2016, 87, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Dingus, T.A. Estimates of prevalence and risk associated with inattention and distraction based upon in situ naturalistic data. Ann. Adv. Automot. Med. 2014, 58, 60–68. [Google Scholar]
  26. Ilias, D.; Caravatto Baras, F.; Crespo, M.; Pardo De Alexandre, G.; Martinez, J.E.; Cristina, V.; Santucci, R.; Regina, S.; Nascimento, D. Mobile phone using while driving among young university students. Rev. Fac. Ciênc. Méd. Sorocaba 2012, 14, 123–125. [Google Scholar]
  27. Ministério da Saúde. População Residente em 2017—Estimativas para o TCU. 2019. Available online: http://tabnet.datasus.gov.br/cgi/deftohtm.exe?ibge/cnv/poptbr.def (accessed on 13 January 2020).
  28. Lucas, F.R.; Russo, L.E.A.; Kawashima, R.S.; Figueira, A.D.C.; Larocca, A.P.C.; Kabbach, F.I. Uso de simuladores de direção aplicado ao projeto de segurança viária. Bol. Ciencias Geod. 2013, 19, 341–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. dos Santos, M.I.; de Oliveira, P.T.M.e.S.; Ribeiro, R.L.; Larocca, A.P.C.; Junior, F.I.K. Conceito, configuração e aplicação de um simulador de direção no Brasil—Estudo de caso. Transportes 2017, 25, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Larocca, A.P.C.; Ribeiro, R.L.; da Cruz Figueira, A.; de Oliveira, P.T.M.e.S.; Lulio, L.C.; Rangel, M.A.C. Analysis of perception of vertical signaling of highways by drivers in a simulated driving environment. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2018, 58, 471–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Dingus, T.A.; Guo, F.; Lee, S.; Antin, J.F.; Perez, M.; Buchanan-King, M.; Hankey, J. Driver crash risk factors and prevalence evaluation using naturalistic driving data. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 2636–2641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Caird, J.K.; Willness, C.R.; Steel, P.; Scialfa, C. A meta-analysis of the effects of cell phones on driver performance. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2008, 40, 1282–1293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Hosking, S.G.; Young, K.L.; Regan, M.A. The effects of text messaging on young drivers. Hum. Factors 2009, 51, 582–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; Haque, M.M.; King, M.; Washington, S. Understanding the impacts of mobile phone distraction on driving performance: A systematic review. Transp. Res. Part C Emerg. Technol. 2016, 72, 360–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Strayer, D.L.; Drews, F.A.; Crouch, D.J. A comparison of the cell phone driver and the drunk driver. Hum. Factors 2006, 48, 381–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Farmer, C.M.; Braitman, K.A.; Lund, A.K. Cell phone use while driving and attributable crash risk. Traffic Inj. Prev. 2010, 11, 466–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; Haque, M.M.; King, M.; Washington, S. “Mate! I’m running 10 min late”: An investigation into the self-regulation of mobile phone tasks while driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2019, 122, 134–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Steinberger, F.; Moeller, A.; Schroeter, R. The antecedents, experience, and coping strategies of driver boredom in young adult males. J. Saf. Res. 2016, 59, 69–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Young, K.L.; Lenné, M.G. Driver engagement in distracting activities and the strategies used to minimise risk. Saf. Sci. 2010, 48, 326–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; King, M.; Haque, M.M.; Washington, S. Risk factors of mobile phone use while driving in Queensland: Prevalence, attitudes, crash risk perception, and task-management strategies. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0183361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Wandtner, B.; Schumacher, M.; Schmidt, E.A. The role of self-regulation in the context of driver distraction: A simulator study. Traffic Inj. Prev. 2016, 17, 472–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; Haque, M.M.; King, M.; Washington, S. Self-regulation of driving speed among distracted drivers: An application of driver behavioral adaptation theory. Traffic Inj. Prev. 2017, 18, 599–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; Haque, M.M.; King, M.; Demmel, S. Driving behaviour while self-regulating mobile phone interactions: A human-machine system approach. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2018, 118, 253–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Fitch, G.; Toole, L.; Grove, K. Investigating Drivers’ Compensatory Behavior when Using a Mobile Device; Report #17-UT-048; Housed at the Virginia Tech Transportation Institute: Blacksburg, VA, USA, 2017; pp. 1–31. [Google Scholar]
  45. Li, X.; Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; Rakotonirainy, A.; Yan, X. Collision risk management of cognitively distracted drivers in a car-following situation. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2019, 60, 288–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Scott-Parker, B.; Oviedo-Trespalacios, O. Young driver risky behaviour and predictors of crash risk in Australia, New Zealand and Colombia: Same but different? Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 99, 30–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; Haque, M.M.; King, M.; Washington, S. Effects of road infrastructure and traffic complexity in speed adaptation behaviour of distracted drivers. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 101, 67–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O.; Afghari, A.P.; Haque, M.M. A hierarchical Bayesian multivariate ordered model of distracted drivers’ decision to initiate risk-compensating behaviour. Anal. Methods Accid. Res. 2020, 26, 100121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Images collected by cameras: (a) frontal left, (b) frontal right and (c) internal.
Figure 1. Images collected by cameras: (a) frontal left, (b) frontal right and (c) internal.
Ijerph 17 06412 g001
Figure 2. Typical urban roads: (a) main, (b) local and (c) rural urbanized road.
Figure 2. Typical urban roads: (a) main, (b) local and (c) rural urbanized road.
Ijerph 17 06412 g002
Figure 3. Heat map of the volume of routes and road network in Curitiba main area.
Figure 3. Heat map of the volume of routes and road network in Curitiba main area.
Ijerph 17 06412 g003
Figure 4. Mobile phone usage time histogram (a) and boxplot (b) representation.
Figure 4. Mobile phone usage time histogram (a) and boxplot (b) representation.
Ijerph 17 06412 g004
Figure 5. Histogram of percentage of time using cell phone (a) and frequency of cell phone use (b) (for all participants).
Figure 5. Histogram of percentage of time using cell phone (a) and frequency of cell phone use (b) (for all participants).
Ijerph 17 06412 g005
Figure 6. Interval plot graphs for average time (a) and frequency of MPU (b) by speed category.
Figure 6. Interval plot graphs for average time (a) and frequency of MPU (b) by speed category.
Ijerph 17 06412 g006
Table 1. General info of participants.
Table 1. General info of participants.
DriverAgeGender (M/F) 1Driving Time Experience (Years)Car ModelCar Model YearUnit of Power (HP) 2
D131F10Chevrolet/Prisma201297
D238M<1Renault/Scenic2009115
D319M<1VW/Fox2010104
D423M4GM/Zafira2002116
D538F21VW/Fox201376
D625M7Citröen/DS32012165
1 M, Male; F, Female; 2 HP, Horsepower.
Table 2. International studies used for results comparison.
Table 2. International studies used for results comparison.
CountryAuthorsI1 1I2 2I3 3I4 4I5 5
United StatesFunkhouser and Sayer (2012) [13]
NetherlandsChristoph, Nes and Knapper (2013) [16]
GermanyMetz, Landau and Just (2014) [18]
AustraliaYoung et al. (2019) [22]
SwedenTivesten and Dozza (2014) [19]
Tivesten and Dozza (2015) [20]
United StatesDingus et al. (2016) [31]
NetherlandsChristoph, Wesseling and van Nes (2019) [17]
FinlandKujala and Mäkelä (2018) [21]
1 I1, average time using mobile phone; 2 I2, percentage of trips with MPU; 3 I3, percentage of time using the mobile phone; 4 I4, frequency of MPU; 5 I5, average instantaneous speed during MPU.
Table 3. Total number of trips and number and proportion of trips using a mobile phone by each driver.
Table 3. Total number of trips and number and proportion of trips using a mobile phone by each driver.
DriverTotal Number of TripsShare of Total Trip (SD = 8%)Traveled Distance (km)Share of Total Traveled Distance
(SD = 4%)
Share of Traveled Distance in Urban Roads (%)Share of Traveled Distance in Highways (%)
D12914.29227.61017.47100.000.00
D2147.88263.55717.8596.103.90
D3178.37207.39915.7399.860.14
D44823.65235.22817.73100.000.00
D55627.59286.99721.4389.1310.87
D63718.23129.7529.79100.000.00
Total-100%1303100%--
Table 4. Share of trips per duration and share of travel time per speed.
Table 4. Share of trips per duration and share of travel time per speed.
DriverShare of Trips per Duration (%)Share of Travel Time per Speed (%)
0–15 min15–30 minAbove 30 min0–30 km/h30–60 km/hAbove 60 km/h
D113.7951.7234.4871.0426.742.23
D27.1421.4371.4367.0430.622.34
D323.5341.1835.2957.1834.178.66
D475.0025.000.0060.2134.495.31
D575.0012.5012.5063.0232.624.36
D691.898.110.0064.9930.564.45
Mean47.7326.6625.6263.9131.534.46
Table 5. Different type of use regarding the mobile phone by total usage time and I1.
Table 5. Different type of use regarding the mobile phone by total usage time and I1.
TypeUsage Time (s)Usage Time (%)I1 (s)
95% CI [LL–UL] 3
Checking/browsing824446.1120.31 [23.20–17.41]
Holding391921.9237.68 [28.57–46.79]
Calling/voice message258514.4663.05 [39.84–86.26]
On-holder16399.1711.07 [8.65–13.50]
Texting13227.3935.73 [15.98–24.98]
Other93 10.52-
NPI (Not Possible to Identify)76 20.43-
1 Only 7 observations; 2 Only 3 observations; 3 I1, Average usage time; CI, Confidence interval; LL, Lower limit; UL, Upper limit.
Table 6. Descriptive Statistic for I3, I4 e I5 values.
Table 6. Descriptive Statistic for I3, I4 e I5 values.
Statistic ParameterI3 1 (%)
n = 201
I4 2 (uses/h)
n = 201
I5 3 (km/h)
n = 627
Mean7.038.3712.77
Standard deviation11.1610.5914.14
Minimum value0.000.000.00
1º quartile (Q1)0.000.000.92
Median1.905.268.21
3º quartile (Q3)8.8913.0322.08
Maximum value65.7557.7561.96
1 I3, percentage of time using the mobile phone; 2 I4, frequency of MPU; 3 I5, average instantaneous speed during MPU.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Bastos, J.T.; Santos, P.A.B.d.; Amancio, E.C.; Gadda, T.M.C.; Ramalho, J.A.; King, M.J.; Oviedo-Trespalacios, O. Naturalistic Driving Study in Brazil: An Analysis of Mobile Phone Use Behavior while Driving. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6412. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph17176412

AMA Style

Bastos JT, Santos PABd, Amancio EC, Gadda TMC, Ramalho JA, King MJ, Oviedo-Trespalacios O. Naturalistic Driving Study in Brazil: An Analysis of Mobile Phone Use Behavior while Driving. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2020; 17(17):6412. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph17176412

Chicago/Turabian Style

Bastos, Jorge Tiago, Pedro Augusto B. dos Santos, Eduardo Cesar Amancio, Tatiana Maria C. Gadda, José Aurélio Ramalho, Mark J. King, and Oscar Oviedo-Trespalacios. 2020. "Naturalistic Driving Study in Brazil: An Analysis of Mobile Phone Use Behavior while Driving" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 17: 6412. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph17176412

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop