Next Article in Journal
Eco-Evolutionary Dynamics in Microbial Communities from Spontaneous Fermented Foods
Next Article in Special Issue
Status of Constipation and Its Association with Sarcopenia in Older Adults: A Population-Based Cohort Study
Previous Article in Journal
Feasibility of Using Bioelectrical Impedance Analysis for Assessing Youth Weight and Health Status: Preliminary Findings
Previous Article in Special Issue
Cognitive Function and Whole-Brain MRI Metrics Are Not Associated with Mobility in Older Adults with Multiple Sclerosis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Dual-Task-Based Drum Playing with Rhythmic Cueing on Motor and Attention Control in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease: A Preliminary Randomized Study

1
Music Therapy Education, Graduate School of Education, Ewha Womans University, Seoul 03760, Korea
2
Laboratory of Brain & Cognitive Sciences for Convergence Medicine, Hallym University College of Medicine, Anyang 14068, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(19), 10095; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph181910095
Submission received: 20 July 2021 / Revised: 20 September 2021 / Accepted: 23 September 2021 / Published: 26 September 2021

Abstract

:
Although there have been increasing reports regarding the effectiveness of dual-task interventions in rehabilitation, the scope of this research is limited to gross motor movement, such as gait among patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD). To expand the dual-task paradigm to upper extremity motor and attention control in PD, drum playing with modulation of musical elements was attempted. The objective of this study was to evaluate the effects of a drum playing intervention with rhythmic cueing on upper extremity motor control and attention control in patients with PD. Twelve participants were randomly assigned to the drum playing intervention with rhythmic cueing group or the control group. The results showed that the drum playing with rhythmic cueing (DPRC) group significantly increased their sustained time of entrainment (45 BPM) and their latency time until entrainment from pretest to posttest. For the DPRC group, the latency time until entrainment was significantly improved, and improvements in cognitive measures were also found. This study shows that DPRC has great potential to improve upper extremity motor control and attention control and supports the development of new interventions that include this technique for rehabilitation in patients with PD.

1. Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a progressive neurodegenerative disorder that results in motor symptoms involving difficulty with movement initiation and disruptions in movement control [1] as well as nonmotor symptoms [2] involving impairments in memory, attention, and executive function [3]. Most of the motor symptoms of PD, such as tremor, rigidity, bradykinesia, and movement freezing [4], are caused by a dopaminergic deficiency in the basal ganglia, including the loss of dopamine cells in the basal nucleus [5]. Even a 50% loss in dopamine cells has been associated with involuntary motor disorders due to decreased functioning of the central nervous system [6]. In turn, decreased motor control affects the trajectory, timing, and consistency of movement and leads to difficulties with stretch and grab movements in daily living [7,8,9].
A deficiency of dopamine in the basal ganglia is also associated with neural network disruption resulting in cognitive impairment [5]. Almost 40% of PD patients experience cognitive decline [10], and decline in cognitive performance is associated with decreased social interaction, decreased participation in activities of daily living (ADL) [11,12,13,14,15], and greater risk of mortality [16]. As expected, limited motor and cognitive functioning makes it more difficult to engage in ADL which contributes to decreased quality of life among patients with PD [13].
In previous studies, performing a motor-cognitive dual task requires increased cognitive loads resulting in increases of gait variabilities [17], reduces automaticity, and reduces cognitive resources [18,19]. Attention control is critical in dual-task performance to produce coordinated functional movements [20,21,22]. Brain imaging studies show that dual-task performance requires connectivity with several brain regions, including both motor and cognitive areas [19,23], which leads to greater efficiency in performing dual tasks. Due to the neural connectivity associated with dual tasks, a wide range of dual-task training protocols (i.e., walking while talking, walking while performing calculations) has been developed that show positive effects on complex motor functioning with cognitive loads [24].
Recently, a rhythm-motor task has been proposed as a new paradigm to enhance dual-task performance because rhythmic cueing can play a role in mediating cognitive loads in a motor-cognitive dual task, [25], which is especially important for older adults with cognitive aging, decrements in gait integrity [26], and increased tendency for falls [27]. Research in this area has focused on the mechanism of auditory-motor interaction in dual-task performance involving upper arm movement [25,26,27,28,29,30,31]. In particular, playing rhythmic patterns using two hands requires activation of motor and cognitive networks to induce successful task performance [32]. Because the strength and timing of arm and hand movements vary during drum playing [33], drum playing of rhythmic patterns can improve bimanual coordination and accuracy of movement [34].
Rhythmic cueing has long been employed in PD rehabilitation to compensate for reduced movement automaticity due to dopaminergic denervation in the basal ganglia [19]. Movement automaticity in dual-task execution is particularly aggravates in relation to frontal executive dysfunction among patients with PD [35]. It shows that entrainment of brain oscillations to rhythmically cued movement results in behavioral advantages for patients with PD [36]. By coupling regular rhythmic cues to motor tasks, dual task could be successfully performed by allocating attention at anticipated time points [37] and directing attention to the concurrent task [38]. Recently, an fMRI study found greater internetwork connectivity between the auditory network and the executive control network of the brain modulating activity in cortico-cerebellar networks [39]. This was interpreted as the compensatory role of auditory cues in enhancing dual-task performance of PD patients by reducing dual-task interference [39]. These results highlight the potential effectiveness of using rhythmic cueing in dual-task training for improved motor and cognitive functioning among PD patients.
In addition to rhythm perception through rhythmic cueing, rhythm production in a form of drum playing can be primary task in the dual-task paradigm because it requires the recruitment of attentional resources for movement execution. Successful motor control of the upper limbs is based on integration of cognitive, sensory, and motor information with timely response [40,41]. Considering PD-induced impaired automaticity, which is observed even in well-practiced movements [42,43], rhythmic cueing with drum playing can serve as repetitive upper arm movement by using an attentional control strategy.
Given the implications of previous studies, we hypothesized that PD patients participating in dual-task-based drum playing with rhythmic cueing (DPRC) would demonstrate greater improvements in motor control, Nine Hole Peg Test (NHPT) scores, and attention control compared to a control group. This preliminary study examined the effects of DPRC on the motor and cognitive functioning of patients with PD. In addition to the rehabilitative effects, it was expected that patients with PD would enjoy the social aspects of the group drum playing experience.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Twenty patients with PD, ages 40–60 years, were recruited from local community centers and online PD support groups. Eligibility criteria included a Stage II or III diagnosis on the Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) scale [44] and a score over 22 on the Korean version of the mini-mental state examination (K-MMSE) [45]. Additionally, participants had to be able to independently move without the use of a gait orthosis and be free of any hearing loss diagnosis. Of the 20 initially identified participants, four did not meet the inclusion criteria, and four others were excluded from analyses because they dropped out of the intervention for health reasons.

2.2. Procedure

Study participants were provided with a detailed description of the procedures to be followed in the study and written informed consent prior to their participation. The informed consent form included a description of the program content, the duration of the study, the procedures to be used, the expected benefits and risks of participation in the study, and confidentiality. Additionally, we provided participants with information regarding the study aims and methods and their right to withdraw anytime without reprisal. The researcher who evaluated the results was not involved in the intervention and had no knowledge of which group the participants had been assigned to in order to prevent any possible judgment or manipulation of the results during the evaluations. The study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki principles and reports the required information accordingly.
Participants who volunteered to participate were randomly assigned to either the experimental group or the control group. The random assignment procedure was performed with numbers generated by a program of Microsoft Office Excel (Microsoft, Redmond, WA, USA). Of the 20 initially identified participants, four did not meet the inclusion criteria, and four others were excluded from analyses because they dropped out of the study due to health reasons. As a result, 16 people were included in the two groups, then, after 4 dropouts, 12 people, six in each group, were analyzed (see Figure 1). Three 50-min group sessions were held each week for 12 weeks, and assessments were conducted before and after the intervention. During the intervention, the control group attended regular programs, such as gait rehabilitation and speech therapy at the community center, and participants of the control group did not attend any music- or cognitive-based programs.

2.3. Measurements

In this study, the musical instrument digital interface (MIDI) drum(ALESIS, Cumberland, RI, USA) tapping task [46] was used to evaluate motor control function during bimanual tapping. The tapping consisted of two task conditions: drum tapping with and without cueing (see Table 1). In the first stage, participants were asked to tap a drum to a self-paced tempo for 15 s. with both hands simultaneously as regularly as possible. The time from when the direction to start was provided until each participant started tapping was the latency time until self-paced tapping became regular. Then, they were provided with rhythmic cueing at regular intervals for 45 and 105 BPM for 15 s and were instructed to tap their drum in as equal intervals as possible to the regular rhythmic cueing. The time from the presence of cueing until each participant tapped their drum was the latency time until entrainment for 45 and 105 BPM. Next, participants were provided with changes in tempo. In this condition, the BPM of regular rhythmic cueing was changed from 45 to 105 BPM and from 105 to 45 BPM. Additionally, participants were instructed to shift their drum tapping as soon as possible after a tempo change. The time from 45 to 105 BPM or 105 to 45 BPM until entrainment was the latency time for changes in tempo. Finally, the participants were instructed to continue tapping without rhythmic cueing and with rhythmic cueing removed at 15 s after the starting point to measure sustained time for movement entrainment. Sustained time of entrainment was measured for 45 and 105 BPM (see Figure 2). In addition, the NHPT [47] was used to measure fine motor function, as this score is affected by muscle strength, tactile sensitivity of the thumb, and presence of intention tremor. The NHPT [47] requires participants to repeatedly place and then remove nine pegs into nine holes, one at a time, as quickly as possible. The Korean trail making test for the elderly (K-TMT-e) measures visual attention, processing speed, mental flexibility, and executive function [48]. Part A of the K-TMT-e [48] asks participants to connect 15 numbered circles in sequential order, and Part B asks them to connect 15 circles alternating between sequential numbers and words (e.g., 1-Monday-2-Thusday). In this study, completion times on the K-TMT-e [48] were used to assess attention control. The Korean Stroop Test (KST) [49] assesses the ability to inhibit cognitive interference. This interference occurs when the processing of a stimulus feature affects the simultaneous processing of another attribute of the same stimulus [50]. In the KST [49], participants are required to read two different tables. First, the WR condition requires participants to read the names of colors printed in black ink. Second, the CR condition requires participants to name the color of words printed in different colored ink (for instance, the word “red” is printed in green ink).

2.4. Intervention of Drum Playing with Rhythmic Cueing

The intervention consisted of the bimanual exercise of drum playing to strengthen motor control. The musical structure of the DPRC intervention gave a temporal cue for movement timing during the participants’ musical performance, and rhythmic cueing was provided as a stimulus to activate cognitive processing during musical performance. The DPRC intervention consisted of five stages (see Table 2).
The first stage involved playing with regular rhythmic cueing in which drum playing occurred following a metronome beat of music with regular tempo and repeated musical structure. The second stage involved playing the drum according to a changing tempo. At this stage, the rhythmic cueing alternated between 45 and 105 BPM with predictable structure. Common to both stages was the demand for exact motor function and sustained attention while playing the DPRC. Participants had to sustain their auditory attention to track the rhythmic cueing, and this sustained attention caused entrainment between auditory functioning and upper extremity motor functioning. Next, the third stage involved sustaining drum playing according to self-selected rhythmic cueing: simultaneous presentation of regular rhythmic cueing by metronome or unpredictable melodic cueing by electronic piano. Unpredictable melodic cueing consisted of dynamic syncopation and irregular rhythm patterns. In particular, during Stage 3, participants needed to exhibit inhibitory control of selective attention. During drum playing to regular rhythmic cueing, participants needed to sustain constant movement patterns and inhibit dynamic unpredictable melodic cueing. In Stage 4, participants switched between stop and initiation according to two different regular rhythmic cueing patterns played by touch bells: one pattern included stable beat intervals while the other one had a syncopated rhythm pattern. Stage 5 involved switching between drum playing and singing according to the same rhythmic cueing patterns used in Stage 4. Participants initiated drum playing when a stable rhythmic pattern was introduced by touch bells, and participants switched tasks from drum playing to singing when the rhythmic pattern changed. The processes required during Stages 4 and 5 were the most demanding of the intervention, equally requiring high levels of attention and motor control. In other words, the task switching activity following rhythmic cueing was a dual task, requiring divided attention to simultaneously perform the cognitive processing and exercise tasks triggered by external auditory stimuli.

2.5. Data Collection and Analysis

Drum tapping data were collected through the following steps. First, the latency time until regularly self-paced tapping measured the time interval from the verbal starting cue until participants began tapping their drum. Second, the latency time until entrainment for 45 and 105 BPM in response to auditory stimulation was measured via synchronization errors in the drum-tapping task. The synchronization errors were calculated as the time interval between stimulation and response during self-paced tapping. For the mean synchronization errors, the timing of the tap following the onset of rhythmic cueing was calculated and averaged for each trial. Third, the sustained time of entrainment for 45 and 105 BPM was calculated for the tapping time without rhythmic cueing (see Figure 2). Finally, the NHPT score measured the execution time of each hand. Parts A and B of the K-TMT-e were scored based on task completion time. The KST was scored based on the completion time of each task. For each task, average and variability (standard deviation) were calculated to measure significance of completion times and task scores. Data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS 22.0) software, and the data were analyzed descriptively to determine group means and standard deviations for the measures. The normality of data was verified using a Shapiro–Wilk test. The results of the normality test demonstrate that the data came from a normal distribution of motor control and attention control parameters (ranging from 0.056 to 0.670). In addition, the mixed model ANOVA was implemented to compare the measured data at pre-test and post-test between the groups and measured within a factor of time and between groups and interaction effect. Additionally, to examine if the effect of drum playing with rhythmic cueing varied depending on the intervention between groups of parameters, the effect size estimate was analyzed using a Cohen’s d measure. An effect size of 0.2–0.4 was considered a low effect size, with 0.5–0.7 was a moderate effect size, and more than 0.8 was considered a large effect size.

3. Results

In this study, the mean age of the participants was 62.16 years and the mean duration of disease was 5.25 years. In addition, there were no significant differences between the two groups in terms of age, duration of disease, education level, the H&Y scale, and the K-MMSE (see Table 3).
Changes in the mean and standard deviation of the data from pre-test to post-test for each group and effect size of parameters between groups are displayed in Table 4. The motor control measures, including the MIDI-drum tapping task and NHPT, and the cognitive measures are presented.
In addition, a mixed model ANOVA was conducted to compare the DPRC group and control group in terms of changes in each measure across time. The results are displayed in Table 5. There were significant time effects in motor control measures in the condition of the latency time until entrainment at 105 BPM, sustained time of entrainment at 45 BPM, and execution time of the left hand for which large effect sizes were measured in Table 4. These results indicate that changes in measures were significant across time. According to the results for latency time until entrainment at 105 BPM, both groups showed increases at post-test. And for the sustained time of entrainment at 45 BPM, only the DPRC group showed increases at post-test. Additionally, there were significant time effects in execution time on the NHPT for the left hand. Both groups showed decreases in execution time at post-test (see Figure 3).
Additionally, significant group effects in changes at posttest compared to the pretest were observed in motor control measures in the condition of the latency time until the entrainment in 105 BPM for which large effect size was measured in Table 4. While the DPRC group exhibited an increase in latency time following entrainment at 105 BPM, the control group showed a greater increase in this latency time. In particular, a significant interaction effect for time and group was observed for latency time until entrainment at 105 BPM (see Table 5).

4. Discussion

We investigated the effects of DPRC on the motor control and cognitive functioning of patients with PD. Results showed that the latency time before self-paced tapping and the latency time before tapping entrainment in response to rhythmic cueing did not differ between groups. Meanwhile, changes in tempo matched to movement resulted in changes in dual-task performance. Significant differences were found between the groups for the condition of sustained movement at a slow tempo. Along with these results, we found that bimanual DPRC yielded changes in attention control as manifested by improvement in inhibitory control during cognitive processing.
It is remarkable that DPRC improved performance on the cognitively demanding dual tasks. Each step of drum playing involves regular cueing with temporal alternations, which helps patients with PD practice focusing more attention on bimanual motor execution following auditory external cues. As several previous studies suggest, temporal preparation effects driven by rhythmic cueing could be presented during drum playing with cueing [51,52]. Besides the rhythmic aspect, various musical elements, such as pitch and timbre, in intervention contexts provide complex perceptual information as auditory input. With this auditory-motor interaction during drum playing, higher level cognitive processing may be stimulated. This is shown in the MIDI drum tapping task results indicating that significant differences exist between sustained time for movement entrainment in rhythmic cueing at slow tempo. Moving at slower tempo requires tempo adjustment to external auditory cues with a higher demand on cognitive functions [53,54,55]. This implies that bimanual DPRC facilitates intense auditory motor interaction requiring high-level cognitive processing during bimanual tasks for patients with PD.
In particular, we found a significant effect for slow tempo (BPM = 45) on sustained time for movement entrainment in the DPRC group. This task of sustaining regular intervals and continuing bimanual motor performance requires more control in the timing and speed of bimanual performance compared to simpler motor tasks without auditory cues, such as sequential finger tapping movements [56]. Thus, slow and repetitive movement tasks can be considered key to dual-task training for PD patients by enhancing time-keeping abilities during auditory-motor interaction [32,57,58,59,60]. Additionally, this supports that temporal characteristics in rhythmic cueing serve as movement timing cues to activate the motor system with attention control [61]. We also found significant improvement in left hand scores of the NHPT. Compared to the average scores of hand function tests reported in previous studies of PD patients [62], this result suggests that DPRC was effective in producing improvements in non-dominant hand function. Overall, the results indicate that regular intervals with external auditory cues constitute an internal timekeeping system and that this mechanism can improve perceptual accuracy of timing and thus be effectively applied to interventions targeting sensorimotor functioning and motor coordination.
Given the impairment in frontal cortical activity due to basal ganglia dysfunction in PD [63], executive function during repetitive movement plays an important role in controlling movement performance [64]. Our data point to independence between hand function or simple motor performance abilities and functional changes in cognitive processing. Considering the movement of bimanual drum playing, the level of movement involved in the task is simultaneous and simple up-down upper extremity movement without individual finger movements associated with dexterity. Further studies should be conducted with various musical instruments or more complex rhythmic patterns in order to substantiate both cognitive and motor improvements.
This study is not without limitations. First our sample size was small and our results should be considered with caution. Although randomization was employed in the study design, we could not blind the intervention due to the nature of music intervention studies. Future studies should be designed as long-term studies and involve rigorous data collection on executive function considering the specific neurological impairments of patients with PD.

5. Conclusions

In sum, the current study provides interesting findings revealing the effects of bimanual DPRC on enhancing attention controls for patients with PD. Regular rhythmic cueing has the power to enhance temporal preparation during temporal changes are made during motor tasks, particularly when temporal direction is getting slower. Overall, results unveil instrument playing with rhythmic cueing based on a dual-task paradigm can facilitate simultaneous motor and attention control functions and meaningful changes in dual-task performance outcomes for patients with PD. Variation of tempi in rhythmic cueing and complexity of intervention tasks affect motor control function, attention control, and demand on cognitive processing. Bimanual drum playing as a musical application can be used as an efficient rehabilitative intervention modality for patients with PD. The dual-task paradigm should continue to be applied in rehabilitative intervention contexts.

Author Contributions

J.-K.P. collected data and participated in the data analysis and manuscript writing; S.J.K. served as the PI of the study, designed the study procedure, and supported all phases of producing and analyzing the data and writing the current version of the manuscript. Both authors approved the final version of the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki principles and conforms to all CONSORT guidelines and reports the required information accordingly. And ethical approval was not obtained for this study due to discrepancies between the period of program participant recruitment and the progress schedule of the research institution. Authors received the institutional ethical education that is required for researchers. Accordingly, this study was provided with the detailed procedures to be followed in the study and informed written consent prior to their participation. Additionally, we provided participants with information regarding the study aims and methods, their right to withdraw anytime without reprisal. The researcher who evaluated the results was not involved in the intervention and had no knowledge of which group the participants had been assigned to prevent any possible judgment or manipulation of the results during the evaluations.

Informed Consent Statement

Participants were provided with the detailed procedures to be followed in the study and informed written consent prior to their participation. The informed consent form included a description of the program content, the duration of the study, the procedures to be used, the expected benefits and risks of participation in the study, and confidentiality. Additionally, we provided participants with information regarding the study aims and methods and their right to withdraw anytime without reprisal.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Crowley, E.; Nolan, Y.; Sullivan, A. Exercise as a therapeutic intervention for motor and non-motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease: Evidence from rodent models. Prog. Neurobiol. 2019, 172, 2–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Braak, H.; Del Tredici, K.; Rüb, U.; De Vos, R.A.; Steur, E.N.J.; Braak, E. Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 2003, 24, 197–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Remz, M.; Salinas, M. Psychosis in Parkinson’s Disease. Psychiatr. Ann. 2020, 50, 113–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Rodriguez-Oroz, M.C.; Jahanshahi, M.; Krack, P.; Litvan, I.; Macias, R.; Bezard, E.; Obeso, J.A. Initial clinical manifestations of Parkinson’s disease: Features and pathophysiological mechanisms. Lancet Neurol. 2009, 8, 1128–1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Robbins, T.W.; Cools, R. Cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Mov. Disord. 2014, 29, 597–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Venderova, K.; Park, D.S. Programmed cell death in Parkinson’s disease. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 2012, 2, a009365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Lin, K.-C.; Hsieh, Y.-W.; Wu, C.-Y.; Chen, C.-L.; Jang, Y.; Liu, J.-S. Minimal detectable change and clinically important difference of the Wolf Motor Function Test in stroke patients. Neurorehabilit. Neural Repair 2009, 23, 429–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Moustafa, A.A.; Chakravarthy, S.; Phillips, J.R.; Gupta, A.; Keri, S.; Polner, B.; Frank, M.J.; Jahanshahi, M. Motor symptoms in Parkinson’s disease: A unified framework. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016, 68, 727–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Almeida, Q.J.; Wishart, L.R.; Lee, T.D. Disruptive influences of a cued voluntary shift on coordinated movement in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2003, 41, 442–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Yarnall, A.J.; Breen, D.P.; Duncan, G.W.; Khoo, T.K.; Coleman, S.Y.; Firbank, M.J.; Nombela, C.; Winder-Rhodes, S.; Evans, J.R.; Rowe, J.B. Characterizing mild cognitive impairment in incident Parkinson disease: The ICICLE-PD study. Neurology 2014, 82, 308–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Amboni, M.; Barone, P.; Hausdorff, J.M. Cognitive contributions to gait and falls: Evidence and implications. Mov. Disord. 2013, 28, 1520–1533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Derejko, M.; Slawek, J.; Wieczorek, D.; Brockhuis, B.; Dubaniewicz, M.; Lass, P. Regional cerebral blood flow in Parkinson’s disease as an indicator of cognitive impairment. Nucl. Med. Commun. 2006, 27, 945–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Lawson, R.A.; Yarnall, A.J.; Duncan, G.W.; Breen, D.P.; Khoo, T.K.; Williams-Gray, C.H.; Barker, R.A.; Collerton, D.; Taylor, J.-P.; ICICLE-PD Study Group. Cognitive decline and quality of life in incident Parkinson’s disease: The role of attention. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 2016, 27, 47–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Shipley, B.A.; Der, G.; Taylor, M.D.; Deary, I.J. Cognition and mortality from the major causes of death: The Health and Lifestyle Survey. J. Psychosom. Res. 2008, 65, 143–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Zhou, S.; Chen, X.; Wang, C.; Yin, C.; Hu, P.; Wang, K. Selective attention deficits in early and moderate stage Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci. Lett. 2012, 509, 50–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Willis, A.W.; Schootman, M.; Kung, N.; Evanoff, B.A.; Perlmutter, J.S.; Racette, B.A. Predictors of survival in patients with Parkinson disease. Arch. Neurol. 2012, 69, 601–607. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Yogev, G.; Giladi, N.; Peretz, C.; Springer, S.; Simon, E.S.; Hausdorff, J.M. Dual tasking, gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: Which aspects of gait are attention demanding? Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005, 22, 1248–1256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Jankovic, J. Parkinson’s disease: Clinical features and diagnosis. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2008, 79, 368–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Wu, T.; Hallett, M. A functional MRI study of automatic movements in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain 2005, 128, 2250–2259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Plummer-D’Amato, P.; Altmann, L.J.; Behrman, A.L.; Marsiske, M. Interference between cognition, double-limb support, and swing during gait in community-dwelling individuals poststroke. Neurorehabilit. Neural Repair 2010, 24, 542–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Silsupadol, P.; Siu, K.-C.; Shumway-Cook, A.; Woollacott, M.H. Training of balance under single-and dual-task conditions in older adults with balance impairment. Phys. Ther. 2006, 86, 269–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Yogev-Seligmann, G.; Hausdorff, J.M.; Giladi, N. The role of executive function and attention in gait. Mov. Disord. Off. J. Mov. Disord. Soc. 2008, 23, 329–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Wu, T.; Hallett, M. The cerebellum in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 2013, 136, 696–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Fritz, N.E.; Cheek, F.M.; Nichols-Larsen, D.S. Motor-cognitive dual-task training in neurologic disorders: A systematic review. J. Neurol. Phys. Ther. 2015, 39, 142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Kim, S.J.; Cho, S.-R.; Yoo, G.E. The applicability of rhythm-motor tasks to a new dual task paradigm for older adults. Front. Neurol. 2017, 8, 671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Terrier, P.; Reynard, F. Effect of age on the variability and stability of gait: A cross-sectional treadmill study in healthy individuals between 20 and 69 years of age. Gait Posture 2015, 41, 170–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Herman, T.; Mirelman, A.; Giladi, N.; Schweiger, A.; Hausdorff, J.M. Executive control deficits as a prodrome to falls in healthy older adults: A prospective study linking thinking, walking, and falling. Biomed. Sci. Med. Sci. 2010, 65, 1086–1092. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Chen, J.L.; Zatorre, R.J.; Penhune, V.B. Interactions between auditory and dorsal premotor cortex during synchronization to musical rhythms. Neuroimage 2006, 32, 1771–1781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Haslinger, B.; Erhard, P.; Altenmüller, E.; Schroeder, U.; Boecker, H.; Ceballos-Baumann, A.O. Transmodal sensorimotor networks during action observation in professional pianists. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 282–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. LaGasse, A.B.; Knight, A. Rhythm and music in rehabilitation: A critical review of current research. Crit. Rev. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 2011, 23, 49–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Thaut, M.H.; Stephan, K.M.; Wunderlich, G.; Schicks, W.; Tellmann, L.; Herzog, H.; McIntosh, G.C.; Seitz, R.J.; Hömberg, V. Distinct cortico-cerebellar activations in rhythmic auditory motor synchronization. Cortex 2009, 45, 44–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Molinari, M.; Leggio, M.G.; De Martin, M.; Cerasa, A.; Thaut, M. Neurobiology of rhythmic motor entrainment. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2003, 999, 313–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Alexander, J.S.; Fachner, J.; Magee, W.L. Upper limb rehabilitation in chronic stroke using neurologic music therapy: Two contrasting case studies to inform on treatment delivery and patient suitability. Nord. J. Music Ther. 2019, 28, 382–404. [Google Scholar]
  34. Malcolm, M.P.; Massie, C.; Thaut, M. Rhythmic auditory-motor entrainment improves hemiparetic arm kinematics during reaching movements: A pilot study. Top. Stroke Rehabil. 2009, 16, 69–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Vervoort, G.; Heremans, E.; Bengevoord, A.; Strouwen, C.; Nackaerts, E.; Vandenberghe, W.; Nieuwboer, A. Dual-task-related neural connectivity changes in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience 2016, 317, 36–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Schroeder, C.E.; Lakatos, P. Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection. Trends Neurosci. 2009, 32, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Bella, S.D.; Benoit, C.E.; Farrugia, N.; Schwartze, M.; Kotz, S.A. Effects of musically cued gait training in Parkinson’s disease: Beyond a motor benefit. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2015, 1337, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Galletly, R.; Brauer, S.G. Does the type of concurrent task affect perferred and cued gait in people with Parkinson’s disease? Aust. J. Physiother. 2005, 51, 175–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Braunlich, K.; Seger, C.A.; Jentink, K.G.; Buard, I.; Kluger, B.M.; Thaut, M.H. Rhythmic auditory cues shape neural network recruitment in Parkinson’s disease during repetitive motor behavior. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2019, 49, 849–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Alberts, J.L.; Saling, M.; Adler, C.H.; Stelmach, G.E. Disruptions in the reach-to-grasp actions of Parkinson’s patients. Exp. Brain Res. 2000, 134, 353–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Pelton, T.; van Vliet, P.; Hollands, K. Interventions for improving coordination of reach to grasp following stroke: A systematic review. Int. J. Evid. Based Healthc. 2012, 10, 89–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ma, H.I.; Hwang, W.J.; Lin, K.C. The effects of two different auditory stimuli on functional arm movement in persons with Parkinson’s disease: A dual-task paradigm. Clin. Rehabil. 2009, 23, 229–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Wu, T.; Hallett, M.; Chan, P. Motor automaticity in Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2015, 82, 226–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Hoehn, M.M.; Yahr, M.D. Parkinsonism: Onset, progression, and mortality. Neurology. 1998, 50, 318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Shin, M.H.; Lee, Y.M.; Park, J.M.; Kang, C.J.; Lee, B.D.; Moon, E.; Chung, Y.I. A combination of the Korean version of the mini-mental state examination and Korean dementia screening questionnaire is a good screening tool for dementia in the elderly. Psychiatry Investig. 2011, 8, 348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Kim, S.J.; Cho, S.R.; Yoo, G.E. Age-Related Changes in Bimanual Instrument Playing with Rhythmic Cueing. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Mathiowetz, V.; Weber, K.; Kashman, N.; Volland, G. Adult norms for the nine hole peg test of finger dexterity. Occup. Ther. J. Res. 1985, 5, 24–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Jang, J.W.; Kim, K.; Baek, M.J.; Kim, S.Y. A comparison of five types of trail making test in Korean elderly. Dement. Neurocognitive Disord. 2016, 15, 135–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Seo, E.H.; Lee, D.Y.; Choo, I.H.; Kim, S.G.; Kim, K.W.; Youn, J.C.; Jhoo, J.H.; Woo, J.I. Normative study of the stroop color and word test in an educationally diverse elderly population. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2008, 23, 1020–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Ludwig, C.; Borella, E.; Tettamanti, M.; De Ribaupierre, A. Adult age differences in the Color Stroop Test: A comparison between an Item-by-item and a Blocked version. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2010, 51, 135–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Jones, C.R.; Jahanshahi, M. Motor and Perceptual Timing in Parkinson’s Disease. In Neurobiology of Interval Timing; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 265–290. [Google Scholar]
  52. Rohenkohl, G.; Coull, J.T.; Nobre, A.C. Behavioural dissociation between exogenous and endogenous temporal orienting of attention. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e14620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Bangert, A.S.; Balota, D.A. Keep up the pace: Declines in simple repetitive timing differentiate healthy aging from the earliest stages of Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. JINS 2012, 18, 1052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. McAuley, J.D.; Jones, M.R.; Holub, S.; Johnston, H.M.; Miller, N.S. The time of our lives: Life span development of timing and event tracking. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2006, 135, 348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. McAuley, J.D.; Henry, M.J.; Tkach, J. Tempo mediates the involvement of motor areas in beat perception. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2012, 1252, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Loehrer, P.A.; Nettersheim, F.S.; Jung, F.; Weber, I.; Huber, C.; Dembek, T.A.; Pelzer, E.A.; Fink, G.R.; Tittgemeyer, M.; Timmermann, L. Ageing changes effective connectivity of motor networks during bimanual finger coordination. NeuroImage 2016, 143, 325–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Thaut, M.H.; Abiru, M. Rhythmic auditory stimulation in rehabilitation of movement disorders: A review of current research. Music Percept. 2010, 27, 263–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Trapp, S.; Lepsien, J.; Sehm, B.; Villringer, A.; Ragert, P. Changes of hand switching costs during bimanual sequential learning. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e45857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Serrien, D.J.; Swinnen, S.P.; Stelmach, G.E. Age-related deterioration of coordinated interlimb behavior. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2000, 55, P295–P303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Shetty, A.K.; Vinutha Shankar, M.; Annamalai, N. Bimanual coordination: Influence of age and gender. J. Clin. Diagn. Res. JCDR 2014, 8, 15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Morillon, B.; Hackett, T.A.; Kajikawa, Y.; Schroeder, C.E. Predictive motor control of sensory dynamics in auditory active sensing. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2015, 31, 230–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Alonso, C.C.; de Freitas, P.B.; Pires, R.S.; de Oliveira, D.L.; Freitas, S.M.F. Accuracy, sensitivity and specificity of different tests to detect impaired hand function in Parkinsons disease. medRxiv 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Galvan, A.; Devergnas, A.; Wichmann, T. Alterations in neuronal activity in basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits in the parkinsonian state. Front. Neuroanat. 2015, 9, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  64. Clark, D.J. Automaticity of walking: Functional significance, mechanisms, measurement and rehabilitation strategies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Flowchart for group allocation.
Figure 1. Flowchart for group allocation.
Ijerph 18 10095 g001
Figure 2. The measurement paradigm of the MIDI drum tapping task. The latency time until the entrainment refers to time from cueing until each participant’s tapping interval started synchronizing. The sustained time of entrainment refers to time until each participant’s tapping became regular without rhythmic cueing.
Figure 2. The measurement paradigm of the MIDI drum tapping task. The latency time until the entrainment refers to time from cueing until each participant’s tapping interval started synchronizing. The sustained time of entrainment refers to time until each participant’s tapping became regular without rhythmic cueing.
Ijerph 18 10095 g002
Figure 3. Changes in motor control measures for the DPRC and control groups. Panel (A) shows change in latency time until entrainment for 105 BPM, panel (B) shows change in sustained time of entrainment for 45 BPM, and panel (C) shows change in execution time of left hand for NHPT.
Figure 3. Changes in motor control measures for the DPRC and control groups. Panel (A) shows change in latency time until entrainment for 105 BPM, panel (B) shows change in sustained time of entrainment for 45 BPM, and panel (C) shows change in execution time of left hand for NHPT.
Ijerph 18 10095 g003
Table 1. The MIDI drum tapping task.
Table 1. The MIDI drum tapping task.
Tapping TaskCueingCondition of Cueing
Without rhythmic cueingTapping to own paceNo-
With rhythmic cueingTapping to follow regular rhythmic cueingYesProviding regular rhythmic cueing for 15 s
Shifting tapping to match slow and fast tempoYesChanging tempo of regular rhythmic cueing after 15 s
Sustained tapping to regular intervals with cueing removedY→NRemoving regular rhythmic cueing after 15 s
Table 2. Intervention of drum playing with rhythmic cueing.
Table 2. Intervention of drum playing with rhythmic cueing.
Stage Intervention TaskRegular CueingInterrupted CueingContent
1Drum playing with regular rhythmic cueingYNTracking rhythmic cueing of regular interval of metronome beats
2Drum playing with alternating tempoYNTracking rhythmic cueing according to changes in tempo of 45 and 105 BPM
3Drum playing with regular rhythm and unexpected stimulus interruptionYYSustained regular interval of rhythmic cueing with unpredicted melodic cueing
4Task shifting between initiation and stop with rhythmic cueing of different rhythmic patternsYYDuring tracking of rhythmic cueing for regular tempo, participants shift tasks from initiating to stopping when provided with cueing of different rhythmic patterns
5Task shifting between playing and singing with different rhythmic patternsYYDuring tracking of rhythmic cueing for regular tempo, participants shift tasks between playing and singing when provided with different rhythmic patterns
Table 3. Participants’ demographic information.
Table 3. Participants’ demographic information.
CharacteristicDPRC M ± SDControl M ± SDZp
Age (years)61.6 ± 4.963.1 ± 10.1−0.4340.673
Duration of disease (years)5.6 ± 3.14.8 ± 1.41.0000.363
Education (years)12.3 ± 1.212.3 ± 3.1−0.4160.686
H&Y scale2.3 ± 0.81.6 ± 0.50.4550.660
MMSE-K score28.3 ± 1.226.3 ± 1.91.5700.132
Note. DPRC: drum playing with rhythmic cueing group; Control: control group; H&Y Scale: the Hoehn and Yahr scale; MMSE-K: the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination.
Table 4. Changes in motor control and cognitive measures for each group.
Table 4. Changes in motor control and cognitive measures for each group.
ParameterDPRC Group (n = 6)Control Group (n = 6)
Pre M ± SDPost M ± SDPre M ± SDPost M ± SDEffects Size
Motor control measures
Latency time until regularly self-paced tappingSelf-paced0.84 ± 0.370.55 ± 0.230.71 ± 0.520.96 ± 0.20.94
Latency time until entrainment45 BPM 4.05 ± 3.044.41 ± 2.783.87 ± 2.98.49 ± 4.310.53
105 BPM3.27 ± 0.653.56 ± 1.253.46 ± 3.145.33 ± 3.881.46
Latency time until entrainment for changes in tempo45→105 BPM4.01 ± 4.591.93 ± 0.44.75 ± 4.315.64 ± 4.320.84
105→45 BPM4.91 ± 2.783.59 ± 2.874.49 ± 1.037.03 ± 3.750.39
Sustained time of entrainment45 BPM1.52 ± 2.199.65 ± 5.562.96 ± 3.921.03 ± 1.730.9
105 BPM2.55 ± 1.37.12 ± 5.232.17 ± 1.183.11 ± 2.450.68
Execution time of NHPTRight28.78 ± 6.1427.64 ± 6.3929.42 ± 5.4539.60 ± 8.680.03
Left34.51 ± 11.9930.47 ± 8.8428.00 ± 4.5828.69 ± 1.710.87
Cognitive measures
K-TMT-e_A19.16 ± 5.4119.18 ± 4.9221.12 ± 5.0721.94 ± 5.650.16
K-TMT-e_B42.67 ± 18.0633.68 ± 13.4041.06 ± 15.7933.43 ± 8.560.07
KST_WR_Time65.55 ± 8.4665.88 ± 15.7163.51 ± 10.4665.27 ± 10.580.14
KST_CR_Time121.69 ± 34.13109.73 ± 26.86119.08 ± 21.38129.70 ± 27.251.04
Note. DPRC: drum playing with rhythmic cueing group; Control: control group; BPM: beats per minute; K-TMT-e: Korean trail making test for the elderly; KST: Korean Stroop test; WR: word reading condition; CR: color reading condition.
Table 5. The results of a mixed model repeated measures ANOVA.
Table 5. The results of a mixed model repeated measures ANOVA.
ParameterRepeated Measures Results
Time EffectGroup EffectTime * Group
(t, p)(t, p)(t, p)
Motor control measures
Latency time until regularly self-paced tappingSelf-paced0.011, 0.9170.764, 0.4032.704, 0.131
Latency time until the entrainment45 BPM 2.686, 0.1320.178, 0.6820.873, 0.372
105 BPM6.231, 0.032 **5.407, 0.042 **6.469, 0.029 **
Latency time until the entrainment in changes tempo45→105 BPM1.214, 0.2960.939, 0.3550.477, 0.505
105→45 BPM1.094, 0.3200.990, 0.3432.124, 0.176
Sustained time of entrainment45 BPM8.926, 0.014 **0.017, 0.9002.448, 0.149
105 BPM3.130, 0.1070.031, 0.8641.414, 0.262
Execution time of NHPTRight0.284, 0.6060.035, 0.8560.004, 0.954
Left10.994, 0.008 ***0.110, 0.7472.327, 0.158
Cognitive measures
K-TMT-e_A0.083, 0.7790.616, 0.4510.078, 0.786
K-TMT-e_B2.302, 0.1600.016, 0.9010.015, 0.903
KST_WR_Time0.132, 0.7240.038, 0.8490.062, 0.808
KST_CR_Time0.011, 0.9170.279, 0.6093.295, 0.100
Note. * shows the interaction effect. ** p < 0.05. *** p < 0.01. DPRC, drum playing with rhythmic cueing group; control, control group; sec, seconds, BPM, beats per minute. K-TMT-e, Korean trail making test for the elderly (Yi et al., 2007); KST, Korean Stroop test (Kim et al., 2004); WR, word reading condition; CR, color reading condition.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Park, J.-K.; Kim, S.J. Dual-Task-Based Drum Playing with Rhythmic Cueing on Motor and Attention Control in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease: A Preliminary Randomized Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 10095. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph181910095

AMA Style

Park J-K, Kim SJ. Dual-Task-Based Drum Playing with Rhythmic Cueing on Motor and Attention Control in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease: A Preliminary Randomized Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(19):10095. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph181910095

Chicago/Turabian Style

Park, Jin-Kyoung, and Soo Ji Kim. 2021. "Dual-Task-Based Drum Playing with Rhythmic Cueing on Motor and Attention Control in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease: A Preliminary Randomized Study" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 19: 10095. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph181910095

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop