Next Article in Journal
Relationships among Food Label Use, Motivation, and Dietary Quality
Next Article in Special Issue
Estimated Daily Intake and Seasonal Food Sources of Quercetin in Japan
Previous Article in Journal
Black Tea Lowers Blood Pressure and Wave Reflections in Fasted and Postprandial Conditions in Hypertensive Patients: A Randomised Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Comparative Validity and Reproducibility of a Diet Quality Index for Adults: The Australian Recommended Food Score
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Role of Dietary Pattern Analysis in Determining Cognitive Status in Elderly Australian Adults

by
Kimberly Ashby-Mitchell
1,*,
Anna Peeters
2 and
Kaarin J. Anstey
1
1
Centre for Research on Ageing, Health & Wellbeing, The Australian National University, Florey, Building 54, Mills Road, Acton, ACT 2601, Australia
2
Baker IDI Heart and Diabetes Institute, 75 Commercial Rd, Melbourne VIC 3004, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2015, 7(2), 1052-1067; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/nu7021052
Submission received: 20 November 2014 / Revised: 19 December 2014 / Accepted: 20 January 2015 / Published: 4 February 2015
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Assessment of Nutrient Intakes)

Abstract

:
Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was used to determine the association between dietary patterns and cognitive function and to examine how classification systems based on food groups and food items affect levels of association between diet and cognitive function. The present study focuses on the older segment of the Australian Diabetes, Obesity and Lifestyle Study (AusDiab) sample (age 60+) that completed the food frequency questionnaire at Wave 1 (1999/2000) and the mini-mental state examination and tests of memory, verbal ability and processing speed at Wave 3 (2012). Three methods were used in order to classify these foods before applying PCA. In the first instance, the 101 individual food items asked about in the questionnaire were used (no categorisation). In the second and third instances, foods were combined and reduced to 32 and 20 food groups, respectively, based on nutrient content and culinary usage—a method employed in several other published studies for PCA. Logistic regression analysis and generalized linear modelling was used to analyse the relationship between PCA-derived dietary patterns and cognitive outcome. Broader food group classifications resulted in a greater proportion of food use variance in the sample being explained (use of 101 individual foods explained 23.22% of total food use, while use of 32 and 20 food groups explained 29.74% and 30.74% of total variance in food use in the sample, respectively). Three dietary patterns were found to be associated with decreased odds of cognitive impairment (CI). Dietary patterns derived from 101 individual food items showed that for every one unit increase in ((Fruit and Vegetable Pattern: p = 0.030, OR 1.061, confidence interval: 1.006–1.118); (Fish, Legumes and Vegetable Pattern: p = 0.040, OR 1.032, confidence interval: 1.001–1.064); (Dairy, Cereal and Eggs Pattern: p = 0.003, OR 1.020, confidence interval: 1.007–1.033)), the odds of cognitive impairment decreased. Different results were observed when the effect of dietary patterns on memory, processing speed and vocabulary were examined. Complex patterns of associations between dietary factors and cognition were evident, with the most consistent finding being the protective effects of high vegetable and plant-based food item consumption and negative effects of ‘Western’ patterns on cognition. Further long-term studies and investigation of the best methods for dietary measurement are needed to better understand diet-disease relationships in this age group.

1. Introduction

Cognitive impairment is a condition in which a person has difficulty with memory, learning, concentrating or making decisions that affect their daily life [1]. Diet is among several modifiable factors found to influence cognitive function [2,3,4]. Age is presently the strongest known predictor for cognitive decline, and cognitive impairment (CI) has been shown to adversely affect quality of life and functional ability [5,6]. Risk reduction is especially important because there is still no effective treatment for dementia [7].
Studies aimed at elucidating the association between diet and cognitive function have utilised both the single nutrient and dietary pattern approaches [8,9]. While the single-nutrient approach has addressed various public health problems, many researchers theorise that due to high correlations between individual food constituents, there should be a shift toward analysis using a dietary pattern approach [10,11]. Evidence on the effect of dietary lipids, B-vitamins, antioxidants, fish, alcohol, vegetables and legumes have all produced varying results, and further research is needed into biomarkers for particular nutrients and cognitive endpoints in order for any definitive population-based conclusions to be reached [2,12,13,14,15]. Diets low in saturated fat, high in legumes, fruits and vegetables, moderate in ethanol intake and low in meat and dairy have also been highlighted as being beneficial to neurological function. One of the most studied dietary patterns is the Mediterranean diet, a diet rich in cereals, olive oil, fish, fruits and vegetables and low in dairy and meat, with a moderate consumption of red wine. This diet has been linked to increased survival, reduced risk of cancers, cardiovascular disease, longevity and cognitive impairment [16]. However, it is important to consider that there may be other dietary patterns, yet to be identified, that may have similar benefits and that can be applied to various sociocultural and demographic settings.
Few studies have examined the effect of dietary patterns on cognitive function using a data-driven method and even fewer of these studies have utilised Australian data. We identified only two studies utilising a data-driven approach to dietary analysis that have examined links with cognition in an Australian sample. The first used data from the Melbourne Collaborative Cohort Study in conducting factor analysis to determine the effect of dietary intake on psychological distress in older Australians [17] and the second utilised data from the Personality and Total Health (PATH) Through Life Study to examine the diet-depression relationship in three cohorts [18].
PCA is a data-driven approach that reduces a large number of food variables into a smaller set that captures the major dietary traits in the population [19]. In nutritional epidemiology, PCA can be used to investigate exposure-disease associations. As it relates to older age groups, such information can serve to develop age-specific guidelines and policies. One of the major criticisms of PCA, however, is that results can differ based on the methods employed during variable reduction and classification [20,21] and there is presently no accepted gold standard for dietary analysis to guide researchers.
The present study therefore has two aims. First, it addresses the question of how classification systems used to reduce food variables before the application of PCA affect the observed association between diet and cognitive function. Second, it evaluates the association between dietary patterns and cognitive function in a population-based cohort of Australian adults. In addition, it aims to determine the variance in food use explained by the different variable reduction methods employed, i.e. using 101 individual food items, 32 food groups and 20 food groups.

2. Experimental Section

2.1. Study Design and Sample

The study utilised secondary data derived from the AusDiab study, a population-based national survey of the general (non-institutionalised) Australian population aged 25 years and older [22]. The baseline examination was undertaken in 1999–2000 (n = 11,247), with follow-ups conducted in 2004–2005 (n = 8798) and 2011–2012 (n = 6186) [22]. Dietary data were obtained from a sub-group of the sample using a questionnaire (Wave 1: n = 3298) [22]. Measurement of cognitive function was conducted on those who attended survey sites in the third Wave of data collection (n = 4764) [23]. The present study focuses on the older segment of the sample (age 60+ at baseline) that completed the food frequency questionnaire at Wave 1 and the mini-mental state examination and tests of memory, verbal ability and processing speed at Wave 3 (n = 577).
We excluded 2721 participants from the current analysis since these participants had no dietary and/or cognitive data recorded.

2.2. Cognitive Outcome Measurement

The mini-mental state examination (MMSE) was used for data collection in 2011–2012 (AusDiab Wave 3) to determine CI status. Participants were classified based on their MMSE score as either cognitively impaired (score of 0–23) or not cognitively impaired (score of 24–30) [24].
The California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) was used to assess memory using a 16-point scoring system. For this test, participants were asked to recall and repeat a list of 16 common shopping items that had been read to them by an interviewer. During a short delay of 20 min, during which participants were given other tasks to perform, the interviewer then asked the participant to recall the 16 common shopping list items again (delayed recall). The Spot-the-Word test (STW) was used in this study to test participants’ vocabulary and verbal knowledge with scores ranging from 0 to 60. STW testing involved presenting participants with pairs of items, one of which was a real word and the other a non-word, and then requiring participants to identify the word. Performance on the STW has not been shown to decline with age and is highly correlated with verbal acumen [25]. Finally, processing speed was tested using the Symbol-Digit Modalities Test (SDMT). Participants were provided with a reference key and asked to pair geometric figures with specific numbers. Using the SDMT, participants were scored from 0–60 on the number of correct answers provided in 90 s.

2.3. Food Consumption Data and Classification

The AusDiab semi-quantitative food frequency questionnaire consisted of 121 items that asked participants about their consumption of 101 food items [26]. This questionnaire assessed usual intake and recorded the amount and types of specific food items consumed by participants. In some cases, for example casseroles and potatoes, pictures of serving sizes were provided so that persons could indicate whether they had more or less of a given food item each day and each week, using the past 12 months as a reference. Participants were asked to specify the number of times they had specific food items in the past year by checking 1 of 10 frequency categories ranging from ‘never’ to ‘three or more times per day’. The average daily intake of food weight in grams was subsequently computed and used in the present analysis.
Three methods were used in order to classify these foods before applying PCA. In the first instance, the 101 individual food items asked about in the questionnaire were used (no categorisation). In the second and third instances, foods were combined and reduced to 32 and 20 food groups, respectively, based on nutrient content and culinary usage—a method employed in several other published studies for PCA [21,27] (see Table 1). Some foods were not categorised and were kept separate since they did not comfortably fit into any of the categories, e.g. pizza and meat pies [21,28]. More specifically, for the reduction of 101 items to 32 food groups, individual items were classed into groups, e.g., the item ‘Processed Meats’ was a tally of a participant’s bacon, ham, salami and sausage consumption in grams/day, while the item ‘Red Meats’ was a tally of beef, pork, lamb, veal and hamburger in grams/day. In the final classification system, the 32 food groups were further categorised into broader groups, which resulted in 20 food groups, e.g. the item ‘Meats’ was a tally of a participant’s ‘Processed Meats’ and ‘Red Meats’ consumption in grams/day.
Table 1. Food groupings used in the dietary pattern analysis.
Table 1. Food groupings used in the dietary pattern analysis.
Method 1Method 2Method 3
Food ItemFood CategoryFood Category
Bacon, ham, salami, sausagesProcessed MeatsMeat
Beef, pork, lamb, veal, hamburgerRed MeatsMeat
Fish, fried fish, tinned fishFishFish
ChickenPoultryPoultry
EggsEggsEggs
ButterButterFats and Oils
Margarine, poly/mono-unsaturated margarineMargarineFats and Oils
Butter and margarine blendsButter and Margarine BlendsFats and Oils
Reduced-fat/skim milk, low-fat cheese, yoghurtLow-fat Dairy ProductsDairy
Full-cream milk, hard/firm/soft/ricotta/cottage/cream cheese, ice-cream, flavoured-milk drinkHigh-fat Dairy ProductsDairy
Red/white/fortified wineWineAlcohol
Light/heavy beerBeerAlcohol
Other spiritsOther SpiritsAlcohol
Tinned fruit, oranges, apples, pears, bananas, melon, pineapple, strawberries, apricots, peaches, mangoFruitFruit
Fruit juiceFruit JuiceFruit Juice
Cabbage, cauliflower, broccoliCruciferous VegetablesVegetables
Carrot, pumpkinDark-yellow VegetablesVegetables
Tomatoes, tomato sauceTomatoesVegetables
Lettuce, spinachGreen, leafy VegetablesVegetables
Peas, green beans, bean sprouts, baked beans, tofu, other beans, soya milkLegumesVegetables
Cucumber, celery, beetroot, mushrooms, zucchini, capsicum, avocadoOther VegetablesVegetables
Onion, garlicGarlic and OnionsVegetables
PotatoesPotatoesVegetables
ChipsChips/French friesChips/French Fries
All-bran, bran flakes, Weet-Bix, cornflakes, porridge, muesli, wholemeal/rye/multi-grain breadWhole GrainsWhole Grains
High-fibre white/white bread, rice, pasta, crackersRefined GrainsRefined Grains
PizzaPizzaPizza
Sweet biscuits, cakes, crisps, chocolateSnacksSnacks
Nuts, peanut butterNutsNuts
Jam, vegemiteCondimentsCondiments
SugarSugarSugar
Meat piesMeat PiesMeat Pies

2.4. Statistical Analysis

PCA using SPSS version 22 was conducted to identify underlying dietary patterns. In determining the number of components to retain for further analysis, we considered component eigenvalues greater than 1 along with examination of scree plots. Components were rotated by an orthogonal (varimax) rotation to improve interpretability. Overall though, the comprehensibility and interpretability of the rotated factors were considered along with the aforementioned criteria. Similar to other studies, derived components were labelled based on our description of the observed patterns [29].
Dietary pattern scores were calculated for each individual at Wave 1 using all three classification methods (individual food items, 32 food groups and 20 food groups). Scores for an observed pattern were computed using the following equation: i =   j [ ( b i j / λ i ) X j ]   [29]. Variables with factor loadings of ≥0.30 were included in the weighted average [30,31].
Logistic regression analysis was performed to determine the association between dietary pattern scores at Wave 1 and cognitive status at Wave 3 using all three food item categorisation methods, i.e. PCA based on 101 individual food items, 32 food groups or 20 food groups. The interaction between dietary pattern score and exercise time was also examined to determine whether there was any association with cognitive status using all three food categorisation methods.
Generalized linear models (GLM) were used to estimate the associations between dietary pattern scores at Wave 1 and memory, verbal ability and processing speed using all three food variable reduction methods.

3. Results

Descriptive statistics for the sample are presented in Table 2. A total of 577 participants (49.22% female) had both diet and cognitive data recorded at Wave 1.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics at Wave 1 for the AusDiab sample included in the study (n = 577).
Table 2. Descriptive statistics at Wave 1 for the AusDiab sample included in the study (n = 577).
VariablesWave 1
Age Range60–83
Mean Age (SD)66.07 (4.85)
Female (%)284 (49.22)
BMI (SD)26.89 (4.09)
Secondary School (%)242 (24.4)
Tertiary Level (%)229 (40.1)
Other - Trade, Technician, Primary Only (%)100 (17.4)
Current Smoker (%)29 (5.1)
Ex-Smoker (%)182 (32.0)
Non-Smoker357 (62.9)
Exercise Mean (SD), mins./week292.45 (324.21)
MMSE Score27.41 (2.44)
CVLT Score5.17 (2.30)
STW Score50.30 (6.84)
SDMT Score38.63 (10.74)
Impaired (%)44 (7.63)

3.1. Dietary Pattern Analysis

Classification method affected the number and components of the patterns identified. Variable reduction using 20 food groups explained a greater proportion of variance in the sample than variable reduction using 32 food groups and 101 individual food items. Use of 20 food groups explained 30.74% of total variance in food use in the sample. Comparatively, use of 101 individual foods explained 23.22% and use of 32 food groups explained 29.74% of total variance in food use.
Table 3. Results of logistic regression analyses showing associations between CI at Wave 3 and dietary patterns obtained using 101 food items, 32 food groups and 20 food groups at Wave 1 (odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals shown in brackets).
Table 3. Results of logistic regression analyses showing associations between CI at Wave 3 and dietary patterns obtained using 101 food items, 32 food groups and 20 food groups at Wave 1 (odds ratios with 95% confidence intervals shown in brackets).
Dietary Pattern 1Dietary Pattern 2Dietary Pattern 3Dietary Pattern 4Dietary Pattern 5Dietary Pattern 6Dietary Pattern 7
101 Food ItemsFruit & Vegetable Snack & Processed FoodsVegetableMeatFish, Legumes & VegetableVegetable, Pasta & AlcoholDairy, Cereal & Eggs
OR (95% CI)1.061
(1.006–1.118)
p = 0.030 *
1.051
(0.967–1.143)
p = 0.239
0.986
(0.916–1.061)
p = 0.701
1.005
(0.964–1.048)
p = 0.806
1.032
(1.001–1.064)
p = 0.040 *
1.000
(0.965–1.037)
p = 0.994
1.020
(1.007–1.033)
p = 0.003 **
32 Food GroupsWesternPrudentVegetable, Grains & WineHigh-Fat
OR (95% CI)1.005
(0.994–1.016)
p = 0.409
0.997
(0.984–1.010)
p = 0.643
1.008
(0.995–1.020)
p = 0.229
0.999
(0.992–1.007)
20 Food GroupsVarietyWesternDairy, Grains & Alcohol
OR (95% CI)1.006
(0.994–1.018)
p = 0.333
1.008
(0.986–1.031)
p = 0.497
1.001
(0.998–1.005)
p = 0.383
Model adjusted for age, sex, energy, education, BMI, smoking status, STW and exercise time; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.

3.1.1. Wave 1 Dietary Patterns Using 101 Individual Food Items

Seven dietary patterns were extracted using PCA with varimax rotation. The rotated component matrix with factor loadings is shown in Supplementary Table 1.
The first dietary pattern identified was labelled ‘Fruit and Vegetable’ because of the high loadings of unprocessed fruit and vegetables observed. The second dietary pattern identified was labelled ‘Snack and Processed Food’ due to the high factor loadings observed for foods that could be qualified as such, e.g., cakes, jam, ice cream, sausages, and salami. Dietary pattern labels for the other observed dietary structures can be viewed in Supplementary Table 1. Together, the dietary patterns identified accounted for 23% of total variance in the sample.

3.1.2. Wave 1 Dietary Patterns Using 32 Food Groups

After applying PCA, four dietary patterns were extracted. The rotated component matrix with factor loadings is shown in Supplementary Table 2.
The first pattern identified was labelled ‘Western’ because of the predominantly high loadings of processed meats, refined grains and convenience foods. The second dietary pattern identified was labelled ‘Prudent’ and had characteristically high factor loadings of fish, vegetables and fruit. Dietary pattern labels for the other observed dietary structures can be viewed in Supplementary Table 4. Together, the dietary patterns identified accounted for 30% of total variance in the sample.

3.1.3. Wave 1 Dietary Patterns Using 20 Food Groups

Three dietary patterns were extracted using PCA with varimax rotation. The rotated component matrix with factor loadings is shown in Supplementary Table 3.
The first pattern identified was labelled ‘Variety’ because of the high loadings of a wide variety of foods—vegetables, fruit, fish, meat and nuts. The second dietary pattern was labelled ‘Western’ because of the high factor loadings of high-fat and high-sugar foods. The final dietary pattern was labelled ‘Dairy, Grains and Alcohol’ due to the high factor loadings of these foods recorded. Together, these dietary patterns identified accounted for 31% of total variance in the sample.

3.2. Dietary Pattern as a Predictor of CI Using the MMSE

Logistic regression analysis using dietary pattern scores obtained from all three variable reduction techniques (i.e., 101 individual food items, 32 food groups and 20 food groups) was conducted to examine the relationship between dietary pattern and CI. Covariates included the independent variables age, sex, energy, education, BMI, smoking status, exercise time and Spot-the-Word (as a control for premorbid intelligence) [32].
The only significant dietary predictors of CI were obtained using 101 individual food items. Three of the seven dietary patterns identified were observed to be significant predictors of CI. For every one unit increase in these pattern scores, the odds of CI decreased ((Fruit and Vegetable Pattern: p = 0.030, OR 1.061, confidence interval: 1.006–1.118); (Fish, Legumes and Vegetable Pattern: p = 0.040, OR 1.032, confidence interval: 1.001–1.064); (Dairy, Cereal and Eggs Pattern: p = 0.003, OR 1.020, confidence interval: 1.007–1.033)).
Table 4. Results of GLM showing associations between cognitive function at Wave 3 and dietary patterns obtained using 101 individual food items, 32 food groups and 20 food groups at Waves 1, 2 and 3 ( β values with Standard Errors shown in brackets).
Table 4. Results of GLM showing associations between cognitive function at Wave 3 and dietary patterns obtained using 101 individual food items, 32 food groups and 20 food groups at Waves 1, 2 and 3 ( β values with Standard Errors shown in brackets).
Dietary Pattern 1Dietary Pattern 2Dietary Pattern 3Dietary Pattern 4Dietary Pattern 5Dietary Pattern 6Dietary Pattern 7
101 Food ItemsFruit & Vegetable Snack & Processed FoodsVegetableMeatFish, Legumes & VegetableVegetable, Pasta & AlcoholDairy, Cereal & Eggs
CVLT0.012 (0.013)
p = 0.336
0.020 (0.015)
p = 0.186
−0.001 (0.012)
p = 0.930
0.000 (0.005)
p = 0.984
−0.002 (0.007)
p = 0.793
0.004 (0.006)
p = 0.551
−4.474 (0.002)
p = 0.986
SDMT0.097 (0.057)
p = 0.091
0.060 (0.067)
p = 0.365
0.013 (0.054)
p = 0.801
0.014 (0.023)
p = 0.536
−0.062 (0.032)
p = 0.054
−0.003 (0.028)
p = 0.916
−0.016 (0.011)
p = 0.149
STW0.077 (0.039)
p = 0.051
0.080 (0.046)
p = 0.086
0.046 (0.037)
p = 0.224
0.007 (0.020)
p = 0.722
0.000 (0.022)
p = 0.994
0.000 (0.021)
p = 0.982
0.002 (0.008)
p = 0.799
32 Food GroupsWesternPrudentVegetable, Grains & WineHigh-Fat
CVLT−0.008 (0.003)
p = 0.001 **
−0.005 (0.003)
p = 0.067
0.001 (0.003)
p = 0.764
−0.001 (0.001)
p = 0.711
SDMT−0.024 (0.011)
p = 0.035 *
−0.035 (0.011)
p = 0.002 **
0.024 (0.012)
p = 0.034 *
0.005 (0.006)
p = 0.403
STW−0.006 (0.008)
p = 0.467
−0.006 (0.008)
p = 0.425
0.013 (0.008)
p = 0.119
0.001 (0.005)
p = 0.774
20 Food GroupsVarietyWesternDairy, Grains & Alcohol
CVLT−0.003 (0.003)
p = 0.272
−0.004 (0.005)
p = 0.376
−0.002 (0.001)
p = 0.005**
SDMT−0.026 (0.011)
p = 0.018 *
−0.007 (0.021)
p = 0.740
−0.005 (0.003)
P = 0.149
STW−0.008 (0.008)
p = 0.291
0.002 (0.015)
p = 0.901
−0.001 (0.002)
p = 0.618
Model adjusted for age, sex, energy, education, BMI, smoking status, STW and exercise time; * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01.
When the interaction term ‘dietary pattern score × exercise time’ was included in the model, no significant results were obtained.

3.3. Dietary Pattern as a Predictor of Memory, Vocabulary and Verbal Knowledge and Processing Speed

Using dietary pattern scores calculated from 101 individual food items, there were no dietary patterns observed to be significantly predictive of memory, processing speed or verbal knowledge.
Using 32 food groups, however, the ‘Western’ dietary pattern was a predictor of poorer memory and processing speed ( β = −0.008, SE = 0.003, p = 0.001 and β = −0.024, SE = 0.011, p = 0.035). In addition, the ‘Prudent’ dietary pattern was also a predictor of poorer processing speed ( β = −0.035, SE = 0.011, p = 0.002).
When 20 food groups were used to calculate dietary pattern scores, the ‘Dairy, grains and alcohol’ dietary pattern was predictive of poorer memory while the ‘Variety’ dietary pattern was associated with poorer processing speed ( = −0.002, SE = 0.001, p = 0.005 and When 20 food groups were used to calculate dietary pattern scores, the ‘Dairy, grains and alcohol’ dietary pattern was predictive of poorer memory while the ‘Variety’ dietary pattern was associated with poorer processing speed ( β = −0.002, SE = 0.001, p = 0.005 and β = −0.026, SE = 0.011, p = 0.018 respectively).

4. Discussion

The present study is one of the few that use a data-driven method of dietary analysis to assess the relationship between diet and cognitive function in older Australian adults. A number of findings from this study are noteworthy. First, the broader the categories used in grouping foods, the greater the variability in food use that was explained. It was observed, however, that the results of logistic regression were more sensitive when dietary analysis was based on individual food items than food groups. From these data we observed that for every one unit increase in ‘Fruit and Vegetable’, ‘Fish, Legumes and Vegetable’ and ‘Dairy, Cereal and Eggs’ dietary pattern scores, the odds of CI decreased.
When looking at the relationship between dietary pattern and memory, processing speed and vocabulary, no significant results were observed using 101 individual food items. Using 32 food groups, the ‘Western’ dietary pattern was found to be predictive of poorer memory and processing speed, the ‘Vegetable, Grains and Wine’ pattern was a predictor of better processing speed while the ‘Prudent’ pattern was predictive of poorer processing speed. Using 20 food groups we observed that the ‘Variety’ dietary pattern was a predictor of poorer processing speed and the ‘Dairy, Grains and Alcohol’ pattern predictive of poorer memory.
We found that the method of reducing food variables affected the amount of variance in food use that was explained. Similarly to other published findings, the broader the categories used, the greater the variability in food use explained [21]. We suggest this may be due to the inclusion of foods that are both weakly and strongly correlated with a specific pattern in the broader categorisations which leads to an increase in the information captured [21]. Interestingly, for logistic regression analyses, it was only when the level of detail in the items included in PCA-derived dietary patterns increased that significant associations between diet and cognitive impairment were observed, i.e., it was variable reduction that utilised dietary pattern scores from individual foods that produced the only significant results. This may be because analysis using individual foods captures more meaningful results as it is able to show whether consumption or non-consumption of specific food items is associated with disease.
Our results are consistent with previous studies in showing that diets with high loadings of vegetables and other plant-based food items (fruit, grains and legumes) resulted in reduced odds of disease and improved cognitive function [21,27]. In a study of 6911 Chinese subjects aged 65 and older who formed part of the Chinese Longitudinal Health Longevity Study, lower intakes of vegetables and legumes were associated with cognitive decline when using MMSE as a measure of cognitive function [33]. Multivariate logistic regression showed that always eating vegetables and always consuming legumes were inversely associated with cognitive decline [33]. Additionally, in a study of 2,148 community-based elderly subjects without dementia in New York, higher intakes of cruciferous and dark and green leafy vegetables were found to be associated with a decreased risk of developing AD [34]. The benefits of diets high in vegetables extend beyond the cognitive domain. In a European study aimed to investigate the effect of the Mediterranean diet (MeDi) on mortality, greater adherence was associated with a more than 50% lower rate of all-causes and cause-specific mortality [35]. McCann et al. [21], in a study examining the effect of dietary patterns on estimation of endometrial cancer risk, found that dietary patterns high in fruit, vegetables and whole-grains resulted in reduced endometrial cancer risks. Similarly, dietary patterns high in vegetables, grain and fruit have been found to be associated with a modestly lower risk for type 2 diabetes [27]. This seemingly protective association between plant-based foods and disease may be the result of the high concentration of antioxidant nutrients present in vegetables and fruits and their role in suppressing inflammation. There is evidence that oxidative stress and inflammation can lead to impaired cognitive function because of an increase in free radicals and the damage they cause to neuronal cells [14].
The Mediterranean diet (MeDi), one of the most studied dietary patterns, describes a diet rich in cereals, olive oil, fish, fruits and vegetables and low in dairy and meat with a moderate consumption of red wine. This diet has been linked to increased survival, reduced risk of cancers, cardiovascular disease, longevity and CI [11,16,36,37,38]. In the present study, diets rich in vegetables, grain and wine were found to be predictive of better processing speed and diets high in vegetables and plant-based food items were generally associated with better cognitive outcomes.
Worth noting is the finding that the ‘Prudent’ dietary pattern was predictive of poorer processing speed. This dietary pattern was so labelled because of its high loadings of fish, fruit and vegetables, nuts and whole-grains. Perhaps an explanation of this lies in the method in which food items are prepared or in analysing whether there is actually a protective effect of foods contained in this pattern. For instance, while many studies have examined the effect of fish consumption on cognition, some clarification is still needed on the purported link between the two. In a study of 6150 Chicago residents aged 65 and older to examine whether intake of fish and omega-3 fatty acids protects against age-related cognitive decline, it was reported that fish consumption may be associated with slower cognitive decline with age [39]. Similar findings were also reported by Kalmijn et al. in 1997, who found statistically significant decreased risks of AD with higher fish consumption [40]. In two more recent Australian studies, one reported that higher fish consumption was associated with an increased risk of cognitive disorder [15] while the other found no evidence to support the hypothesis that higher proportions of fish intake benefits cognitive performance in normal older adults [41].
The ‘Variety’ dietary pattern was also found to be predictive of poorer processing speed. This pattern, so named because of high factor loadings in a variety of foods, is of interest because dietary guidelines for Australia and the rest of the world highlight the benefits of consuming a wide variety of foods. Therefore, in food and nutrition policy, there is a need to ensure that messages about the method of food preparation, processing and portion sizes of consumables are equally stressed.
In the present study, the ‘Western’ dietary pattern was predictive of poorer memory and processing speed. This is supported by other research which has reported that the ‘Western’ dietary pattern is associated with cognitive decline and reduced executive function [42].
One of the limitations of this study lies in its inability to report disease incidence as cognitive data were only collected at one time point (Wave 3). Additionally, no data were collected on executive function, and dietary intake is self-reported. There is also some subjectivity in determining food groups before application of PCA, but the method of food variable reduction we employed has been widely used in other studies [21,27]. It is also possible that the results observed may represent a selection bias, as only older adults with dietary and cognitive data were included in the study (n = 577). This has the effect of limiting the generalizability of the study’s findings. Finally, while we focused on the methodology of grouping foods in this paper, we were still unable to clearly identify guidelines for future researchers to follow. This is a major issue for diet-cognition research and suggests the need for further investigation and development of more robust and consensus-led methodologies in the field.
Despite its limitations, this study adds to the sparse body of literature examining the relationship between dietary patterns and CI among older adults, both in Australia and internationally. Furthermore, the study’s focus on older age groups whose dietary patterns have not been widely studied and reported is noteworthy. Finally, the study’s ability to answer a methodological question that has been one of the main critiques of PCA makes it noteworthy—how do variable reduction methods before the application of PCA affect the results obtained? This question is significant when examining the relationship between dietary patterns and cognition since there is a level of subjectivity involved in reducing food variables, and these can affect the observed associations with cognitive function [27].
Future studies examining the association between dietary intake and cognitive status will be useful to identify other patterns associated with CI and to examine more nuanced issues as they relate to diet and cognitive function.

5. Conclusions

Our findings showed that diets with high factor loadings of fruit, vegetables and plant-based food items conferred cognitive benefits, while those with high factor loadings of high-fat and convenience foods are linked to poorer cognitive outcomes. These results are similar to those of other studies which show that diets with high loadings of vegetables, fruit and grain reduce the odds of a myriad of diseases [21,27,43]. In addition, we demonstrated that the method of variable reduction in dietary studies may influence results, and suggest that further work is required to establish robust and replicable methods of dietary analysis for use in research into cognitive ageing. Additional studies that focus on the dietary habits of those over age 60 would be useful in order to further elucidate more specific details between dietary patterns, types and amount of fat, protein and carbohydrates, number of calories, and micro and macronutrients that are linked with optimal cognitive function and reduced risk of CI in older adults. Such information is required to provide support for the development of policies that promote optimal cognitive health in ageing.

Supplementary Files

Supplementary File 1

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence in Population Ageing Research (project number CE110001029).
AP is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council Career Development Award (1045456).
KJA is funded by NHMRC Fellowship #1002560. We acknowledge support from the NHMRC Dementia Collaborative Research Centres.
The AusDiab study co-coordinated by the Baker IDI Heart and Diabetes Institute, gratefully acknowledges the support and assistance given by: K Anstey, B Atkins, B Balkau, E Barr, A Cameron, S Chadban, M de Courten, D Dunstan, A Kavanagh, D Magliano, S Murray, N Owen, K Polkinghorne, J Shaw, T Welborn, P Zimmet and all the study participants.
Also, for funding or logistical support, we are grateful to: National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC grants 233200 and 1007544), Australian Government Department of Health and Ageing, Abbott Australasia Pty Ltd, Alphapharm Pty Ltd, Amgen Australia, AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, City Health Centre-Diabetes Service-Canberra, Department of Health and Community Services —Northern Territory, Department of Health and Human Services—Tasmania, Department of Health—New South Wales, Department of Health—Western Australia, Department of Health—South Australia, Department of Human Services—Victoria, Diabetes Australia, Diabetes Australia Northern Territory, Eli Lilly Australia, Estate of the Late Edward Wilson, GlaxoSmithKline, Jack Brockhoff Foundation, Janssen-Cilag, Kidney Health Australia, Marian & FH Flack Trust, Menzies Research Institute, Merck Sharp & Dohme, Novartis Pharmaceuticals, Novo Nordisk Pharmaceuticals, Pfizer Pty Ltd, Pratt Foundation, Queensland Health, Roche Diagnostics Australia, Royal Prince Alfred Hospital, Sydney, Sanofi Aventis, Sanofi-Synthelabo, and the Victorian Government’s OIS Program.

Author Contributions

Kimberly Ashby-Mitchell was responsible for designing the study, conducting analyses, interpreting the output of analyses and preparing the manuscript for submission. Anna Peeters interpreted data and revised the manuscript for intellectual content. Kaarin J. Anstey interpreted data and revised the manuscript for intellectual content.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have reported no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Cognitive Impairment: A Call for Action Now; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: Atlanta, GA, USA, 2011.
  2. Gillette-Guyonnet, S.; Secher, M.; Vellas, B. Nutrition and neurodegeneration: Epidemiological evidence and challenges for future research. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2013, 75, 738–755. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  3. McNeill, G.; Winter, J.; Jia, X. Diet and cognitive function in later life: A challenge for nutrition epidemiology. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2009, 63, S33–S37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Shatenstein, B.; Ferland, G.; Belleville, S.; Gray-Donald, K.; Kergoat, M.-J.; Morais, J.; Gaudreau, P.; Payette, H.; Greenwood, C. Diet quality and cognition among older adults from the NuAge study. Exp. Gerontol. 2012, 47, 353–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Barnes, D.E.; Yaffe, K. Predicting dementia: Role of dementia risk indices. Future Neurol. 2009, 4, 555–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Matthews, F.E.; Jagger, C.; Miller, L.L.; Brayne, C. Education differences in life expectancy with cognitive impairment. J. Gerontol. Ser. A: Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2009, 64, 125–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Andrade, C.; Radhakrishnan, R. The prevention and treatment of cognitive decline and dementia: An overview of recent research on experimental treatments. Indian J. Psychiatry 2009, 51, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Jacobs, D.R.; Steffen, L.M. Nutrients, foods, and dietary patterns as exposures in research: A framework for food synergy. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2003, 78, 508–513. [Google Scholar]
  9. Waijers, P.M.; Feskens, E.J.; Ocké, M.C. A critical review of predefined diet quality scores. Br. J. Nutr. 2007, 97, 219–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Jacobs, D.R.; Tapsell, L.C. Food, not nutrients, is the fundamental unit in nutrition. Nutr. Rev. 2007, 65, 439–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Scarmeas, N.; Stern, Y.; Mayeux, R.; Manly, J.J.; Schupf, N.; Luchsinger, J.A. Mediterranean diet and mild cognitive impairment. Arch. Neurol. 2009, 66, 216–225. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  12. Morris, M.; Evans, D.A.; Bienias, J.L.; Tangney, C.C.; Bennett, D.A.; Wilson, R.S.; Aggarwal, N.; Schneider, J. Consumption of fish and n-3 fatty acids and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Arch. Neurol. 2003, 60, 940–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Nelson, C.; Wengreen, H.J.; Munger, R.G.; Corcoran, C.D. Dietary folate, vitamin B-12, vitamin B-6 and incident Alzheimer’s disease: The Cache County memory, health, and aging study. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2009, 13, 899–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Wärnberg, J.; Gomez-Martinez, S.; Romeo, J.; Díaz, L.-E.; Marcos, A. Nutrition, inflammation, and cognitive function. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1153, 164–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Cherbuin, N.; Anstey, K.J. The Mediterranean diet is not related to cognitive change in a large prospective investigation: The PATH through life study. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2012, 20, 635–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Scarmeas, N.; Stern, Y.; Tang, M.-X.; Mayeux, R.; Luchsinger, J.A. Mediterranean diet and risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 2006, 59, 912–921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Hodge, A.; Almeida, O.P.; English, D.R.; Giles, G.G.; Flicker, L. Patterns of dietary intake and psychological distress in older Australians: Benefits not just from a Mediterranean diet. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2013, 25, 456–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Jacka, F.N.; Cherbuin, N.; Anstey, K.J.; Butterworth, P. Dietary patterns and depressive symptoms over time: Examining the relationships with socioeconomic position, health behaviours and cardiovascular risk. PLoS One 2014, 9, e87657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Reedy, J.; Wirfält, E.; Flood, A.; Mitrou, P.N.; Krebs-Smith, S.M.; Kipnis, V.; Midthune, D.; Leitzmann, M.; Hollenbeck, A.; Schatzkin, A. Comparing 3 dietary pattern methods—Cluster analysis, factor analysis, and index analysis—With colorectal cancer risk the NIH–AARP diet and health study. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2010, 171, 479–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Fabrigar, L.R.; Wegener, D.T.; MacCallum, R.C.; Strahan, E.J. Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychol. Methods 1999, 4, 272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. McCann, S.E.; Marshall, J.R.; Brasure, J.R.; Graham, S.; Freudenheim, J.L. Analysis of patterns of food intake in nutritional epidemiology: Food classification in principal components analysis and the subsequent impact on estimates for endometrial cancer. Public Health Nutr. 2001, 4, 989–997. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  22. Dunstan, D. Diabesity and Associated Disorders in Australia,2000: The Accelerating Epidemic: The Australian Diabetes, Obesity and Lifestyle Study (AusDiab); International Diabetes Institute: Melbourne, Australia, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  23. Tanamas, S.K. The Australian Diabetes, Obesity and Lifestyle Study; Baker IDI Heart and Diabetes Institute: Melbourne, Australia, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  24. Anstey, K.J.; von Sanden, C.; Luszcz, M.A. An 8-year prospective study of the relationship between cognitive performance and falling in very old adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2006, 54, 1169–1176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Baddeley, A.; Emslie, H.; Nimmo-Smith, I. The Spot-the-Word test: A robust estimate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision. Br. J. Clin. Psychol./Br. Psychol. Soc. 1993, 32, 55–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Grantham, N.M.; Magliano, D.J.; Hodge, A.; Jowett, J.; Meikle, P.; Shaw, J.E. The association between dairy food intake and the incidence of diabetes in Australia: The Australian diabetes obesity and lifestyle study (AusDiab). Public Health Nutr. 2013, 16, 339–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Van Dam, R.M.; Rimm, E.B.; Willett, W.C.; Stampfer, M.J.; Hu, F.B. Dietary patterns and risk for type 2 diabetes Mellitus in U.S. men. Ann. Intern. Med. 2002, 136, 201–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Khani, B.R.; Ye, W.; Terry, P.; Wolk, A. Reproducibility and validity of major dietary patterns among Swedish women assessed with a food-frequency questionnaire. J. Nutr. 2004, 134, 1541–1545. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  29. Hu, F.B.; Rimm, E.; Smith-Warner, S.A.; Feskanich, D.; Stampfer, M.J.; Ascherio, A.; Sampson, L.; Willett, W.C. Reproducibility and validity of dietary patterns assessed with a food-frequency questionnaire. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1999, 69, 243–249. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  30. Hosking, D.; Danthiir, V. Retrospective lifetime dietary patterns are associated with demographic and cardiovascular health variables in an older community-dwelling Australian population. Br. J. Nutr. 2013, 110, 2069–2083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Northstone, K.; Ness, A.; Emmett, P.; Rogers, I. Adjusting for energy intake in dietary pattern investigations using principal components analysis. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 62, 931–938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Crowell, T.A.; Vanderploeg, R.D.; Small, B.J.; Graves, A.B.; Mortimer, J.A. Elderly norms for the Spot-the-Word test. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2002, 17, 123–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Chen, X.; Huang, Y.; Cheng, H.G. Lower intake of vegetables and legumes associated with cognitive decline among illiterate elderly Chinese: A 3-year cohort study. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2012, 16, 549–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Gu, Y.; Nieves, J.W.; Stern, Y.; Luchsinger, J.A.; Scarmeas, N. Food combination and Alzheimer disease risk: A protective diet. Arch. Neurol. 2010, 67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Knoops, K.B.; de Groot, L.M.; Kromhout, D.; Perrin, A.E.; Moreiras-Varela, O.; Menotti, A.; van Staveren, W.A. Mediterranean diet, lifestyle factors, and 10-year mortality in elderly european men and women: The hale project. JAMA 2004, 292, 1433–1439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Sofi, F.; Abbate, R.; Gensini, G.F.; Casini, A. Accruing evidence on benefits of adherence to the Mediterranean diet on health: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2010, 92, 1189–1196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Solfrizzi, V.; Panza, F.; Frisardi, V.; Seripa, D.; Logroscino, G.; Imbimbo, B.P.; Pilotto, A. Diet and Alzheimer’s disease risk factors or prevention: The current evidence. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2011, 11, 677–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Trichopoulou, A.; Costacou, T.; Bamia, C.; Trichopoulos, D. Adherence to a Mediterranean diet and survival in a Greek population. N. Engl. J. Med. 2003, 348, 2599–2608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Morris, M.C. The role of nutrition in Alzheimer’s disease: Epidemiological evidence. Eur. J. Neurol.: Off. J. Eur. Fed. Neurol. Soc. 2009, 16 (Suppl. 1), 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  40. Kalmijn, S.; Launer, L.J.; Ott, A.; Witteman, J.C.; Hofman, A.; Breteler, M.M. Dietary fat intake and the risk of incident dementia in the Rotterdam Study. Ann. Neurol. 1997, 42, 776–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Danthiir, V.; Hosking, D.; Burns, N.R.; Wilson, C.; Nettelbeck, T.; Calvaresi, E.; Clifton, P.; Wittert, G.A. Cognitive performance in older adults is inversely associated with fish consumption but not erythrocyte membrane n-3 fatty acids. J. Nutr. 2014, 144, 311–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Gardener, S.L.; Rainey-Smith, S.R.; Barnes, M.B.; Sohrabi, H.R.; Weinborn, M.; Lim, Y.Y.; Harrington, K.; Taddei, K.; Gu, Y.; Rembach, A. Dietary patterns and cognitive decline in an Australian study of ageing. Mol. Psychiatry 2014, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Chan, R.; Chan, D.; Woo, J. A cross sectional study to examine the association between dietary patterns and cognitive impairment in older Chinese people in Hong Kong. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2013, 17, 757–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ashby-Mitchell, K.; Peeters, A.; Anstey, K.J. Role of Dietary Pattern Analysis in Determining Cognitive Status in Elderly Australian Adults. Nutrients 2015, 7, 1052-1067. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/nu7021052

AMA Style

Ashby-Mitchell K, Peeters A, Anstey KJ. Role of Dietary Pattern Analysis in Determining Cognitive Status in Elderly Australian Adults. Nutrients. 2015; 7(2):1052-1067. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/nu7021052

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ashby-Mitchell, Kimberly, Anna Peeters, and Kaarin J. Anstey. 2015. "Role of Dietary Pattern Analysis in Determining Cognitive Status in Elderly Australian Adults" Nutrients 7, no. 2: 1052-1067. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/nu7021052

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop