The Eastern Cultural Signature of Traditional Chinese Medicine: Empirical Evidence and Theoretical Perspectives

Abstract

Background: Holistic thinking, which is rooted in Eastern culture, is assumed to be the core of traditional Chinese medicine (TCM). Recently, such holistic thinking has been proposed to be applicable to Western medicine practices for alleviating serious side effects; however, the obscure and often ill-defined terms of TCM, such as qi, yin yang, and wuxing, pose considerable obstacles for further understanding TCM. In the present study, we explored whether and how TCM is actually related to the scientific construct of holistic thinking, to elucidate the particular cultural signature of TCM. Methods: A random sample of 101 college students majoring in TCM and 93 non-medical college students was recruited for the study. Two psychological scales—the Chinese Holistic Thinking Scale and the TCM Competence Scale were used respectively to measure the holistic thinking and participants’ ability to apply the TCM in practice. Results: We found that individuals who thought more holistically were better at applying TCM to modern medical problems. Interestingly, TCM was associated with holistic thinking in both TCM and non-medical students, suggesting that this association is intrinsic. Further exploration revealed that the association and variability facets of Eastern holistic thinking—which emphasize that the world is interconnected and ever-changing, respectively—significantly accounted for the individual differences in competence in utilizing TCM in practice. Conclusion: In short, our study provides the first empirical evidence linking TCM to the Eastern holistic thinking style, which not only deepens the understanding of TCM from a scientific perspective but also promotes dialogue between TCM and Western medicine for building safer and more effective health care systems.

Share and Cite:

H. Lu, Y. Wang, Y. Song and J. Liu, "The Eastern Cultural Signature of Traditional Chinese Medicine: Empirical Evidence and Theoretical Perspectives," Chinese Medicine, Vol. 4 No. 3, 2013, pp. 79-86. doi: 10.4236/cm.2013.43012.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

[1] K. Chen and H. Xu, “The integration of Traditional Chinese Medicine and Western Medicine,” European Review, Vol. 11, No. 2, 2003, pp. 225-235.
[2] I. Holliday, “Traditional Medicines in Modern Societies: An Exploration of Integrationist Options through East Asian Experience,” Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, Vol. 28, No. 3, 2003, pp. 373-389. doi:10.1076/jmep.28.3.373.14587
[3] J. Lake, “The Integration of Chinese Medicine and Western Medicine: Focus on Mental Illness,” Integrative Medicine, Vol. 3, No. 4, 2004, pp. CB-CJ.
[4] G. J. Andrews, J. Evans and S. McAlister, “Creating the Right Therapy Vibe: Relational Performances in Holistic Medicine,” Social Science & Medicine, Vol. 83, April 2013, pp. 99-109. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.01.008
[5] T. J. Kaptchuk, “The Web That Has No Weaver: Understanding Chinese Medicine,” McGraw-Hill, New York, 2000.
[6] E. Yu, “Essential Traditional Chinese Medicine: Western Scientific Medicine Perspective,” Hong Kong Practitioner, Vol. 23, No. 1, 2001, pp. 20-27.
[7] D. E. Kendall, “Dao of Chinese Medicine: Understanding an Ancient Healing Art,” Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2002.
[8] M. Koo and I. Choi, “Becoming a Holistic Thinker: Training Effect of Oriental Medicine on Reasoning,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 31, No. 9, 2005, pp. 1264-1272. doi:10.1177/0146167205274692
[9] T. P. Lam, “Strengths and Weaknesses of Traditional Chinese Medicine and Western Medicine in the Eyes of Some Hong Kong Chinese,” Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, Vol. 55, No. 10, 2001, pp. 762-765. doi:10.1136/jech.55.10.762
[10] D. Normile, “The New Face of Traditional Chinese Medicine,” Science, Vol. 299, No. 5604, 2003, pp. 188-190. doi:10.1126/science.299.5604.188
[11] J. Qiu, “Traditional Medicine: A Culture in the Balance,” Nature, Vol. 448, No. 7150, 2007, pp. 126-128. doi:10.1038/448126a
[12] D. J. Munro, “Individualism and Holism: Studies in Confucian and Taoist Values,” Center for Chinese Studies, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, 1985.
[13] H. Nakamura, “Ways of Thinking of Eastern Peoples,” University of Hawaii Press, Honolulu, 1964.
[14] J. Needham, “Science and Civilisation in China. Vol. 4: Physics and Physical Technology,” Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1962.
[15] R. E. Nisbett, K. Peng, I. Choi and A. Norenzayan, “Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic Versus Analytic Cognition,” Psychological Review, Vol. 108, No. 2, 2001, p. 291. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291
[16] D. Oyserman and S. W. Lee, “Does Culture Influence What and How We Think? Effects of Priming Individualism and Collectivism,” Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 134, No. 2, 2008, pp. 311-342. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311
[17] R. E. Nisbett and T. Masuda, “Culture and Point of View,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 100, No. 19, 2003, pp. 11163-11170. doi:10.1073/pnas.1934527100
[18] R. E. Nisbett and Y. Miyamoto, “The Influence of Culture: Holistic versus Analytic Perception,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Vol. 9, No. 10, 2005, pp. 467-473. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.004
[19] R. E. Nisbett, “The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Differently and Why,” Free Press, New York, 2003.
[20] H. F. Chua, J. E. Boland and R. E. Nisbett, “Cultural Variation in Eye Movements during Scene Perception,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 102, No. 35, 2005, pp. 12629-12633. doi:10.1073/pnas.0506162102
[21] L.-J. Ji, K. Peng and R. E. Nisbett, “Culture, Control, and Perception of Relationships in the Environment,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 78, No. 5, 2000, pp. 943-955.
[22] A. Boduroglu, P. Shah and R. E. Nisbett, “Cultural Differences in Allocation of Attention in Visual Information Processing,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Vol. 40, No. 3, 2009, pp. 349-360. doi:10.1177/0022022108331005
[23] S. Duffy and S. Kitayama, “Mnemonic Context Effect in Two Cultures: Attention to Memory Representations?” Cognitive Science, Vol. 31, No. 6, 2007, pp. 1009-1020. doi:10.1080/03640210701703808
[24] M. Doherty, H. Tsuji and W. A. Phillips, “The ContextSensitivity of Visual Size Perception Varies across Cultures,” Perception, Vol. 37, No. 9, 2008, pp. 1426-1433. doi:10.1068/p5946
[25] T. Masuda, R. Gonzalez, L. Kwan and R. E. Nisbett, “Culture and Aesthetic Preference: Comparing the Attention to Context of East Asians and Americans,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 34, No. 9, 2008, pp. 1260-1275. doi:10.1177/0146167208320555
[26] K. Rayner, X. Li, C. C. Williams, K. R. Cave and A. D. Well, “Eye Movements during Information Processing Tasks: Individual Differences and Cultural Effects,” Vision Research, Vol. 47, No. 21, 2007, pp. 2714-2726. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2007.05.007
[27] X. Li, J. Zhang, Y. Huang, M. Xu and J. Liu, “Nurtured to Follow the Crowd: A Twin Study on Conformity,” Chinese Science Bulletin, Vol. 58, No. 10, 2013, pp. 1175-1180. doi:10.1007/s11434-013-5701-x
[28] I. Choi and R. E. Nisbett, “Situational Salience and Cultural Differences in the Correspondence Bias and ActorObserver Bias,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 24, No. 9, 1998, pp. 949-960. doi:10.1177/0146167298249003
[29] I. Choi, R. E. Nisbett and A. Norenzayan, “Causal Attribution across Cultures: Variation and Universality,” Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 125, No. 1, 1999, pp. 47-63. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.47
[30] Lee F, Hallahan M, Herzog T: Explaining real-life events: How culture and domain shape attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 22, No. 7, 1996, pp. 732-741. doi:10.1177/0146167296227007
[31] M. W. Morris and K. Peng, “Culture and Cause: American and Chinese Attributions for Social and Physical Events,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 67, No. 6, 1994, pp. 949-949. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949
[32] T. Masuda and S. Kitayama, “Perceiver-Induced Constraint and Attitude Attribution in Japan and the US: A Case for the Cultural Dependence of the Correspondence Bias,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 40, No. 3, 2004, pp. 409-416. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.08.004
[33] Y. Miyamoto and S. Kitayama, “Cultural Variation in Correspondence Bias: The Critical Role of Attitude Diagnosticity of Socially Constrained Behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 83, No. 5, 2002, pp. 1239-1248. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1239
[34] A. Norenzayan, I. Choi and R. E. Nisbett, “Cultural Similarities and Differences in Social Inference: Evidence from Behavioral Predictions and Lay Theories of Behavior,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 28, No. 1, 2002, pp. 109-120. doi:10.1177/0146167202281010
[35] M. D. Lieberman, J. M. Jarcho and J. Obayashi, “Attributional Inference across Cultures: Similar Automatic Attributions and Different Controlled Corrections,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 31, No. 7, 2005, pp. 889-901. doi:10.1177/0146167204274094
[36] K. Peng, “Naive Dialecticism and Its Effects on Reasoning and Judgment about Contradiction,” University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, 1997.
[37] K. Peng and R. E. Nisbett, “Culture, Dialectics, and Reasoning about Contradiction,” American Psychologist, Vol. 54, No. 9, 1999, pp. 741-754. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.9.741
[38] I. Choi and R. E. Nisbett, “Cultural Psychology of Surprise: Holistic Theories and Recognition of Contradiction,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 79, No. 6, 2000, pp. 890-905. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.890
[39] L.-J. Ji, R. E. Nisbett and Y. Su, “Culture, Change, and Prediction,” Psychological Science, Vol. 12, No. 6, 2001, pp. 450-456. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00384
[40] L.-J. Ji, Z. Zhang and T. Guo, “To Buy or to Sell: Cultural Differences in Stock Market Decisions Based on Price Trends,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol. 21, No. 4, 2008, pp. 399-413. doi:10.1002/bdm.595
[41] J. Spencer-Rodgers, M. J. Williams and K. Peng, “Cultural Differences in Expectations of Change and Tolerance for Contradiction: A Decade of Empirical Research,” Personality and Social Psychology Review, Vol. 14, No. 3, 2010, pp. 296-312. doi:10.1177/1088868310362982
[42] Y. Hou, “Research Progress in Thinking Styles from the Perspective of Cultural Psychology,” Advances in Psychological Science, Vol. 15, No. 2, 2007, pp. 211-216.
[43] Y. Hou, M. Zhang and X. Wang, “The Relationship between Adolescents’ Thinking Style and their Coping Style,” Chinese Mental Health Journal, Vol. 21, No. 3, 2007, pp. 158-161.
[44] Y. B. Hou and Y. Zhu, “The Chinese Holistic Thinking Styles: Their Structure and Effect,” APA Annual Symposium, Chicago, 22-25 August 2002, pp. 22-25.
[45] Y. Hou, Y. Zhu and K. Peng, “How the Thinking Style Affect Managers’ Attribution,” In: D. Wang and Y. Hou, Eds., Personality and Social Psychology, Peking University Press, Beijing, 2004, pp. 115-130.
[46] L.-J. Ji, Z. Zhang and R. E. Nisbett, “Is It Culture or Is It Language? Examination of Language Effects in CrossCultural Research on Categorization,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 87, No. 1, 2004, pp. 5765.
[47] A. Norenzayan, E. E. Smith, B. J. Kim and R. E. Nisbett, “Cultural Preferences for Formal versus Intuitive Reasoning,” Cognitive Science, Vol. 26, No. 5, 2002, pp. 653-684. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2605_4
[48] E. E. Buchtel and A. Norenzayan, “Which Should You Use, Intuition or Logic? Cultural Differences in Injunctive Norms about Reasoning,” Asian Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 11, No. 4, 2008, pp. 264-273. doi:10.1111/j.1467-839X.2008.00266.x
[49] L.-J. Ji, T. Guo, Z. Zhang and D. Messervey, “Looking into the Past: Cultural Differences in Perception and Representation of Past Information,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 96, No. 4, 2009, pp. 761-769. doi:10.1037/a0014498
[50] W. W. Maddux and M. Yuki, “The ‘Ripple Effect’: Cultural Differences in Perceptions of the Consequences of Events,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 32, No. 5, 2006, pp. 669-683. doi:10.1177/0146167205283840
[51] Y. H. Zhang and K. Rose, “A Brief History of Qi,” Paradigm Publications, Brookline, 2001.
[52] L. Lao, L. Xu and S. Xu, “Traditional Chinese Medicine,” In: A. Langler, P. J. Mansky and G. Seifert, Eds., Integrative Pediatric Oncology, Springer, Berlin, 2012, pp. 125-135. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-04201-0_9
[53] C.-Y. Kim and B. Lim, “Modernized Education of Traditional Medicine in Korea: Is It Contributing to the Same Type of Professionalization Seen in Western Medicine?” Social Science & Medicine, Vol. 58, No. 10, 2004, pp. 1999-2008. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00405-2

Copyright © 2024 by authors and Scientific Research Publishing Inc.

Creative Commons License

This work and the related PDF file are licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.