Next Article in Journal
Natural Occurrence of Deoxynivalenol and Its Acetylated Derivatives in Chinese Maize and Wheat Collected in 2017
Next Article in Special Issue
Special Issue: Evolutionary Ecology of Venom
Previous Article in Journal
ERβ and NFκB—Modulators of Zearalenone-Induced Oxidative Stress in Human Prostate Cancer Cells
Previous Article in Special Issue
Diet Breadth Mediates the Prey Specificity of Venom Potency in Snakes
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Fangs for the Memories? A Survey of Pain in Snakebite Patients Does Not Support a Strong Role for Defense in the Evolution of Snake Venom Composition

1
Molecular Ecology and Fisheries Genetics Laboratory, School of Natural Sciences, Bangor University, Bangor LL57 2UW, UK
2
Department of Biosciences, College of Science, Swansea University, Swansea SA2 8PP, UK
3
Snakebite Assist, Pretoria ZA-0001, South Africa
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 13 February 2020 / Revised: 16 March 2020 / Accepted: 19 March 2020 / Published: 22 March 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Evolutionary Ecology of Venom)

Abstract

:
Animals use venoms for multiple purposes, most prominently for prey acquisition and self-defense. In snakes, venom composition often evolves as a result of selection for optimization for local diet. However, whether selection for a defensive function has also played a role in driving the evolution of venom composition has remained largely unstudied. Here, we use an online survey of snakebite victims to test a key prediction of a defensive function, that envenoming should result in the rapid onset of severe pain. From the analysis of 584 snakebite reports, involving 192 species of venomous snake, we find that the vast majority of bites do not result in severe early pain. Phylogenetic comparative analysis shows that where early pain after a bite evolves, it is often lost rapidly. Our results, therefore, do not support the hypothesis that natural selection for antipredator defense played an important role in the origin of venom or front-fanged delivery systems in general, although there may be intriguing exceptions to this rule.
Key Contribution: Through an online survey of snakebite victims, we show that the venoms of a wide range of venomous snakes do not cause the early, severe pain typically associated with defensive venoms. This provides strong evidence against the hypothesis of widespread selection for a defensive function as a major driver of snake venom evolution.

1. Introduction

“Bee stings hurt. So do wasp stings, scorpion stings, the bites of centipedes, and the venom injections of many other animals, including snakes. To inflict pain is not necessarily to the advantage of an animal that uses its venom strictly for incapacitation of prey. In fact, it may be to its disadvantage because pain may induce increased struggling on the part of the prey. But venoms are also used defensively, and it is in that context that they may derive their effectiveness largely, if not exclusively, from their pain-inducing qualities. It is principally because venoms are painful that they can function in defense”.
—Eisner and Camazine [1]
Venoms are widespread across the animal kingdom, and have evolved numerous times in a broad range of phyla [2], with further examples still being discovered regularly, such as venomous crustaceans [3] and frogs [4]. The biological functions of venomous secretions include primarily predation and anti-predator defense, as well as intraspecific competition, reproduction, and digestion [2,5]. While a primary function can be identified for most venom systems, many venomous animals use their venoms for multiple purposes. In particular, animals with primarily foraging venoms frequently employ these for anti-predator defense [2].
Among venomous animals, snakes have received the greatest amount of research attention, due to their medical significance [6], and because the large volumes of venom secreted by many species greatly facilitate toxicological research. Snake venoms are highly variable in composition at all taxonomic levels, from ontogenetic variation within individuals [7] to geographic variation within species [8] and differences between higher taxa. The mechanisms and selective drivers of this variation have attracted extensive research attention.
Snakes use their venoms for both foraging and self-defense, but the relative importance of these as drivers of venom evolution has remained poorly understood. The “life-dinner principle” [9] suggests that defense, where the snake is fighting for its life, should take precedence over foraging efficiency, where a suboptimal strategy would merely result in reduced energy intake. However, most of the literature on the selective drivers shaping venom composition has focussed on the role of diet.
Studies in multiple taxa and using diverse approaches have accumulated a considerable body of evidence that many snake venoms have evolved under selection to optimize their prey-specific toxicity. Diet-related evolutionary effects were first discovered through correlations between venom composition and diet in Calloselasma rhodostoma [10]. Direct functional evidence in the shape of prey-specific lethality has been demonstrated on multiple occasions. For instance, the venoms of naturally arthropod-eating species of Echis and Vipera are more toxic to invertebrate prey than those of predominantly vertebrate-feeding congeneric species [11,12,13]. Prey-specific venom toxicity has also been detected in the venoms of different species of Sistrurus [14], and across multiple species of New World coral snakes (Micrurus) [15]. Among colubrid venoms, individual toxins with specific toxicity to avian and lizard prey have been documented in Boiga spp. [16,17,18], Oxybelis fulgidus [19] and Spilotes sulphureus [20]. Patterns of ontogenetic variation in venom composition in vipers have also been found to reflect ontogenetic diet changes [21]. Moreover, many prey species have evolved various levels of resistance to snake venoms [22,23,24], resulting in a toxic arms race that has led to prey-specific venom evolution in the snakes [25]. While the link between diet and venom composition may not be universal (e.g., Zancolli et al. [26]), the idea that venom composition is driven primarily by selection for prey subjugation has become the dominant paradigm in snake venom evolution.
While the venom system of most non-front-fanged snakes is of limited effectiveness against predators [27], we know from the global impact of snakebite that front-fanged venomous snakes frequently use their venoms in self-defense, often to devastating effect. This is supported by the evolution of highly specific defensive adaptations, such as hooding, tail vibration, scale rubbing and the rattle [28]. The frequent evolution of venom resistance among snake predators [25,29], predator avoidance of front-fanged snakes [30,31], the evolution of innate avoidance of characteristic venomous snake colour patterns [32,33], and the evolution of Batesian mimicry of front-fanged snakes [34,35] all indicate that venom can be an effective defense against at least some predators. However, whereas adaptation of venom composition to natural prey has become a well-documented phenomenon, we remain largely ignorant whether natural selection for defensive purposes may also have played a role in driving venom composition [36]. Harry W. Greene recognized this deficit in 2013 [28] predicting that “we’ll soon be asking if toxins had more to do with defense than heretofore realized”.
To test for selection for a defensive function, it is essential to first consider the requirements for a defensive venom: for a venom to be effective in that role, it must repel a predatory attack sufficiently rapidly for its producer to escape serious injury or death. This is most readily achieved through the rapid infliction of pain beyond that expected from the physical trauma of the bite alone [1,37]. In human patients, these characteristics are evident from clinical cases involving many primarily defensive animal venoms. For instance, virtually all venomous fish use their potent venoms solely for defense, invariably causing intense pain immediately upon envenomation [38,39,40,41,42]. Similarly, the entirely defensive venoms of non-predatory hymenopterans such as honeybees are equally notable for the immediate pain following the sting. Other invertebrates that use their venom for both predation and defense nevertheless include specifically pain-inducing toxins in their venom. This includes many scorpions [43] and centipedes of the genus Scolopendra that produce symptoms which, although rarely fatal to humans, are characterized by intense pain immediately upon envenomation, caused by a specific pain-causing toxin [44]. These offer examples of venom which are highly effective both in predatory and defensive contexts.
Whereas rapid-onset pain is ubiquitous and well documented in the examples of clearly defensive venoms, we lack systematic information on pain after snakebite. It is widely acknowledged that snakebites often entail significant or extreme pain [45,46,47]. However, the timeframe of its development is rarely stated. From anecdotal reports, we know that bites by many species result in great variation in the level and time course of pain experienced, with some bites resulting in immediate intense pain while others cause none [48]. Moreover, pain often appears to be a delayed symptom secondary to other venom effects, such as severe swelling or local tissue destruction [48]. Indeed, some snakes are notorious for the lack of early pain caused by their bites: for instance, in Bungarus envenomations, which often occur while the victim is asleep, initial pain is often never felt [49].
Limited evidence exists of specific pain-inducing toxins in certain species. Bohlen et al. [50] discovered the first snake venom toxin to specifically cause pain in the venom of Micrurus tener. MitTx was found to have no other function than to activate acid-sensing ion channels (ASICs), producing pain. MitTx has subsequently also been found in the venom of M. mosquitensis and M. nigrocinctus [51], but interestingly, the closely related M. fulvius lacks MitTx [52,53], indicating that this pain-inducing toxin is phylogenetically labile within Micrurus. More recently the Lys49 myotoxin BomoTx, found in the venom of Bothrops moojeni, was discovered to induce intense pain [54] through the promotion of ATP release, which consequently activates the P2X2 and/or P2X3 purinergic receptors. However, the relationship between the presence or absence of these toxins and the actual pain experienced by bitten adversaries has not been explored.
The very limited data currently available on the ability of different snake venoms to cause early pain post-bite restricts our ability to infer the role of antipredator defense in driving the evolution of snake venom composition. The assessment of pain from envenomation is potentially complicated by taxonomic differences in nociceptor function and pain perception. However, the structure of nociceptors appears to be highly conserved across both vertebrates and invertebrates, as does the central processing of nociception, which gives rise to the perception of pain [55]. There are exceptions to these rules, such as the lack of sensitivity to capsaicin in birds or to acidity in naked mole rats [55], and specific resistance in some specialized predators of venomous organisms, such as the specific blocking of scorpion venom-induced algesia documented in scorpion-feeding grasshopper mice (Onychomys sp.) [43]. However, it seems highly likely that most predators are likely to show similar patterns of nociceptor activation in response to venomous challenges, especially in terms of their time-course. This also suggests that the pain experience of a human snakebite patient is likely to be representative of that of other generalized predators.
Since the testing of nociceptor activation in the laboratory is time-consuming and may be difficult to relate to the perceived level of pain in vivo [44], we sought instead to assess the defensive potential of different snake venoms by using human snakebite victims as a model system that allows data on pain perception to be recalled and directly communicated. An increasing number of humans interact regularly and intentionally with venomous snakes in a professional capacity or as part of leisure activities, and as a result, numerous bites by a wide variety of snake species occur every year [56,57,58]. These well-informed bite victims represent a potentially valuable source of information on snakebite symptoms, as they are capable of providing positive identification of the snake species, and, due to their awareness of the risks of their activities, they are likely to be on average less susceptible to fear-induced memory distortions than unprepared victims of entirely unexpected ‘accidental’ bites. The large body of collective experience of snakebites among reptile workers thus represents an unparalleled source of information on the development of pain after snakebite.
Here, we exploit this collective reservoir of knowledge through the use of a questionnaire that seeks to establish the severity and, more importantly, the time course of pain development in patients envenomed by a diversity of snake species spanning the phylogenetic breadth of venomous caenophidians. We postulate that any venom at least partly shaped by selection for antipredator defense should cause pain of rapid onset to deter a predator in the critical early stages of any encounter, potentially giving the snake a chance to escape before being seriously injured or killed. While the presence of early pain after a bite does not necessarily indicate adaptation to a primarily defensive function, absence of early pain would preclude such a role. We also predict that any at least partly defensive venom should generate a trajectory of pain that would be consistent between patients: although the perceived intensity of pain from a bee sting may vary between individuals, they invariably cause early pain, and the same would be expected of other defensive venoms. The aim of this survey is thus not to compare absolute pain levels across snake species, but instead to begin to understand the pain trajectory as an ecologically informative attribute of snakebite in the context of defense.

2. Results

The distribution of sex to age of the 584 individual bite reports received in this study are shown in Table 1. In all snake families, mean pain levels within one and five minutes after the bite were considerably lower than the maximum pain level reported in the later phases of envenoming (Figure 1). The pain became too distracting for normal activities within the ecologically crucial first 5 min in only 14.55% of bite victims, and later than 5 min in another 30.82% (Figure 2). Remarkably, 54.62% reported never experiencing pain great enough to make normal activities impossible. Moreover, the pain experienced by different individuals bitten by the same species varied immensely, not only in its absolute level but also in its trajectory. Figure 3 shows the mean and individual pain trajectories for 12 representative and well-sampled species from all snake families. While absolute pain levels are likely to vary subjectively, the trajectory of pain development also varied extensively within many species (e.g., Agkistrodon contortrix, Vipera berus, Atractaspis bibronii), but was much more consistent in others (e.g., Crotalus atrox, Bitis arietans) (Figure 3).
Consistent with these results, and despite the phylogenetically widespread nature of envenomations causing early pain, our ancestral state estimates suggest that the majority of the history of venomous reptiles has been characterized by venoms causing little pain, particularly no early pain (Figure 4). Nevertheless, there are two prominent exceptions to this pattern: Elapidae and the New World radiation of pit vipers. Interestingly we estimate that these two deeper origins of early-pain-inducing venoms arose in different ways. In the ancestor of elapid snakes, the venom most likely caused early pain with little intraspecific variation, whereas in New World pit vipers intraspecific variation consisting of all three possible states (no, early, and late pain) is the estimated ancestral state (Figure 4). The estimated transition rates between states also suggest little evidence for a pervasive influence of a defensive function over the evolutionary history of venomous reptiles in general (Table 2). Specifically, states which include early pain (with or without intraspecific variation) tend to have higher transition rates which involve loss of early pain, suggesting it is not maintained by strong selection. Note that transition rates are not clade-specific but apply across the whole tree, so they do not preclude an effect of antipredator defense in particular clades (such as elapids as highlighted above), but suggest limited influence of defense in general.
The results from our variance partitioning analysis (using phylogenetic mixed models) suggest that most of the variation in levels of pain depends on the bitten individual (for immediate and early pain) and the phylogenetic history of the snake species which inflicted the bite (for the maximum pain resulting from the bite) (Figure 5). Phylogeny had a much stronger influence on the severity of pain than species-specific effects, which suggests that particular clades have characteristic venom compositions that influence the level of pain experienced from a bite. Nevertheless, despite explaining ~95% of the variance in maximum pain throughout the bite, any influence related to the snake responsible for the bite is relatively minor (~25%) for early pain-induction compared to victim characteristics (~75%). Because early pain is likely to be a key component of a defense-driven venom, our results suggest that, although there may be important differences between different clades of snakes, the overall evidence of selection for defense is limited. Note that we did find the predicted consistency across individual bites, which explains almost none of the variation (~0.3% for early pain and ~1.5% for maximum pain; Figure 5), but if we assume that humans are sufficiently analogous to other predators then the effect of the individual bitten suggests that early pain is likely to be particularly severe only in some bitten individuals.

3. Discussion

In summary, our results provide little evidence of pervasive selection for a defensive function in the evolution of snake venoms. The overall pattern from envenomed bites suggests that the majority of bites cause relatively little early pain, compared to the much higher levels of pain experienced later during the course of the envenoming. Strikingly, in the vast majority of bites sustained by our respondents, pain only became too distracting for other activities much later than during the first few minutes, and, even more surprisingly, in 54.62%, this never happened. This suggests that the venoms of these snakes would be ineffective in deterring a continued attack by a predator within an ecologically relevant timeframe.
Moreover, respondents bitten by some species had pain experiences that cannot be attributed solely to inter-individual differences in pain sensitivity, but that instead suggest intraspecific differences in venom activity. Even though we only considered bites with evidence of envenoming, some respondents bitten by species such as Crotalus atrox, Vipera berus and Notechis scutatus reported no pain whatsoever in the early or even later stages of envenoming, while others reported a strong later increase in pain, or even high early pain levels (Figure 3). Even accounting for individual differences in pain sensitivity, these extreme differences are difficult to reconcile with being due to identical venoms. Instead, they suggest intraspecific variation in venom composition with regard to algesic activity. This would be unexpected in a scenario of pervasive selection for a defensive function.
Similarly, the phylogenetic comparative analyses found little support for strong selection for a defensive function across the clade as a whole, and certainly not early in the caenophidian (or toxicoferan) radiation. Early pain as a consequence of venom appears to have evolved repeatedly, in particular, we find evidence for deeper origins at the base of the Elapidae and the New World pitvipers (with these deeper origins being far less likely to be explained by noise in the data). The evolution of consistent early pain in the Elapidae may be related to elapid venoms being typically more neurotoxic and so potentially targeting pain receptors directly (either as a directly selected or exapted effect), whereas in vipers the pain may be the result of SVMPs or similar toxins breaking down tissue and so is under weaker (if any) direct selection and is consequently more variable. This interpretation is consistent with our results from Figure 4, as the origin of early pain in elapids is estimated to be fairly consistently early pain, whereas in New World pitvipers it is estimated that bites could variably cause early, late, or no pain. If true, it suggests that elapid snakes are the best clade upon which to focus future efforts on understanding defense-driven evolution of pain. It also opens the intriguing possibility that spitting cobras (as the only snakes with unambiguously defensive adaptations of venom use) may have been exapted for defensive use of venom via early-pain inducing elapid ancestors.
Our estimated transition rates for pain trajectories find that the rate of loss of early pain was systematically higher than its rate of gain. This again suggests a lack of widespread selection pressure for a defensive function, as the early pain necessary for defense both evolves (relatively) infrequently and seems to have little selection pressure maintaining it when it does.
While the general pattern argues against a pervasive selection for defense, some taxa with divergent patterns such as relatively flat pain trajectories are worth noting, in particular Causus rhombeatus, Hydrodynastes gigas and especially Demansia psammophis (Figure 3). To identify outliers with potentially more defensive venoms, we explored the onset of incapacitating pain in the better-sampled species (N ≥ 5). Out of these 34 species, only four caused early incapacitating pain in more than 40% of all cases, andin more than 50% of those cases in which incapacitating pain occurred at all: Causus rhombeatus, Agkistrodon piscivorus, Pseudechis australis and the combined bites of all Demansia species. The individual variation in pain perceptions in the early stages of a bite may be relevant here as pain may only be experienced as incapacitating by a minor-moderate proportion of victims. In principle this could still lead to effective defense against some proportion of the predator community, but studies on variability in pain responses in natural predators are needed to further examine this possibility. In any case, even if effective against some predators, the inconsistency of the results adds to the weight of evidence against a strong role for defense in snake venom evolution in general. Moreover, we also stress again that early pain is necessary, but not sufficient, to infer selection for a defensive function, since it may also represent a mere side-effect of another venom activity.
In one of the few studies explicitly addressing the relationship between venom toxicological function and defensive adaptations, Panagides et al. [59] noted an association between the defensive adaptations of cobras (Naja) and relatives and venom cytotoxicity. They interpreted cytotoxicity as a defensive adaptation, on the assumption that it would be associated with greater pain. Our data do not support this inference, as the average pain trajectories of all Naja species in our dataset (but not Hemachatus) display the typical pattern of much lower pain in the first five minutes after the bite than later (Figure 6). Out of 26 Naja bites (all species), only four (15%), each by a different species, resulted in early incapacitating pain, and 19 (73%) never reached that pain level. While the sample sizes for the individual species are small, the emerging pattern does not support strong selection for a defensive function, contrary to the interpretation of Panagides et al. [59]. This may be because the assumption that cytotoxicity is a good proxy of early pain in cobras is incorrect. The clearest example of adaptations for defensive use of snake venoms is in venom spitting in cobras, which suggests that spitting cobras should cause more rapid early pain thannon-spitting species. Unfortunately, our sampling is insufficient to determine whether this is the case.
In contrast to the overall conclusions of this study, the evolution of specifically nociceptor-targeted toxins, such as BomoTX in Bothrops moojeni [54] and MitTX in Micrurus tener [50], strongly suggests a defensive function in those species. However, at least in coral snakes, the phylogenetically inconsistent distribution of this toxin argues against consistently strong selection for defense in this clade: MitTX is present in M. tener, M. nigrocinctus and M. mosquitensis, but absent in M. fulvius, the sister species of M. tener. A similar dimeric toxin is also present in the more distantly related M. dumerili and M. frontalis [60], and apparent homologues have been found in additional venoms [61], but again without a clear phylogenetic pattern.
The effect of these specifically algesic toxins on pain levels and trajectories in vivo remains largely unexplored. In a series of 39 bites by M. fulvius, which lacks MitTX, local pain appeared to be largely absent [62]. However, in another study [63], at last one patient bitten by the same species reported radiating pain. In comparison, 42.7% of 82 M. tener bites in Texas were followed by local pain (on an unknown timescale), but this was severe enough to require analgesia in only 15.9% [64]. A number of otherwise symptomatic patients in the latter series did not report pain, suggesting a lack of the kind of consistent pattern of early pain following fish or honeybee envenoming. Our sample of Micrurus bites is insufficient to add to this discussion, except to note that two bites by M. nigrocinctus resulted in little early pain.
Another factor arguing against pervasive selection for defense is the atrophy of the venom apparatus in snakes feeding on undefended prey. Among non-front-fanged snakes, the bird egg specialist Dasypeltis is phylogenetically nested in a clade of venomous opisthoglyphous genera such as Boiga and Telescopus [65], but its venom apparatus is atrophied [66]. Among front-fanged snakes, several elapid lineages that have specialized on the consumption of fish eggs (Emydocephalus spp. and Aipysurus eydouxii) display a greatly reduced venom apparatus and a series of deleterious mutations in their main toxin genes [67,68,69]. This suggests that, in the absence of a foraging function, there were no further selective pressures for the retention of a venom apparatus.
Inevitably, studies like the present one, that are based on the recollections of individuals that lived through a potentially traumatic experience, are likely to result in noisy data with multiple potential sources of error. These include faulty memory, subjective biases, individual differences in pain perception and tolerance, misidentification of snakes, and noise from a wide variety of unknown factors, such as site of bite, quantity and depth of venom injection, individual venom variation etc. Nevertheless, retrospective reports of pain intensity are commonly used and, given sufficient sample sizes, are often sufficiently reliable for epidemiological studies (e.g., Brauer et al. [70]).
Despite the inevitable statistical noise in survey data, they have allowed us to exploit the large body of collective experience accumulated by the herpetological community to assess the algesic properties of snakebites across a considerable breadth of snake diversity in a manner unachievable by other means. They have yielded a strongly supported and consistent pattern of limited early pain after snakebites, compared to higher maximum pain later, and a lack of early incapacitation from pain. This study thus adds to the evidence that venom in snakes has evolved for primarily foraging purposes and suggests that any effectiveness as a defensive adaptation is restricted to particular cases rather than a general (or early) driver of venom evolution.
This leads to the question of why, against the predictions of the life-dinner principle, selection for defense did not play a greater role in the evolution of snake venom. It may be that for the most part, biting is the final strategy in a snake’s defensive arsenal, because contact with the predator increases the risk of injury to the snake [71]. To reduce the necessity for this risk, snakes have evolved other defensive strategies that they employ before biting to deter and evade predation [72,73]. The evolution of behaviour to utilise alternative defensive strategies prior to biting may have reduced the selective pressures of defense upon the composition and toxicological effects of snake venoms.
Another reason may lie in the extremely lethal power of many front-fanged venomous snakes [74]. Numerous venomous organisms, such as insects and most fish, rely on painful rather than lethal venomous defenses, where individual predators are deterred by pain resulting from individual stings, and each sting needs to cause pain to generate that deterrence. Front-fanged venomous snakes have sufficient lethal potential to incapacitate or kill many predators. As a result, rather than relying on deterrence of individual predators through pain, deterrence may also develop through social learning in some predators witnessing the death, suffering or incapacitation of a conspecific or relative [34,75], or through natural selection for innate avoidance [32,33]. Neither of these mechanisms requires early pain or other specifically defensive adaptations of venom composition, but mathematical models have suggested instead that a quantitative increase in lethality may be selected for under some scenarios [76].
Despite the lack of pervasive selection for defense revealed here, the role of snake venom in antipredator defense, and the ecological and evolutionary factors that may influence such interactions, remain potentially rewarding subjects for further investigation. Currently, we lack even the most basic quantitative data on the use of venom in interactions between snakes and their predators, including any indication of how often snakes ever employ venom defensively, and how frequently this use of venom affects the outcome of these encounters. Although our current study suggests that defense has not been the primary driver of snake venom evolution in general, particularly early in the history of the clade, we also suggest that some exceptions may exist in certain groups. Moreover, groups that diverge from the majority of snakes in their use of venom, especially spitting cobras, may represent rewarding targets for more detailed investigations of when, why, and how antipredator defense might act as an important factor in snake venom evolution.

4. Materials and Methods

4.1. Questionnaire and Data Collection

To obtain data on the collective experience of snakebite pain from the herpetological community, we designed a questionnaire to chart the time course of pain development in envenomations. Respondents were asked to identify the species of snake they were bitten by and rate their level of pain experienced at three time periods: immediate (within 1 min post-bite), early (1 to 5 min post-bite) and later (maximum level of pain), utilizing an 11 point (0–10) Numeric Rating Scale (NRS) to give the most reliable account of pain experienced [77,78,79]. Respondents were then asked when the pain became ‘too distracting’ (distraction index), defined as how long after the bite the level of pain became too intense to continue with intended/normal activities: early (subgroups of: immediately, <1 min and 1 to 5 min), late (>5 min), or never.
Other questions related to the sex and age of the respondent at the time of the bite, the site of the envenomation on the body, and the sex, size and life stage of the snake responsible. No other medical symptoms were asked for, but many participants chose to include them, within the subsequent comment section. Where a respondent had received multiple envenomed bites, separate reports were collected for each event. Bite reports from both wild (51.2%) and captive (48.8%) snakes were included in the analyses. “Dry” bites without clinical symptoms of envenoming were excluded.
Professional herpetologists, herpetoculturists and herpetological fieldworkers were targeted to reduce the variance in pain perception in reports. To reach the largest audience of herpetologists possible, the questionnaire was created and distributed electronically using Google Forms (https://goo.gl/forms/A8FdnjVRqMnUnV162). The survey became publicly available in November 2016 and was extensively advertised via e-mail and shared on over 130 herpetological Facebook groups and via Twitter. Here, we analyze responses to the questionnaire received until December 2018. Bites were excluded from our analyses where the victim, at the time of the bite, was <10 years of age, or where a person submitted a report of snakebite sustained by a third party. After removal of reports based on the above criteria, and obviously erroneous or facetious entries, our final dataset used in further analyses contained reports of 584 individual bites, inflicted on 368 individual respondents by a total of 192 snake species.
Inevitably, self-reported, survey-based measures of pain will contain substantial statistical noise. However, as there is no obvious a priori reason to expect bias (systematic errors in a direction that are likely to mislead attempts to answer our specific questions), such noise is likely to be random. The effect of this should simply be to reduce the signal-to-noise ratio in the data, but our large sample size at all levels (bites, people, and snake species) should still provide sufficient statistical power to provide meaningful results. The full survey is available in Figure S1.
All subjects gave their informed consent for inclusion before they participated in the study. The protocol was approved by the Bangor University, College of Natural Sciences Ethics Committee on 14th November 2016 (CNS2016HWS01).

4.2. Data Analysis

Because we were using data across multiple species, we used a phylogenetic comparative approach to investigate the patterns of pain-inducing venoms across species and time [80]. We obtained a phylogeny from the TimeTree database [81] based on the list of species from our dataset. For this purpose (and hence subsequent comparative analyses) five records were removed as they represented unidentified species or hybrids. Thirty-nine species that were present in the dataset but not the TimeTree database were either added manually to the phylogeny at an appropriate position sister to congeneric species (10 species) where possible, or were replaced by ‘phylogenetically equivalent’ species [82] to download the tree (29 species). In the case of phylogenetic equivalent species, we corrected the replacement names to match our dataset after obtaining the phylogeny, so the names in our figures match the dataset. Overall, the final phylogeny and subsequent comparative analyses contained 192 species. Both the phylogeny and list of species, with notes on how species which were present in the dataset, but not the TimeTree database were included, is provided in Table S1. All comparative analyses were conducted in R v3.6.0 [83]. R Code used for analysis is provided in Supplementary Materials.
We used two types of comparative analyses in this study. First, we investigated how our ‘distraction index’ (no pain [‘never’ = pain never became distracting], rapid onset of pain [‘early’ = ≤ 5 min], or delayed onset of pain [‘late’ = > 5 min]) has evolved over the evolutionary history of venomous reptiles using ancestral state estimation. We used Bayesian stochastic mapping [84] implemented in phytools v0.6.99 [85] to estimate ancestral states. Because we had intraspecific variation in our trait of interest (distraction index), we coded our species to include this ‘polymorphism’ with 7 possible states: none, early, late, none+early, none+late, early+late, or none+early+late. This coding also allows us to group these states into those which include early pain onset and those that don’t after the analysis, improving the interpretability of the results in the context of our questions. Ancestral state estimates were based on 1000 simulations under an ‘all rates different’ model for which the transition rates were estimated from the data, as was the prior distribution at the root of the tree.
Second, we investigated how the variation in the magnitude of pain was partitioned for three time periods: immediately (within 1 min of the bite), early (within 5 min of the bite), and throughout the total duration of the bite (i.e., the maximum level of pain experienced). This gives a measure of consistency such that if a large proportion of the variance is attributed to snake species then the pain caused by a particular species should be fairly consistent, but this can vary greatly between snake species. If venom has been selected for a defensive role then we expect that variance explained by individual bites should be low (bites by a given species should be broadly consistent in pain induction). To test this, we constructed three phylogenetic mixed models using MCMCglmm v2.29 [86], one to predict the magnitude of pain (modelled as a Poisson distribution) at each of immediate, early, and maximum time periods. Note that phylogenetic mixed models are not restricted to one datapoint per species, so in addition to allowing us to evaluate intraspecific variation we were also able to use our total dataset of 584 observations for each model. The models included the distraction index as a fixed effect to control for any effects of pain trajectory on the level of pain experienced at a given time point, but inference is primarily based on the random effects included in each model. The random effects were the phylogeny (closely related species cause similar pain levels, distantly related species differ), snake species (pain levels vary based on the particular species that inflicted the bite, regardless of phylogeny), and victim (pain levels vary between people who are bitten). Note that the residual variance in this case can then be considered as an effect of individual bites (pain levels from every individual bite are unique) after accounting for the snake species and clade as well as the person bitten. MCMCglmm uses inverse Wishart prior distributions on parameters and we set all priors to have V = 1 and nu = 0.002. MCMC chains were run for 1.1 million generations, the first 100,000 of which were discarded as burning, and posterior samples were saved every 1000 generations. The quality of each model was checked using autocorrelation plots, Geweke plots, and effective sample sizes (all of which were over 500, mean = 852), and in all cases, models ran well.
We note that the two types of comparative analyses are independent in their interpretations. For instance, even if our mixed models find evidence for much of the variation in pain being due to the individual person bitten, the ancestral state estimations of distraction index are interpretable (despite being concerned mostly with snake species and clade levels). This is because they address two different questions. The mixed models are considering variation in the level of pain experienced, whereas the distraction index (for which we estimate ancestral states) is a measure of the trajectory of the level of pain over the bite. One way to think of this difference is that a bite could start off either mild or very painful and still gradually get worse (or better) over time. Note again that to guard against systematic associations between the level of pain and pain trajectory, our mixed models include the distraction index as a fixed effect (incorporated during the estimation of the variance partitioning of the random effects under examination).

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://0-www-mdpi-com.brum.beds.ac.uk/2072-6651/12/3/201/s1, Figure S1: Pain in Snakebite Questionnaire, R script for Bayesian stochastic mapping and phylogenetic mixed models, Table S1: TimeTree database species list with notes on alterations.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.W.-S. and W.W.; Methodology, H.W.-S., K.A. and W.W.; Software, H.W.-S. and K.A.; Formal Analysis, K.A.; Investigation, A.N., H.W.-S. and W.W.; Data Curation, H.W.-S.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, H.W.-S.; Writing—Review & Editing, H.W.-S., K.A. and W.W.; Visualization, H.W.-S. and K.A.; Supervision, W.W.; Project Administration, H.W.-S. All authors have read and agree to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Acknowledgments

We thank all survey respondents, without whose participation this study would not have been feasible. We also acknowledge the numerous Facebook group admins, who kindly allowed the survey to be posted to their pages, and the numerous individuals who engaged with and shared the questionnaire through social media. For ethics approval, we thank John Latchford of the Bangor University, College of Natural Sciences Ethics Committee. For his R coding advice, we thank Russel Gray.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Eisner, T.; Camazine, S. Spider leg autotomy induced by prey venom injection: An adaptive response to “pain”? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1983, 80, 3382–3385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Casewell, N.R.; Wüster, W.; Vonk, F.J.; Harrison, R.A.; Fry, B.G. Complex cocktails: The evolutionary novelty of venoms. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2013, 28, 219–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. von Reumont, B.M.; Blanke, A.; Richter, S.; Alvarez, F.; Belidorn, C.; Jenner, R.A. The first venomous crustacean revealed by transcriptomics and functional morphology: Remipede venom glands express a unique toxin cocktail dominated by enzymes and a neurotoxin. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2013, 31, 48–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Jared, C.; Mailho-Fontana, P.L.; Antoniazzi, M.A.; Mendes, V.A.; Barbaro, K.C.; Rodrigues, M.F.; Brodie, E.D., Jr. Venomous frogs use heads as weapons. Curr. Biol. 2015, 25, 2166–2170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Arbuckle, K. Evolutionary context of venom in animals. Evol. Venom. Anim. Toxins 2015, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Gutiérrez, J.M.; Calvete, J.J.; Habib, A.G.; Harrison, R.A.; Williams, D.J.; Warrell, D.A. Snakebite envenoming. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 2017, 3, 17063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Durban, J.; Pérez, A.; Sanz, L.; Gómez, A.; Bonilla, F.; Rodríguez, S.; Chacón, D.; Sasa, M.; Angulo, Y.; Gutiérrez, J.M.; et al. Integrated “omics” profiling indicates that miRNAs are modulators of the ontogenetic venom composition shift in the Central American rattlesnake, Crotalus simus simus. BMC Genom. 2013, 14, 234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Glenn, J.L.; Straight, R. Mojave Rattlesnake Crotalus scutulatus scutulatus venom: Variation in toxicity with geographical origin. Toxicon 1978, 16, 81–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Dawkins, R.; Krebs, J.R. Arms races between and within species. Proc. R. Soc. B 1979, 205, 489–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Daltry, J.C.; Wüster, W.; Thorpe, R.S. Diet and snake venom evolution. Nature 1996, 379, 537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Barlow, A.; Pook, C.E.; Harrison, R.A.; Wüster, W. Coevolution of diet and prey-specific venom activity supports the role of selection in snake venom evolution. Proc. R. Soc. B 2009, 276, 2443–2449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Richards, D.P.; Barlow, A.; Wüster, W. Venom lethality and diet: Differential responses of natural prey and model organisms to the venom of the saw-scaled vipers (Echis). Toxicon 2012, 59, 110–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Starkov, V.G.; Osipov, A.V.; Utkin, Y.N. Toxicity of venoms from vipers of Pelias group to crickets Gryllus assimilis and its relation to snake entomophagy. Toxicon 2007, 49, 995–1001. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Gibbs, H.L.; Mackessy, S.P. Functional basis of a molecular adaptation: Prey-specific toxic effects of venom from Sistrurus rattlesnakes. Toxicon 2009, 53, 672–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. da Silva, N.J., Jr.; Aird, S.D. Prey specificity, comparative lethality and compositional differences of coral snake venoms. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Part C Toxicol. Pharmacol. 2001, 128, 425–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Dashevsky, D.; Debono, J.; Rokyta, D.; Nouwens, A.; Josh, P.; Fry, B.G. Three-finger toxin diversification in the venoms of cat-eye snakes (Colubridae: Boiga). J. Mol. Evol. 2018, 86, 531–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Mackessy, S.P.; Sixberry, N.M.; Heyborne, W.H.; Fritts, T. Venom of the brown treesnake, Boiga irregularis: Ontogenetic shifts and taxa-specific toxicity. Toxicon 2006, 47, 537–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Pawlak, J.; Mackessy, S.P.; Fry, B.G.; Bhatia, M.; Mourier, G.; Fruchart-Gaillard, C.; Servent, D.; Ménez, R.; Stura, E.; Ménez, A.; et al. Denmotoxin, a three-finger toxin from the colubrid snake Boiga dendrophila (Mangrove Catsnake) with bird-specific activity. J. Biol. Chem. 2006, 281, 29030–29041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Heyborne, W.H.; Mackessy, S.P. Identification and characterization of a taxon-specific three-finger toxin from the venom of the Green Vinesnake (Oxybelis fulgidus; family Colubridae). Biochimie 2013, 95, 1923–1932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Modahl, C.M.; Mrinalini; Frietze, S.; Mackessy, S.P. Adaptive evolution of distinct prey-specific toxin genes in rear-fanged snake venom. Proc. R. Soc. B 2018, 285, 20181003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Andrade, D.V.; Abe, A.S. Relationship of venom ontogeny and diet in Bothrops. Herpetologica 1999, 55, 200–204. [Google Scholar]
  22. Perez, J.C.; Haws, W.C.; Garcia, V.E.; Jennings, B.M. Resistance of warm-blooded animals to snake venoms. Toxicon 1978, 16, 375–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Poran, N.S.; Coss, R.G.; Benjamini, E.L.I. Resistance of California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi) to the venom of the northern Pacific rattlesnake (Crotalus viridis oreganus): A study of adaptive variation. Toxicon 1987, 25, 767–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Heatwole, H.; Poran, N.S. Resistances of sympatric and allopatric eels to sea snake venoms. Copeia 1995, 1995, 136–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Holding, M.L.; Biardi, J.E.; Gibbs, H.L. Coevolution of venom function and venom resistance in a rattlesnake predator and its squirrel prey. Proc. R. Soc. B 2016, 283, 20152841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Zancolli, G.; Calvete, J.J.; Cardwell, M.D.; Greene, H.W.; Hayes, W.K.; Hegarty, M.J.; Herrmann, H.W.; Holycross, A.T.; Lannutti, D.I.; Mulley, J.F.; et al. When one phenotype is not enough: Divergent evolutionary trajectories govern venom variation in a widespread rattlesnake species. Proc. R. Soc. B 2019, 286, 20182735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Weinstein, S.A.; Warrell, D.A.; White, J.; Keyler, D.E. “Venomous Bites from Non-Venomous Snakes: A Critical Analysis of Risk and Management of “Colubrid Snake Bites; Elsevier: London, UK, 2011; ISBN 978-0123877321. [Google Scholar]
  28. Greene, H.W. Tracks and Shadows, Field Biology as Art; University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-0520232754. [Google Scholar]
  29. Holding, M.L.; Drabeck, D.H.; Jansa, S.A.; Gibbs, H.L. Venom resistance as a model for understanding the molecular basis of complex coevolutionary adaptations. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2016, 56, 1032–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Brodie, E.D., III. Differential avoidance of coral snake banded patterns by free-ranging avian predators in Costa Rica. Evolution 1993, 47, 227–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Wüster, W.; Allum, C.S.; Bjargardóttir, I.B.; Bailey, K.L.; Dawson, K.J.; Guenioui, J.; Lewis, J.; McGurk, J.; Moore, A.G.; Niskanen, M.; et al. Do aposematism and Batesian mimicry require bright colours? A test, using European viper markings. Proc. R. Soc. B 2004, 271, 2495–2499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Smith, S.M. Innate recognition of coral snake pattern by a possible avian predator. Science 1975, 187, 759–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Smith, S.M. Coral snake pattern recognition and stimulus generalization by naïve great kiskadees (Aves: Tyrannidae). Nature 1977, 265, 535–536. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Greene, H.W.; McDiarmid, R.W. Coral snake mimicry: Does it occur? Science 1981, 213, 1207–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Davis Rabosky, A.R.; Cox, C.L.; Rabosky, D.L.; Title, P.O.; Holmes, I.A.; Feldman, A.; McGuire, J.A. Coral snakes predict the evolution of mimicry across New World snakes. Nat. Commun. 2017, 7, 11484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Greene, H.W. Snakes: The Evolution of Mystery in Nature; University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA, 1997; ISBN 978-0520224872. [Google Scholar]
  37. Chahl, L.A.; Kirk, E.J. Toxins which produce pain. Pain 1975, 1, 3–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Fernandez, I.; Valladolid, G.; Varon, J.; Sternbach, G. Encounters with venomous sea-life. J. Emerg. Med. 2011, 40, 103–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Haddad, V.; Stolf, H.O.; Risk, J.Y.; França, F.O.; Cardoso, J.L.C. Report of 15 injuries caused by lionfish (Pterois volitans) in aquarists in Brazil: A critical assessment of the severity of envenomations. J. Venom. Anim. Toxins Incl. Trop. Dis. 2015, 21, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Kizer, K.W.; McKinney, H.E.; Auerbach, P.S. Scorpaenidae envenomation: A five-year poison center experience. JAMA 1985, 253, 807–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Patel, M.R.; Wells, S. Lionfish envenomation of the hand. J. Hand Surg. 1993, 18, 523–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Vetrano, S.J.; Lebowitz, J.B.; Marcus, S. Lionfish envenomation. J. Emerg. Med. 2002, 23, 379–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Rowe, A.H.; Xiao, Y.; Rowe, M.P.; Cummins, T.R.; Zakon, H.H. Voltage-gated sodium channel in grasshopper mice defends against bark scorpion toxin. Science 2013, 342, 441–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Yang, S.; Yang, F.; Wei, N.; Hong, J.; Li, B.; Luo, L.; Rong, M.; Yarov-Yarovoy, V.; Zheng, J.; Wang, K.; et al. A pain-inducing centipede toxin targets the heat activation machinery of nociceptor TRPV1. Nat. Commun. 2015, 6, 8297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Warrell, D.A. Clinical toxicology of snakebite in Africa and the Middle East/Arabian Peninsula. In Handbook of Clinical Toxicology of Animal Venoms and Poisons; Meier, J., White, J., Eds.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 1995; pp. 433–492. ISBN 978-0849344893. [Google Scholar]
  46. Warrell, D.A. Clinical toxicology of snakebite in Asia. In Handbook of Clinical Toxicology of Animal Venoms and Poisons; Meier, J., White, J., Eds.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 1995; pp. 493–594. ISBN 978-0849344893. [Google Scholar]
  47. Warrell, D.A. Snakebites in Central and South America: Epidemiology, clinical features, clinical management. In The Venomous Reptiles of the Western Hemisphere; Campbell, J.A., Lamar, W.W., Eds.; Cornell University Press: Ithaca, NY, USA, 2004; pp. 709–761. ISBN 978-0801441417. [Google Scholar]
  48. Russell, F.E.; Wainschel., J.; Carlson, R.W.; Osborne, A. Snake venom poisoning in the United States. In Animal, Plant and Microbial Toxins, Volume 2 Chemistry, Pharmacology and Immunology; Ohsaka, A., Hayashi, K., Sawai, Y., Eds.; Springer: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1980; pp. 139–234. ISBN 978-1468408911. [Google Scholar]
  49. Ariaratnam, C.A.; Sheriff, M.R.; Theakston, R.D.G.; Warrell, D.A. Distinctive epidemiologic and clinical features of common krait (Bungarus caeruleus) bites in Sri Lanka. Am. J. Trop. Med. Hyg. 2008, 79, 458–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Bohlen, C.J.; Chesler, A.T.; Sharif-Naeini, R.; Medzihradszky, K.F.; Zhou, S.; King, D.; Sánchez, E.E.; Burlingame, A.L.; Basbaum, A.I.; Julius, D. A heteromeric Texas coral snake toxin targets acid-sensing ion channels to produce pain. Nature 2011, 479, 410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Fernández, J.; Vargas-Vargas, N.; Pla, D.; Sasa, M.; Rey-Suárez, P.; Sanz, L.; Gutiérrez, J.M.; Calvete, J.J.; Lomonte, B. Snake venomics of Micrurus alleni and Micrurus mosquitensis from the Caribbean region of Costa Rica reveals two divergent compositional patterns in New World elapids. Toxicon 2015, 107, 217–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Margres, M.J.; Aronow, K.; Loyacano, J.; Rokyta, D.R. The venom-gland transcriptome of the eastern coral snake (Micrurus fulvius) reveals high venom complexity in the intragenomic evolution of venoms. BMC Genom. 2013, 14, 531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Vergara, I.; Pedraza-Escalona, M.; Paniagua, D.; Restano-Cassulini, R.; Zamudio, F.; Batista, C.V.; Possani, L.D.; Alagón, A. Eastern coral snake Micrurus fulvius venom toxicity in mice is mainly determined by neurotoxic phospholipases A2. J. Proteom. 2014, 105, 295–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Zhang, C.; Medzihradszky, K.F.; Sánchez, E.E.; Basbaum, A.I.; Julius, D. Lys49 myotoxin from the Brazilian lancehead pit viper elicits pain through regulated ATP release. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, E2524–E2532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Sneddon, L.U. Comparative physiology of nociception and pain. Physiology 2018, 33, 63–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Warrell, D.A. Commissioned article: Management of exotic snakebites. QJM Int. J. Med. 2009, 102, 593–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Isbister, G.K.; Brown, S.G.A.; ASP Investigators. Bites in Australian snake handlers—Australian snakebite project (ASP-15). QJM Int. J. Med. 2012, 105, 1089–1095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Valenta, J.; Stach, Z.; Michalek, P. Exotic snake bites in the Czech Republic—Epidemiological and clinical aspects during 15-year period (1999–2013). Clin. Toxicol. 2014, 52, 258–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Panagides, N.; Jackson, T.; Ikonomopoulou, M.; Arbuckle, K.; Pretzler, R.; Yang, D.; Ali, S.; Koludarov, I.; Dobson, J.; Sanker, B.; et al. How the cobra got its flesh-eating venom: Cytotoxicity as a defensive innovation and its co-evolution with hooding, aposematic marking, and spitting. Toxins 2017, 9, 103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Sanz, L.; de Freitas-Lima, L.N.; Quesada-Bernat, S.; Graça-de-Souza, V.K.; Soares, A.M.; Calderón, L.D.A.; Calvete, J.J.; Caldeira, C.A. Comparative venomics of Brazilian coral snakes: Micrurus frontalis, Micrurus spixii spixii, and Micrurus surinamensis. Toxicon 2019, 166, 39–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Aird, S.; da Silva, N.; Qiu, L.; Villar-Briones, A.; Saddi, V.; Pires de Campos Telles, M.; Grau, M.; Mikheyev, A. Coralsnake venomics: Analyses of venom gland transcriptomes and proteomes of six Brazilian taxa. Toxins 2017, 9, 187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Kitchens, C.S.; Van Mierop, L.H. Envenomation by the eastern coral snake (Micrurus fulvius fulvius): A study of 39 victims. J. Am. Med Assoc. 1987, 258, 1615–1618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Sasaki, J.; Khalil, P.A.; Chegondi, M.; Raszynski, A.; Meyer, K.G.; Totapally, B.R. Coral snake bites and envenomation in children: A case series. Pediatric Emerg. Care 2014, 30, 262–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Morgan, D.L.; Borys, D.J.; Stanford, R.; Kjar, D.; Tobleman, W. Texas coral snake (Micrurus tener) bites. South. Med. J. 2007, 100, 152–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Figueroa, A.; McKelvy, A.D.; Grismer, L.L.; Bell, C.D.; Lailvaux, S.P. A species-level phylogeny of extant snakes with description of a new colubrid subfamily and genus. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0161070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Fry, B.G.; Scheib, H.; van der Weerd, L.; Young, B.; McNaughtan, J.; Ramjan, S.R.; Vidal, N.; Poelmann, R.E.; Norman, J.A. Evolution of an arsenal: Structural and functional diversification of the venom system in the advanced snakes (Caenophidia). Mol. Cell. Proteom. 2008, 7, 215–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Gopalakrishnakone, P.; Wollberg, M.; Kochva, E. The venom apparatus of sea snakes. In Sea Snake Toxinology; Gopalakrishnakone, P., Ed.; Singapore University Press: Singapore, 1994; pp. 37–65. ISBN 9789971691912. [Google Scholar]
  68. Li, M.; Fry, B.G.; Kini, R.M. Eggs-only diet: Its implications for the toxin profile changes and ecology of the marbled sea snake (Aipysurus eydouxii). J. Mol. Evol. 2005, 60, 81–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Li, M.; Fry, B.G.; Kini, R.M. Putting the brakes on snake venom evolution: The unique molecular evolutionary patterns of Aipysurus eydouxii (Marbled sea snake) phospholipase A2 toxins. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2005, 22, 934–941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Brauer, C.; Thomsen, J.F.; Loft, I.P.; Mikkelsen, S. Can we rely on retrospective pain assessments? Am. J. Epidemiol. 2003, 157, 552–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Wüster, W.; Thorpe, R.S. Dentitional phenomena in cobras revisited: Spitting and fang structure in the Asiatic species of Naja (Serpentes: Elapidae). Herpetologica 1992, 424–434. [Google Scholar]
  72. Greene, H.W. Antipredator mechanisms in reptiles. In Biology of the Reptilia; Gans, C., Huey, R.B., Eds.; Alan, R. Liss: New York, NY, USA, 1988; Volume 16, pp. 1–152. ISBN 978-0-8451-4402-2. [Google Scholar]
  73. Martins, M. Defensive tactics in lizards and snakes: The potential contribution of the Neotropical fauna. An. Etol. 1996, 14, 185–199. [Google Scholar]
  74. Pough, F.H. Mimicry of vertebrates: Are the rules different? Am. Nat. 1988, 131, S67–S102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Klopfer, P.H. An experiment on empathic learning in ducks. Am. Nat. 1957, 91, 61–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Gangur, A.N.; Seymour, J.E.; Liddell, M.J.; Wilson, D.; Smout, M.J.; Northfield, T.D. When is overkill optimal? Tritrophic interactions reveal new insights into venom evolution. Theor. Ecol. 2018, 11, 141–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Downie, W.W.; Leatham, P.A.; Rhind, V.M.; Wright, V.; Branco, J.A.; Anderson, J.A. Studies with pain rating scales. Ann. Rheum. Dis. 1978, 37, 378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Hartrick, C.T.; Kovan, J.P.; Shapiro, S. The numeric rating scale for clinical pain measurement: A ratio measure? Pain Pract. 2003, 3, 310–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Williamson, A.; Hoggart, B. Pain: A review of three commonly used pain rating scales. J. Clin. Nurs. 2005, 14, 798–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Arbuckle, K. Phylogenetic comparative methods can provide important insights into the evolution of toxic weaponry. Toxins 2018, 10, 518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Kumar, S.; Stecher, G.; Suleski, M.; Hedges, S.B. TimeTree: A resource for timelines, timetrees, and divergence times. Mol. Biol. Evol. 2017, 34, 1812–1819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  82. Pennell, M.W.; FitzJohn, R.G.; Cornwell, W.K. A simple approach for maximizing the overlap of phylogenetic and comparative data. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2016, 7, 751–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Version 3.6.0. 2019. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 May 2019).
  84. Bollback, J.P. SIMMAP: Stochastic character mapping of discrete traits on phylogenies. BMC Bioinform. 2006, 7, 88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Revell, L.J. Phytools: An R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol. Evol. 2012, 3, 217–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Hadfield, J.D. MCMC methods for multi-response generalized linear mixed models: The MCMCglmm R package. J. Stat. Softw. 2010, 33, 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Mean pain Numeric Rating Scale (NRS) (on a 0–10 scale) across four major clades of venomous snakes within the first minute after the bite, within 1–5 min after the bite, and the maximum pain experienced at any time.
Figure 1. Mean pain Numeric Rating Scale (NRS) (on a 0–10 scale) across four major clades of venomous snakes within the first minute after the bite, within 1–5 min after the bite, and the maximum pain experienced at any time.
Toxins 12 00201 g001
Figure 2. Percentage of bites where pain became too intense/distracting to continue with intended/normal activities across three time periods; early (within 5 min), late (after over 5 min), and never.
Figure 2. Percentage of bites where pain became too intense/distracting to continue with intended/normal activities across three time periods; early (within 5 min), late (after over 5 min), and never.
Toxins 12 00201 g002
Figure 3. Variation shown of NRS at early, mid and max levels of pain of some of the best-sampled species, and species with unusual patterns. Data plotted of 11 point NRS, where 0 = no pain felt at all and 10 = the maximum level of pain imaginable. Crotalinae: (a) Agkistrodon contortrix [n = 28], (b) Crotalus atrox [n = 24], (c) Crotalus horridus [n = 13]; Viperinae: (d) Bitis arietans [n = 14], (e) Causus rhombeatus [n = 5], (f) Vipera berus [n = 40]; Elapidae: (g) Demansia psammophis [n = 9], (h) Notechis scutatus [n = 9], (i) Pseudechis porphyriacus [n=9]; Atractaspidinae: (j) Atractaspis bibronii [n = 14]; Colubridae: (k) Heterodon nasicus [n = 14], (l) Hydrodynastes gigas [n = 13]. Black dashed lines plot the mean trajectory and value at each time point. Note the relatively flat lines in Causus rhombeatus, Demansia psammophis and Hydrodynastes gigas, indicating a relatively early onset of pain.
Figure 3. Variation shown of NRS at early, mid and max levels of pain of some of the best-sampled species, and species with unusual patterns. Data plotted of 11 point NRS, where 0 = no pain felt at all and 10 = the maximum level of pain imaginable. Crotalinae: (a) Agkistrodon contortrix [n = 28], (b) Crotalus atrox [n = 24], (c) Crotalus horridus [n = 13]; Viperinae: (d) Bitis arietans [n = 14], (e) Causus rhombeatus [n = 5], (f) Vipera berus [n = 40]; Elapidae: (g) Demansia psammophis [n = 9], (h) Notechis scutatus [n = 9], (i) Pseudechis porphyriacus [n=9]; Atractaspidinae: (j) Atractaspis bibronii [n = 14]; Colubridae: (k) Heterodon nasicus [n = 14], (l) Hydrodynastes gigas [n = 13]. Black dashed lines plot the mean trajectory and value at each time point. Note the relatively flat lines in Causus rhombeatus, Demansia psammophis and Hydrodynastes gigas, indicating a relatively early onset of pain.
Toxins 12 00201 g003
Figure 4. Ancestral state estimates for pain trajectories showing either full coding of each individual combination of states (a) or simplified into either inducing early pain (with or without variation) or not (b). Venoms that cause early pain occur frequently across the current diversity of venomous reptiles but with two major exceptions (Elapidae and New World pitvipers) are mostly independent origins.
Figure 4. Ancestral state estimates for pain trajectories showing either full coding of each individual combination of states (a) or simplified into either inducing early pain (with or without variation) or not (b). Venoms that cause early pain occur frequently across the current diversity of venomous reptiles but with two major exceptions (Elapidae and New World pitvipers) are mostly independent origins.
Toxins 12 00201 g004aToxins 12 00201 g004b
Figure 5. Variance in the magnitude of immediate, early, and maximum pain experienced by snakebite victims as explained by variation between individual bites, individual victims, snake species, and snake phylogeny. Note that individual bites are only a minor source of variation after accounting for the snake and the bitten individual, and there is a stark difference between variance components of the level of pain within the first 5 min (which mostly varies based on the bitten individual) and the maximum level of pain experienced over the course of the envenomation (which is mostly related to the phylogenetic history of the snake species involved).
Figure 5. Variance in the magnitude of immediate, early, and maximum pain experienced by snakebite victims as explained by variation between individual bites, individual victims, snake species, and snake phylogeny. Note that individual bites are only a minor source of variation after accounting for the snake and the bitten individual, and there is a stark difference between variance components of the level of pain within the first 5 min (which mostly varies based on the bitten individual) and the maximum level of pain experienced over the course of the envenomation (which is mostly related to the phylogenetic history of the snake species involved).
Toxins 12 00201 g005
Figure 6. Mean pain trajectories for species of Naja and relatives. Note typical pain trajectory with relatively low early pain compared to the maximum later pain levels. Species with n = 1 were excluded.
Figure 6. Mean pain trajectories for species of Naja and relatives. Note typical pain trajectory with relatively low early pain compared to the maximum later pain levels. Species with n = 1 were excluded.
Toxins 12 00201 g006
Table 1. Distribution of bites received by sex to age of victim at the time of bite.
Table 1. Distribution of bites received by sex to age of victim at the time of bite.
Age (years)MaleFemaleUnreportedTotal
Total, n (%)523 (89.6%)51 (8.7%)10 (1.7%)584 (100%)
11–20, n (%)129 (22.1%)11 (1.9%)1 (0.2%)141 (24.3%)
21–30, n (%)164 (28.1%)27 (4.6%)6 (1.0%)197 (33.7%)
31–40, n (%)102 (17.5%)7 (1.2%)1 (0.2%)110 (18.8%)
41–50, n (%)69 (11.8%)3 (0.5%)2 (0.3%)74 (12.7%)
51–60, n (%)41 (7.0%)0041 (7.0%)
≥61, n (%)14 (2.4%)3 (0.5%)017 (2.9%)
Unreported, n (%)4 (0.7%)004 (0.7%)
Table 2. Estimated transition rates between state combinations of pain trajectories according to the fitted model behind our ancestral state estimates. Rates are given as probabilities of transitioning from the state in the row to the state in the column of the table per million years of evolution. States which include early pain-induction are highlighted in bold. The diagonal is marked with - to signify that there is no transition rate from one state to itself since no change happens in that case.
Table 2. Estimated transition rates between state combinations of pain trajectories according to the fitted model behind our ancestral state estimates. Rates are given as probabilities of transitioning from the state in the row to the state in the column of the table per million years of evolution. States which include early pain-induction are highlighted in bold. The diagonal is marked with - to signify that there is no transition rate from one state to itself since no change happens in that case.
From\ToNoneNone+EarlyEarlyEarly+LateNone+Early+LateNone+LateLate
none-0.00400.0110.00300.041
none+early0-0.032000.2050
early00.125-0000
early+late000-00.0700.244
none+early+late0.05700.0160-0.0430.008
none+late0.01800.04200-0.111
late0.14100.0320.0220.0690-

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ward-Smith, H.; Arbuckle, K.; Naude, A.; Wüster, W. Fangs for the Memories? A Survey of Pain in Snakebite Patients Does Not Support a Strong Role for Defense in the Evolution of Snake Venom Composition. Toxins 2020, 12, 201. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/toxins12030201

AMA Style

Ward-Smith H, Arbuckle K, Naude A, Wüster W. Fangs for the Memories? A Survey of Pain in Snakebite Patients Does Not Support a Strong Role for Defense in the Evolution of Snake Venom Composition. Toxins. 2020; 12(3):201. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/toxins12030201

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ward-Smith, Harry, Kevin Arbuckle, Arno Naude, and Wolfgang Wüster. 2020. "Fangs for the Memories? A Survey of Pain in Snakebite Patients Does Not Support a Strong Role for Defense in the Evolution of Snake Venom Composition" Toxins 12, no. 3: 201. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/toxins12030201

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop