Next Article in Journal
Cross-Validation of Predictive Equation for Cardiorespiratory Fitness by Modified Shuttle Walk Test in Adults with Schizophrenia: A Secondary Analysis of the CORTEX-SP Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Indoor Mobility, Frailty, and Disability in Community-Dwelling Older Adults: A Mediation Model
Previous Article in Journal
Factors Associated with Mental Health among International Students during the COVID-19 Pandemic in South Korea
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Crosstalk between Gross and Fine Motor Domains during Late Childhood: The Influence of Gross Motor Training on Fine Motor Performances in Primary School Children

Department of Experimental and Clinical Medicine, Physiological Sciences Section, University of Florence, 50134 Florence, Italy
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18(21), 11387; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph182111387
Submission received: 15 September 2021 / Revised: 26 October 2021 / Accepted: 27 October 2021 / Published: 29 October 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Physical Well-Being and Motor Development over the Life Span)

Abstract

:
Gross and fine motor competence have a close relationship during development and are shown to correlate to some extent. However, the study of the interaction between these domains still requires further insights. In this study, we investigated the developmental changes in overall motor skills as well as the effects of gross motor training programs on fine motor skills in children (aged 6–11, n = 240). Fine motor skills were assessed before and after gross motor intervention using the Box and Block Test. The gross motor intervention was based on the Test of Gross Motor Development—3rd Edition. Results showed that gross and fine motor skills correlate across all years of primary school, both significantly improving with age. Finally, the gross motor intervention appeared to not influence fine motor skills. Our findings show that during primary school age, overall motor development is continuous, but non-linear. From age nine onward, there seems to be a major step-up in overall motor competence, of which teachers/educators should be aware of in order to design motor educational programs accordingly. While gross and fine motor domains might be functionally integrated to enhance children’s motor performances, further research is needed to clarify the effect of gross motor practice on fine motor performances.

1. Introduction

Motor skills refer to the underlying internal pathways responsible for moving the body through space as well as the cognitive processes that give rise to such movements [1]. These are classically divided into two categories, namely gross motor skills and fine motor skills [2]. Specifically, gross motor skills involve the body’s large muscles and pertain to movement of the trunk and limbs whereas fine motor skills involve the body’s small muscles and pertain to movements of wrists and fingers [3,4,5]. Moreover, gross motor skills are further categorized into locomotor and object control skills [6,7].
General development of motor skills undergoes major improvements during the formative years of childhood (i.e., 5–11 years of age) due to the maturation of the central and peripheral nervous system and locomotor system [8]. Research has shown that during child development, gross and fine motor competencies appear to have some correlation [9,10,11,12,13,14]. In fact, it was suggested that specific gross motor activities could involve fine motor adjustments (e.g., ball dribbling and handling, ball-striking with a bat, throwing at a target, skipping through a hopscotch-type pattern) [10,11]. Moreover, the same higher order neuromotor processes appear to be involved in the learning and mastering of both gross and fine motor skills [12]. Accordingly, gross and fine motor skills have been defined as motor domains that partially share the same cognitive processes [13].
Previous studies have investigated the relationship between gross and fine motor skills during various steps of children’s school education, obtaining contentious results when comparing gross and fine individual performance measures [9,13,14,15,16,17]. For instance, Cameron et al. [9] and Oberer et al. [13] showed moderate correlation between gross and fine motor skills. Specifically, Oberer et al. [13] reported a positive correlation in children aged 5.6–7.25 years, assessing both gross and fine motor skills using speed and precision tasks (e.g., jumping sideways and one leg stand for gross motor skills, posting coins, and drawing trail for fine motor skills). Similarly, Cameron and colleagues’ investigation [9] also reported a positive correlation in younger children (aged 3–4 years), assessing gross motor skills using balance, imitation, and hop and skip tasks, whereas fine motor skills were evaluated using spatial organization tasks (e.g., building a tower with bricks and drawing tasks). Furthermore, Dayem et al. [14] showed that an even higher correlation between gross and fine motor skills occurred in children aged 4–6 years, assessing gross motor skills using stationary, locomotor, and object manipulation tasks, while fine motor skills were assessed using a writing task. Conversely, other authors disagree with the positive correlation between gross and fine motor skills [15,16,17]. Specifically, Tortella and colleagues’ study [15] reported that there was no correlation between gross and fine motor skills in pre-school children aged 5–6 years, evaluating gross motor skills using precision, balance, throwing, and walking tasks, while fine motor skills were assessed using manual speed and precision tasks (e.g., building bricks and posting coins). Moreover, Souza et al. [16] found that when investigating global motor performance with the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development—Third Edition, there was a clear individual variability in overall motor proficiency as well as a weak correlation between gross and fine motor skills. Finally, Amaro et al. [17] reported no correlation between gross and fine motor skills in children aged 5–10 years when comparing the scores obtained in the “Körperkoordinationtest für kinder” and Minnesota manual dexterity test, respectively.
These contrasting results may be attributed to the fact that motor skills do not follow linear developmental trajectories [16,18]. Hence, it is not surprising that investigating children of different ages could produce different results. Furthermore, these studies assessed motor skills during short age spans using heterogeneous tasks. To our knowledge, a broader investigation regarding the gross-fine motor development during the entire late childhood developmental stage (i.e., primary school children) with consistent motor assessment methods has yet to be carried out.
Despite the mentioned elements of relationship during development, the influence of gross motor training on fine motor skill enhancement in school age children has not been adequately assessed in the literature. Indeed, research has mainly focused on interactions between gross-fine motor skills and other competence domains (e.g., social skills [19,20], cognitive skills [21,22], academic achievement [23,24]), indicating a positive influence of both gross and fine motor skills on these elements [20,25,26]. However, a specific approach aiming to explore the influence that components of gross and fine motor domains exert on each other is missing. Given that both gross and fine motor skills hold a mutual influence on these fundamental factors for children’s overall well-being, it is plausible that gross and fine motor skills share some elements, which the enhancement of one (i.e., gross motor skills) could also improve the other (i.e., fine motor skills).
In the current study, we sought to expand the focus pertaining to the motor development of primary school children in two directions: first, to investigate developmental changes in overall motor skills during late childhood, which was achieved by comparing both gross motor training results as well as fine motor performances among the different ages. Second, to examine the effects of short gross motor training programs on fine motor skills in children, which was achieved by comparing the pre- and post-training results for the evaluation of fine motor skills.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 240 typically developing male and female children from age six to 11 participated in this study. All subjects were free of any documented visual, motor, and/or neurological impairments, nor any intellectual disabilities. None of the participants were involved in extracurricular sports practice. The study protocol was approved by the institution ethics committee, Prot. N.0018234E, Rif. 63/12. Prior to the start of the study, written informed consent was obtained from the parents/legal guardians of the children. Participants were all tested individually by the principal investigator and six research assistants, all of whom were familiar with the purpose of the study. The participants’ characteristics are summarized in Table 1.
In this study, fine motor skills were evaluated using the Box and Block Test (BBT) [27], whereas gross motor skill training sessions were conducted using the Test of Gross Motor Development—Third Edition (TGMD-3) [6]. Regarding the gross motor training, the total sample of subjects was divided into three subgroups: locomotor and ball skills subgroup (LBS), which executed all the TGMD-3 skills; locomotor skills subgroup (LS), which only executed the six LS subscale skills of TGMD-3; and the ball skills subgroup (BS), which only executed the seven BS subscale skills of TGMD-3 (Table 2). The purpose of the division was to observe whether the practice of a specific subset of skills could be more impactful on manual dexterity performance.
The reasons for dividing the sample into three groups were many, one being that gross motor skills are generally categorized into locomotor and object control [7]. Furthermore, the training of different types of gross motor skills have been shown to influence other aspects related to motor performance [28]. Moreover, short forms of the TGMD have already been conducted in the recent literature [29,30] for training/assessment of just one of the two subsets of gross motor skills included in the test (i.e., locomotor or object manipulation skills).

2.2. Procedures

The study spanned across five consecutive days, similar to previous studies [11,31] (Table 3). At the beginning of the study (i.e., day 1) each participant underwent a baseline evaluation for the fine motor skills of both the dominant and the non-dominant hand. Following that, participants took part in three gross motor skill training sessions (one session per day, i.e., days 2–4) which lasted from 30 to 45 min [32]. During these sessions, the participants’ gross motor skills were also evaluated. Finally, the same procedure for the evaluation of fine motor skills was also executed post-gross motor skills training (i.e., day 5). Both the fine motor skills evaluations and gross motor skills training sessions took place in indoor school gymnasiums.

2.3. Fine Motor Skills Evaluation

The BBT is a simple, validated, and suitable test that can be administered quickly to assess fine motor skills in children from age three onward [27,33,34]. The materials needed for the BBT are a wooden box (53.7 × 25.4 × 8.5 cm) divided into two compartments by a partition (15.2-cm-high) and 150 wooden cubic blocks (2.5 cm per side) [33,34]. We adopted the same procedure for both the fine motor skills evaluation of this study (i.e., baseline and post-gross motor skills training). Each subject was seated on a height-adjustable chair, with the forearms resting on a desk. Both hands were tested separately, starting with the dominant hand, which was determined by asking the participants to write their name on paper [27]. All 150 cubes were placed in one compartment. The test consisted in transferring as many blocks as possible, one block at a time and with one hand, from one compartment to the other in 60 s. Each test was preceded by a 15 s practice period. The cube placement always allows for lateral to medial movements (i.e., when testing the right hand, all the 150 cubes were placed in the right compartment of the box and had to be moved to the left compartment of the box). The number of blocks transferred in 60 s was the outcome score of the test. The maximum total score possible for a single trial was 150, meaning that in 60 s, all cubes were moved from the lateral compartment to the medial compartment.

2.4. Gross Motor Skills Training

The TGMD-3 is a direct observation assessment that measures the performance of various gross motor skills in children ages 3–10.9 years [35]. The continued popularity of the TGMD has been associated with its increasing use in research in child development, physical activity, and public health [36,37,38,39,40]. Particularly, the latest edition of the TGMD (i.e., the TGMD-3) has been proposed as a valid and reliable assessment tool for measuring gross motor skills competence in both pre-school and primary school children [41,42,43,44].
The skills present in the TGMD-3 include a selection of fundamental gross motor skills that are commonly taught in the primary physical education curriculum on an international scale [45]. Specifically, the TGMD-3 assesses 13 fundamental motor skills, partitioned into two subscales: locomotor skills and ball skills. The skills assessed in the locomotor subscale include run, gallop, one-legged hop, skip, jump, and slide. The skills assessed in the ball subscale include two-hand strike, one-hand strike, dribble, kick, catch, overhand throw, and underhand throw. Other than for motor skills assessment purposes, the TGMD has been suggested for training and improvement of specific motor skills [46].
In this study, testing stations were created for each skill (Table 4) and the evaluations were conducted observing the TGMD-3 assessment form guidelines [6], indicating the researcher to illustrate the proper execution of the skill, and then the subject to complete one practice trial, followed by two formal trials.
Each skill was evaluated by examining 3–5 performance criteria [45]. For instance, the gross motor skill named “dribbling” included three different criteria: make ball contact with one hand at the waist level; push (not slap) the ball with the fingertips; and maintain control for four consecutive bounces.
During the skill execution, the evaluator marked “1” in the score box for every performance criterion that the subject correctly demonstrated. If the subject did not demonstrate the appropriate criterion, a score of “0” was recorded in the score box. Total scores from the performance criteria over the two formal trials were summed to create a raw skill score. Raw skill scores were summed to provide a total raw score for either the locomotor or ball skill subscales or combined to provide a total TGMD-3 raw score. The maximum possible scores were 100 for the LBS subgroup, 46 for the LS subgroup, and 54 for the BS subgroup.

2.5. Data Collection

Data obtained consisted of the scores that subjects were given for the fine motor skills evaluations of day 1 (i.e., baseline) and day 5 (post gross motor training). Scores of the gross motor training were taken on day 2, day 3, and day 4. Although we collected data from gross motor training sessions, the TGMD-3 scores were only used for comparison between ages and groups, and not as a measure of gross motor proficiency.

2.6. Statistics

Data were analyzed using MATLAB_R2020b software (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA). Nonparametric analyses were conducted since the Shapiro-Wilk test revealed a non-normal distribution of data (p < 0.001). In order to observe whether motor performances would differ based on sex, we conducted a Mann-Whitney U-test for independent variables comparing girls’ and boys’ BBT scores as well as the girls’ and boys’ TGMD-3 scores. Moreover, to observe whether gross and fine motor performances improve with age, we used the Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient among the subjects’ grades and BBT scores as well as the subjects’ grades and TGMD-3 scores. For the same purpose, the Kruskal-Wallis test for independent variables was conducted using the BBT scores with grades. Similarly, potential differences in gross motor activity due to age were investigated by conducting the Kruskal-Wallis test using the TGMD-3 scores with grades. The Kruskal-Wallis test was followed by the Dunn-Bonferroni adjusted post-hoc test in the case of multiple comparisons. Furthermore, aiming to investigate whether higher fine motor skills performances are related to higher gross motor performances, we used the Spearman correlation among the subjects’ scores in the BBT and TGMD-3. In addition, in order to observe whether there is a difference in fine motor skills performances before and after a short intervention of gross motor training, we conducted a Friedman test for dependent variables between BBT scores at the baseline and post-gross motor skills training.

3. Results

3.1. Sex Differences in Motor Performance

Descriptive data confronting the males’ and females’ BBT and TGMD-3 scores are reported in Table 5 and Table 6, respectively. Furthermore, we explored the possibility that motor performances would differ based on sex when comparing the girls’ BBT scores with the boys’ BBT scores by conducting a Mann-Whitney U-test for independent variables. In a similar fashion, we also compared the girls’ TGMD-3 scores with the boys’ TGMD-3 scores. No significant differences were found in both the BBT and TGMD-3 scores between the boys’ and girls’ performances for all subgroups and across all sessions (Table 7 and Table 8).

3.2. Developmental Progression of Gross Motor Skills

TGMD-3 scores improved with age in all subgroups (LBS, LS, and BS) across all sessions, albeit with some exceptions (Table 9).
In order to investigate the relationship of gross motor skills with age, we compared the subjects’ grade with TGMD-3 scores using the Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient. Correlation between grades and TGMD-3 scores were found to be low to moderate and significant for all of the three gross motor training sessions, ranging from R = 0.33 to 0.59, p < 0.001, except for the LS subgroup in the 3rd session with p < 0.05 (Table 10).
Moreover, in order to evaluate possible differences in gross motor skills due to age, we compared the subjects’ TGMD-3 scores with grades by conducting the Kruskal-Wallis test for independent variables, followed by the Dunn-Bonferroni adjusted post-hoc test for multiple comparison. The analysis returned mixed results among the different sessions for LBS, LS, and BS subgroups, as significant differences were found between grades in all subgroups and sessions, except for the LS subgroup in the third session (Table 11).

3.3. Developmental Progression of Fine Motor Skills

BBT scores improved with age for the dominant and the non-dominant hand both for the baseline and the post-gross motor skills training assessments (Table 12).
In order to investigate the relationship of fine motor skills with age, we compared the subjects’ grade with BBT scores using the Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient. Specifically, the correlation between grade and BBT scores was found to be high and significant for the baseline assessment for both the dominant and non-dominant hand. The same was also found for the post-TGMD-3 assessment (Table 13).
Moreover, in order to evaluate possible differences in fine motor skills due to age, we compared the subjects’ BBT scores with grades by conducting the Kruskal-Wallis test for independent variables, followed by the Dunn-Bonferroni adjusted post-hoc test for multiple comparison. The analysis showed significant differences for the BBT scores among grades, both in the dominant hand and non-dominant hand (Table 14).
All comparisons were found to be statistically significant (both dominant and non-dominant hand with p < 0.001) except for the 3rd vs. 4th grade (p = 0.11 in the dominant hand, p = 0.30 in non-dominant hand) and the 4th vs. 5th grade (p = 0.10 in the dominant hand, p = 0.72 in non-dominant hand). As for the post TGMD-3 assessment, results were similar, as significant differences were found for the BBT scores among grades, both in the dominant hand and non-dominant hand. All comparisons were found to be statistically significant (both dominant and non-dominant hand with p < 0.001) except for the 3rd vs. 4th grade (p = 0.18 in the dominant hand, p = 0.27 in non-dominant hand) and the 4th vs. 5th grade (p = 0.17 in the dominant hand, p = 0.11 in the non-dominant hand).

3.4. Crosstalk between Gross and Fine Motor Skills

In order to investigate the relationship between fine gross and fine motor skills, the Spearman correlation was implemented on the subjects’ BBT and TGMD-3 scores (Table 15).
As for the LBS subgroup, results showed a statistically significant low to moderate correlation between the BBT and TGMD-3 scores (ranging from R = 0.48 to 0.62, p < 0.001). Regarding the LS subgroup, the correlation between the BBT and TGMD-3 scores were found to be low (ranging from R = 0.24 to 0.40), though statistically significant for all sessions (p < 0.05). Concerning the BS subgroup, the analysis indicated similar results to the LBS subgroup, thus a significant low to moderate correlation between the BBT and TGMD-3 scores (ranging from R = 0.40 to 0.57, p < 0.001).
Furthermore, both gross and fine motor skills seemed to improve as sessions progressed, with the exception of the 5th grade (Figure 1).
In order to investigate potential differences in gross motor skills due to the training intervention, the Friedman test was implemented on the TGMD-3 scores for all sessions (i.e., between the 1st and 2nd session, the 2nd and 3rd session, and the 1st and 3rd session). Though TGMD-3 scores seemed to improve as the sessions progressed (Figure 1), none of the changes among the sessions of gross motor training were found to be statistically significant (Table 16).
In order to investigate potential differences in fine motor performance due to gross motor training intervention, the Friedman test was implemented on the BBT scores before and after the TGMD-3 sessions (i.e., baseline and post-TGMD-3 training). Though BBT scores seemed to generally improve from baseline to post-TGMD-3 intervention (Figure 1), no significant results were found for both the dominant hand and non-dominant hand (Table 17).

4. Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to expand the focus pertaining to motor development during primary school age, at the same time investigating the influence of different short gross motor training on fine motor performance. The results showed a general improvement for both gross and fine motor performances with age (Table 9 and Table 12). Moreover, our findings confirm previous observations [9,13] showing a moderate to high correlation between gross and fine motor skills (Table 15). However, neither the improvements nor declines in a single grade performances were found to be significant (p > 0.05) throughout the experiment for both gross (Table 16) and fine motor performances (Table 17). Regarding the gross motor intervention, it is worth mentioning that we used the same test battery to train and assess the exact same skills during the experiment, which could result in a training effect of the specific skills. However, as the scope of the gross motor measurement was to investigate developmental changes (i.e., older children tend to perform better than younger ones with a continuous but non-linear trend), our experimental design did not intend to evaluate changes in gross motor performance across the intervention. Furthermore, it was possible to observe some differences in the developmental path among the three subgroups of gross motor training. Specifically, the LBS and BS subgroup presented consistent differences (p < 0.001) between children aged 6–8 and 9–11, while the LS subgroup presented less consistent differences over gross motor sessions between children aged 6–7 and 10–11 (Table 11). Concerning the influence of gross motor practice on fine motor skills, no differences in fine motor performances were found following the gross motor practice program (Table 17). Regarding sex-related motor performance, no differences were found between male and female performances for both gross and fine motor skills (Table 7 and Table 8).
In this study, both gross and fine motor skills significantly improved with age (Table 11 and Table 14). This trend was expected as competency in fundamental movement skills has been shown to follow an increasing developmental trajectory, with both gross and fine motor skills improving with chronological age [47,48,49]. Moreover, Bolger et al. [50] already observed that in primary school children, older children scored significantly higher than their younger peers in both locomotor and object-control scores in the TGMD-2. Therefore, in relation to the current literature and our findings, it seems that during primary school years, children’s gross and fine motor competence continuously improve with age (see Table 11 and Table 14 and Figure 1).
As was observed, as gross and fine motor skills follow different developmental paths [16,51], it is not surprising that we found the age-fine motor skills correlation to be higher than the age-gross motor skills correlation (Table 10 and Table 13). Moreover, fine motor performances did not differ between the 3rd and 4th grade and between the 4th and 5th grade (i.e., between children aged 9–10 and aged 10–11), although significant differences were found between the 3rd and 5th grade (i.e., between children aged 9 and 11, see Table 14). A possible explanation for this trend is that children aged 9–11 experience a period of stabilization in the physical growth and consolidation of both cognitive and neuro-motor abilities [52,53,54,55,56,57,58]. However, it should be mentioned that while for fine motor skills evaluation we used a single task test (i.e., the BBT), for gross motor assessment, we used a test battery of diversified tasks (i.e., the TGMD-3). As the scope of this paper was to provide a general evaluation of the effect of different types of gross motor training on fine motor performance, we limited the fine motor skills evaluation to a single test (widely used in the literature [27,32,33,34]). As gross motor control is generally considered as a sum of different subsets of skills [7], it is fitting to use different tasks when designing an overall gross motor training intervention. On the other hand, fine motor control is not classically defined by different subsets of skills, and was used in this study as an output measure. Furthermore, other studies have already compared a different number of tasks between gross and fine motor training tests [9,13,14,15,17,30]. Nonetheless, it is possible that the lower correlation and heterogeneity of results we found in the gross motor assessment among grades could be due to the higher request of motor variability [59]. Certainly, a different combination of gross and fine motor skills assessments could provide different results. However, this would go beyond the scope of this paper. Future studies should clarify the aspects related to the influence of different combination of gross and fine motor skills assessments on overall motor performance.
As for the relationship between gross and fine motor skills, high scores on gross motor skills assessment reflected high scores in fine motor skills evaluation across all the sessions of this study and for all the gross motor activity subgroups. These results are in line with the research work of Oberer et al. [13], which suggested a moderate level of relationship between gross and fine motor skills. In addition, Cameron et al. [9] already indicated that children with higher scores in fine motor skills evaluations tended to score higher on the gross motor assessments compared to children with lower scores in gross or fine motor assessments. However, it is worth noting that while both studies were conducted on kindergarten children [9,13], this study expanded the scope of the gross-fine motor relationship to the primary school years. Therefore, including the primary educational stage to the topic of gross–fine motor relationship, this study contributed to further explore the features of overall motor development across all years of childhood.
Although we found a correlation between gross and fine motor skills, this cannot be attributed to a direct influence of the gross motor training used in this study on fine motor performance. This apparent lack of influence may suggest that the designed gross motor training intervention was not adequate for influencing fine motor skills in the short-term, regardless of the type of gross motor activity. The intervention duration might be a critical point (i.e., three sessions of gross motor practice to influence fine motor performance). In this regard, although some studies have also shown that a short-term intervention could influence motor performances [30,60], the general notion is that short-term intervention would not elicit great changes in performances [61,62]. Thus, it is not surprising that the gross motor training in this study did not bring significant differences in fine motor performance. The results support the notion of a certain critical point for intervention duration [61,62]. Hence, more training time may be needed to observe a significant influence of gross motor training on fine motor skills.
Another doubtful element could be related to the design of the gross motor training program. Seeing that in this study we used three different motor training programs composed of numerous tasks (i.e., the TGMD-3 locomotor skills battery, ball skills battery, and the complete battery), it might be appropriate to focus on a reduced number of tasks for efficiently stimulating gross motor skills. As previously mentioned, this would reduce the demand for motor variability, allowing subjects to perform a more specific training that would return less heterogeneous results than those present in this study. Though the training used in this study was relatively task specific, it has been widely used in the literature for the assessment of general gross motor proficiency [35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45]. However, future studies will benefit from the examination of developmentally focused training as well as neutral training programs. Moreover, having found no differences in the type of gross motor training carried out, we would suggest future research to keep investigating the influence of both locomotor and object manipulation training on fine motor performance.
The topic of motor skills differences between male and female children appears to be contentious in the research literature. As for this study, no differences were found between male and female motor performances (Table 7 and Table 8). These results are in line with various research work [57,63] that suggested that there are no sex differences in fine motor skills in primary school children. In addition, Slykerman et al. [64] indicated that there may be no sex differences in the locomotor skills in children with a mean age of 6.5 years. However, other studies suggest otherwise. For instance, when assessing gross motor skills in school age children, Bolger et al. [50] found that boys scored significantly higher than girls in the object-control score while girls scored significantly higher in the locomotor score. For ages 7–8, it has been noted that boys develop ball skills earlier than girls and that girls acquire fine motor skills before boys [65]. Based on the results of this study and on recent literature, our cautious position is that when considering the whole late childhood development stage, there may be no clear differentiation between males and females in overall motor performances. However, these aspects should be investigated with further and dedicated research.

5. Conclusions

The study of interactions between gross and fine motor skills is important for identifying novel strategies to enhance motor learning during childhood. This research expands current findings regarding the relationship between gross and fine motor skills to the late childhood developmental stage. Moreover, our results align with previous findings regarding the positive correlation between gross and fine motor skills. Although we did not observe a short-term influence of gross motor practice on fine motor control, it is possible that longer interventions could provide a more prominent effect on fine motor performances. Thus, designing other types of interventions could be useful to deepen the interaction between the gross and fine motor domains during the children’s school years. Finally, this study showed that overall motor development appears to follow a specific trajectory in primary school subjects. In particular, both males and females aged 9–11 seem to experience a major step-up in both gross and fine motor proficiency compared to their younger peers (ages 6–8). Other than an academic audience, these findings are also valuable for teachers, educators, and trainers (i.e., professionals who design and put into practice children’s motor educational programs). Further studies are needed to better clarify the relationship between gross and fine motor proficiency during different stages of primary school education.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.S., E.J.C., R.B. and D.M.; methodology, V.S. and E.J.C.; software, V.S. and E.J.C.; formal analysis, V.S., E.J.C. and R.B.; investigation, V.S.; data curation, V.S., E.J.C. and R.B.; writing—original draft preparation, V.S.; writing—review and editing, E.J.C., R.B. and D.M.; visualization, V.S., E.J.C. and D.M.; supervision, D.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of Comitato Etico Area Vasta Centro AOUCareggi, Florence, Italy; Prot. N.0018234E, Rif. 63/12.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all the subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available on request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Burton, A.W.; Rodgerson, R.W. New Perspectives on the Assessment of Movement Skills and Motor Abilities. Adapt. Phys. Act. Q. 2001, 18, 347–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Robinson, L.E. The Relationship between Perceived Physical Competence and Fundamental Motor Skills in Preschool Children. Child Care Health Dev. 2011, 37, 589–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Goldstein, S.; Naglieri, J.A. (Eds.) Encyclopedia of Child Behavior and Development; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Cohen, E.J.; Bravi, R.; Bagni, M.A.; Minciacchi, D. Precision in Drawing and Tracing Tasks: Different Measures for Different Aspects of Fine Motor Control. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2018, 61, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bravi, R.; Ioannou, C.I.; Minciacchi, D.; Altenmüller, E. Assessment of the Effects of Kinesiotaping on Musical Motor Performance in Musicians Suffering from Focal Hand Dystonia: A Pilot Study. Clin. Rehabil. 2019, 33, 1636–1648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Ulrich, D. The Test of Gross Motor Development-3 (TGMD-3): Administration, Scoring, & International Norms. Hacet. J. Sport Sci. 2017, 24, 27–33. [Google Scholar]
  7. Webster, E.K.; Martin, C.K.; Staiano, A.E. Fundamental Motor Skills, Screen-Time, and Physical Activity in Preschoolers. J. Sport Health Sci. 2019, 8, 114–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Lipkin, P.H. Motor Development and Dysfunction. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2009, 643–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cameron, C.E.; Brock, L.L.; Murrah, W.M.; Bell, L.H.; Worzalla, S.L.; Grissmer, D.; Morrison, F.J. Fine Motor Skills and Executive Function Both Contribute to Kindergarten Achievement. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 1229–1244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. Seashore, H.G. Some Relationships of Fine and Gross Motor Abilities. Res. Q. Am. Assoc. Health Phys. Educ. Recreat. 1942, 13, 259–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Oxendine, J.B. Generality and Specificity in the Learning of Fine and Gross Motor Skills. Res. Q. Am. Assoc. Health Phys. Educ. Recreat. 1967, 38, 86–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Roebers, C.M.; Kauer, M. Motor and Cognitive Control in a Normative Sample of 7-Year-Olds. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 175–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Oberer, N.; Gashaj, V.; Roebers, C.M. Motor Skills in Kindergarten: Internal Structure, Cognitive Correlates and Relationships to Background Variables. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2017, 52, 170–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Dayem, T.S.A.E.; Salem, E.E.; Hadidy, E.I.E. Correlation between Gross Motor Activities and Hand Writing Skills in Elementary School Children. Trends Appl. Sci. Res. 2015, 10, 259–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Tortella, P.; Haga, M.; Loras, H.; Sigmundsson, H.; Fumagalli, G. Motor Skill Development in Italian Pre-School Children Induced by Structured Activities in a Specific Playground. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0160244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Souza, C.T.; Santos, D.C.; Tolocka, R.E.; Baltieri, L.; Gibim, N.C.; Habechian, F.A. Assessment of global motor performance and gross and fine motor skills of infants attending day care centers. Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 2010, 14, 309–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Amaro, N.; Coelho, L.; Cruz, J.; Matos, R.; Morouço, P. Correlation between Fine and Gross Motor Coordination in Children. Rev. Saúde Públ. Leiria 2014, 48, 273. [Google Scholar]
  18. Flatters, I.; Mushtaq, F.; Hill, L.J.B.; Rossiter, A.; Jarrett-Peet, K.; Culmer, P.; Holt, R.; Wilkie, R.M.; Mon-Williams, M. Children’s Head Movements and Postural Stability as a Function of Task. Exp. Brain Res. 2014, 232, 1953–1970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Dehghan, L.; Mirzakhani, N.; Rezaee, M.; Tabatabaee, M. The Relationship between Fine Motor Skills and Social Development and Maturation. Iran. Rehabil. J. 2017, 15, 407–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Leonard, H.C.; Hill, E.L. Review: The Impact of Motor Development on Typical and Atypical Social Cognition and Language: A Systematic Review. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 2014, 19, 163–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Van der Fels, I.M.J.; te Wierike, S.C.M.; Hartman, E.; Elferink-Gemser, M.T.; Smith, J.; Visscher, C. The Relationship between Motor Skills and Cognitive Skills in 4–16 Year Old Typically Developing Children: A Systematic Review. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2015, 18, 697–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Zeng, N.; Ayyub, M.; Sun, H.; Wen, X.; Xiang, P.; Gao, Z. Effects of Physical Activity on Motor Skills and Cognitive Development in Early Childhood: A Systematic Review. Biomed. Res. Int. 2017, 2017, 2760716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Fernandes, V.R.; Ribeiro, M.L.S.; Melo, T.; de Tarso Maciel-Pinheiro, P.; Guimarães, T.T.; Araújo, N.B.; Ribeiro, S.; Deslandes, A.C. Motor Coordination Correlates with Academic Achievement and Cognitive Function in Children. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Aadland, K.N.; Moe, V.F.; Aadland, E.; Anderssen, S.A.; Resaland, G.K.; Ommundsen, Y. Relationships between Physical Activity, Sedentary Time, Aerobic Fitness, Motor Skills and Executive Function and Academic Performance in Children. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 2017, 12, 10–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Cameron, C.E.; Cottone, E.A.; Murrah, W.M.; Grissmer, D.W. How Are Motor Skills Linked to Children’s School Performance and Academic Achievement? Child Dev. Perspect. 2016, 10, 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Geertsen, S.S.; Thomas, R.; Larsen, M.N.; Dahn, I.M.; Andersen, J.N.; Krause-Jensen, M.; Korup, V.; Nielsen, C.M.; Wienecke, J.; Ritz, C.; et al. Motor Skills and Exercise Capacity Are Associated with Objective Measures of Cognitive Functions and Academic Performance in Preadolescent Children. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0161960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Jongbloed-Pereboom, M.; Nijhuis-van der Sanden, M.W.G.; Steenbergen, B. Norm Scores of the Box and Block Test for Children Ages 3–10 Years. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2013, 67, 312–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Westendorp, M.; Hartman, E.; Houwen, S.; Smith, J.; Visscher, C. The relationship between gross motor skills and academic achievement in children with learning disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 2773–2779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Valentini, N.C.; Rudisill, M.E.; Bandeira, P.F.R.; Hastie, P.A. The development of a short form of the Test of Gross Motor Development-2 in Brazilian children: Validity and reliability. Child Care Health Dev. 2018, 44, 759–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bandeira, P.F.R.; Duncan, M.; Pessoa, M.L.; Soares, Í.; da Silva, L.; Mota, J.; Martins, C. TGMD-2 Short Version: Evidence of Validity and Associations with Sex, Age, and BMI in Preschool Children. J. Motor Learn. Dev. 2020, 8, 528–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Guest, L.; Balogh, R.; Dogra, S.; Lloyd, M. Examining the Impact of a Multi-Sport Camp for Girls Ages 8–11 With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Ther. Recreat. J. 2017, 51, 109–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Strong, W.B.; Malina, R.M.; Blimkie, C.J.R.; Daniels, S.R.; Dishman, R.K.; Gutin, B.; Hergenroeder, A.C.; Must, A.; Nixon, P.A.; Pivarnik, J.M.; et al. Evidence Based Physical Activity for School-Age Youth. J. Pediatr. 2005, 146, 732–737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Mathiowetz, V.; Federman, S.; Wiemer, D. Box and Block Test of Manual Dexterity: Norms for 6–19 Year Olds. Can. J. Occup. Ther. 1985, 52, 241–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Mathiowetz, V. Box and Blocks Test Instructions General Information (Derived from Mathiowetz et Al, 1985). Am. J. Occup. Ther. 1985, 39, 386–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Burns, R.D.; Fu, Y.; Hannon, J.C.; Brusseau, T.A. School Physical Activity Programming and Gross Motor Skills in Children. Am. J. Health Behav. 2017, 41, 591–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Temple, V.A.; Crane, J.R.; Brown, A.; Williams, B.-L.; Bell, R.I. Recreational Activities and Motor Skills of Children in Kindergarten. Phys. Educ. Sport Pedagog. 2016, 21, 268–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jones, R.A.; Okely, A.D.; Hinkley, T.; Batterham, M.; Burke, C. Promoting Gross Motor Skills and Physical Activity in Childcare: A Translational Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2016, 19, 744–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Sgrò, F.; Quinto, A.; Messana, L.; Pignato, S.; Lipoma, M. Assessment of Gross Motor Developmental Level in Italian Primary School Children. J. Phys. Educ. Sport 2017, 17, 1954–1959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Duncan, M.J.; Jones, V.; O’Brien, W.; Barnett, L.M.; Eyre, E.L.J. Self-Perceived and Actual Motor Competence in Young British Children. Percept. Mot. Skills 2018, 125, 251–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Scheuer, C.; Herrmann, C.; Bund, A. Motor Tests for Primary School Aged Children: A Systematic Review. J. Sports Sci. 2019, 37, 1097–1112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Griffiths, A.; Toovey, R.; Morgan, P.E.; Spittle, A.J. Psychometric Properties of Gross Motor Assessment Tools for Children: A Systematic Review. BMJ Open 2018, 8, e021734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Magistro, D.; Piumatti, G.; Carlevaro, F.; Sherar, L.B.; Esliger, D.W.; Bardaglio, G.; Magno, F.; Zecca, M.; Musella, G. Measurement Invariance of TGMD-3 in Children with and without Mental and Behavioral Disorders. Psychol. Assess. 2018, 30, 1421–1429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Rey, E.; Carballo-Fazanes, A.; Varela-Casal, C.; Abelairas-Gómez, C. Reliability of the Test of Gross Motor Development: A Systematic Review. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0236070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Ulrich, D.A. Introduction to the Special Section: Evaluation of the Psychometric Properties of the TGMD-3. J. Mot. Learn. Dev. 2017, 5, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Allen, K.A.; Bredero, B.; Van Damme, T.; Ulrich, D.A.; Simons, J. Test of Gross Motor Development-3 (TGMD-3) with the Use of Visual Supports for Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Validity and Reliability. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2017, 47, 813–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Wrotniak, B.H.; Epstein, L.H.; Dorn, J.M.; Jones, K.E.; Kondilis, V.A. The Relationship between Motor Proficiency and Physical Activity in Children. Pediatrics 2006, 118, e1758–e1765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. McKenzie, T.L.; Sallis, J.F.; Broyles, S.L.; Zive, M.M.; Nader, P.R.; Berry, C.C.; Brennan, J.J. Childhood Movement Skills: Predictors of Physical Activity in Anglo American and Mexican American Adolescents? Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2002, 73, 238–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Okely, A.D.; Booth, M.L.; Chey, T. Relationships between Body Composition and Fundamental Movement Skills among Children and Adolescents. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2004, 75, 238–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Williams, H.G.; Pfeiffer, K.A.; O’Neill, J.R.; Dowda, M.; McIver, K.L.; Brown, W.H.; Pate, R.R. Motor Skill Performance and Physical Activity in Preschool Children. Obesity 2008, 16, 1421–1426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Bolger, L.E.; Bolger, L.A.; O’ Neill, C.; Coughlan, E.; O’Brien, W.; Lacey, S.; Burns, C. Age and Sex Differences in Fundamental Movement Skills Among a Cohort of Irish School Children. J. Mot. Learn. Dev. 2018, 6, 81–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Haibach, P.S.; Collier, D.H.; Reid, G. Motor Learning and Development; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  52. Rosenbloom, L. Motor Development in Early and Later Childhood: Longitudinal Approaches. Arch. Dis. Child. 1994, 71, 391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Lin, J.P.; Brown, J.K.; Walsh, E.G. Physiological Maturation of Muscles in Childhood. Lancet 1994, 343, 1386–1389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Savelsbergh, G.; Wimmers, R.; Van Der Kamp, J.; Davids, K. The Development of Movement Control and Coordination. In Current Issues in Developmental Psychology; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Damon, W.; Eisenberg, N. Handbook of Child Psychology, Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, 5th ed.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006; Volume 3. [Google Scholar]
  56. Fair, D.A.; Cohen, A.L.; Dosenbach, N.U.F.; Church, J.A.; Miezin, F.M.; Barch, D.M.; Raichle, M.E.; Petersen, S.E.; Schlaggar, B.L. The Maturing Architecture of the Brain’s Default Network. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 4028–4032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Cohen, E.J.; Bravi, R.; Minciacchi, D. Assessing the Development of Fine Motor Control in Elementary School Children Using Drawing and Tracing Tasks. Percept. Mot. Skills 2021, 128, 605–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Albuquerque, M.R.; Rennó, G.V.C.; Bruzi, A.T.; Fortes, L.D.S.; Malloy-Diniz, L.F. Association between motor competence and executive functions in children. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 2021, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Eldred, K.; Darrah, J. Using Cluster Analysis to Interpret the Variability of Gross Motor Scores of Children with Typical Development. Phys. Ther. 2010, 90, 1510–1518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Cohen, E.J.; Bravi, R.; Minciacchi, D. The effect of fidget spinners on fine motor control. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 3144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Cohen, K.; Lubans, D.; Morgan, P.; Plotnikoff, R.; Callister, R. Improving Fundamental Movement Skill Competency among Children Attending Schools in Low-Income Communities: The SCORES Cluster RCT. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2014, 18, e12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Lai, S.K.; Costigan, S.A.; Morgan, P.J.; Lubans, D.R.; Stodden, D.F.; Salmon, J.; Barnett, L.M. Do School-Based Interventions Focusing on Physical Activity, Fitness, or Fundamental Movement Skill Competency Produce a Sustained Impact in These Outcomes in Children and Adolescents? A Systematic Review of Follow-up Studies. Sports Med. 2014, 44, 67–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Blank, R.; Miller, V.; Von Voß, H.; Von Kries, R. Effects of Age on Distally and Proximally Generated Drawing Movements: A Kinematic Analysis of School Children and Adults. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 1999, 41, 592–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Slykerman, S.; Ridgers, N.D.; Stevenson, C.; Barnett, L.M. How Important Is Young Children’s Actual and Perceived Movement Skill Competence to Their Physical Activity? J. Sci. Med. Sport 2016, 19, 488–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Junaid, K.A.; Fellowes, S. Gender Differences in the Attainment of Motor Skills on the Movement Assessment Battery for Children. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2006, 26, 5–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Results for gross and fine motor proficiency for the LBS subgroup. The scores are reported as a normalized mean of the BBT and the TGMD-3 per grade (dots) and standard deviations (lines). Performances gradually improved with age for both gross and fine motor skills, but this overall improvement was more evident between the 1st and 4th grade, 1st and 5th grade, 2nd and 4th grade, and 2nd and 5th grade, as evidenced by the significant difference (p < 0.001) in general motor proficiency (see Table 11 and Table 14). Conversely, no significant differences were found in both gross and fine motor performances as the sessions progressed (see Table 16 and Table 17).
Figure 1. Results for gross and fine motor proficiency for the LBS subgroup. The scores are reported as a normalized mean of the BBT and the TGMD-3 per grade (dots) and standard deviations (lines). Performances gradually improved with age for both gross and fine motor skills, but this overall improvement was more evident between the 1st and 4th grade, 1st and 5th grade, 2nd and 4th grade, and 2nd and 5th grade, as evidenced by the significant difference (p < 0.001) in general motor proficiency (see Table 11 and Table 14). Conversely, no significant differences were found in both gross and fine motor performances as the sessions progressed (see Table 16 and Table 17).
Ijerph 18 11387 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of the participants.
Table 1. Characteristics of the participants.
GradeAge (Years)nBoysGirlsAge (Years)
1st Grade6–7664026M 1 ± SD 28.57 ± 2.33
2nd Grade7–8502921Range6–10.6
3rd Grade8–9483018
4th Grade9–10452718
5th Grade10–11312011
Total 240127113
1 M = mean; 2 SD = standard deviation.
Table 2. Number of subjects per grade and subgroups of gross motor skills training.
Table 2. Number of subjects per grade and subgroups of gross motor skills training.
GradeLBS 1LS 2BS 3
1st Grade222222
2nd Grade171617
3rd Grade171615
4th Grade151515
5th Grade11911
Total (%)77 (32.1%)81 (33.7%)82 (34.1%)
1 LBS = Locomotor and ball skills subgroup. 2 LS = Locomotor skills subgroup. 3 BS = Ball skills subgroup.
Table 3. Timeline of the study.
Table 3. Timeline of the study.
ActivityFMS 1 Evaluation
(Baseline)
GMS 2 TrainingFMS 1 Evaluation
(Post GMS 2 Training)
(Test)BBT 3TGMD-3 4BBT 3
Day #Day 1Day 2Day 3Day 4Day 5
Test time5–10 min30–45 min5–10 min
1 FMS = Fine Motor Skills; 2 GMS = Gross Motor Skills; 3 BBT = Box and Block Test; 4 TGMD-3 = Test of Gross Motor Development—Third Edition.
Table 4. Equipment needed for the TGMD-3 stations.
Table 4. Equipment needed for the TGMD-3 stations.
Locomotor SkillsEquipmentMaterialMeasures
ALLMini markersPolyethyleneBase diameter 9.52 cm,
height 16.51 cm
Ball SkillsEquipmentMaterialMeasures
Two hand strikeBatting tee
Baseball
Bat
Rubber, latex free
Rigid polyethylene
Plastic
44.19 cm 43.69 cm 7.62 cm
Diameter 7.62 cm
Barrel diameter 5.72 cm,
height 76.2 cm
One hand strikePickleball paddle Tennis ballPlastic
Rubber and latex
Length 35.6 cm, plastic grid 1.3 cm
Non-pressurized
DribblingPlayground ballsNylon and rubberDiameter 21.59 cm
KickingPlayground ballsNylon and rubberDiameter 21.59 cm
CatchBaseballRigid polyethyleneDiameter 7.62 cm
Overhand throwTennis ballRubber and latexNon-pressurized
Underhand throwTennis ballRubber and latexNon-pressurized
Table 5. Scores 1 of the BBT divided by sex (males M, females F).
Table 5. Scores 1 of the BBT divided by sex (males M, females F).
Dominant HandNon-Dominant Hand
BaselinePostBaselinePost
MFMFMFMF
55 (7)54 (7)50.5 (9)50 (8)55 (8)54 (7)51 (9.25)50 (8)
1 Median (interquartile range).
Table 6. Scores 1 of the TGMD-3 divided by sex (males M, females F).
Table 6. Scores 1 of the TGMD-3 divided by sex (males M, females F).
LBS 2 SubgroupLS 3 SubgroupBS 4 Subgroup
SessionsSessionsSessions
1st2nd3rd1st2nd3rd1st2nd3rd
MFMFMFMFMFMFMFMFMF
80 (13)81 (14)82 (15)81 (16)85 (13)82 (13)46 (9)44 (9)45 (7)42 (8.5)46 (8)44 (10)38 (7.3)36 (5)38 (5.3)36 (8.5)38 (5)37 (5.5)
1 Median (interquartile range), 2 LBS = Locomotor and ball skills subgroup, 3 LS = Locomotor skills subgroup, 4 BS = Ball skills subgroup.
Table 7. Mann-Whitney U-test between male and female BBT scores.
Table 7. Mann-Whitney U-test between male and female BBT scores.
DataBaselinePost
Dominant handU70257188.5
Z0.310.62
p0.760.53
Non-dominant handU71817591.5
Z0.611.40
p0.540.16
Table 8. Mann-Whitney U-test between male and female TGMD-3 scores.
Table 8. Mann-Whitney U-test between male and female TGMD-3 scores.
SubgroupData1st Session2nd Session3rd Session
LBS 1U762.5748.5724.5
Z0.430.560.79
p0.670.580.43
LS 2U618582.5670
Z1.381.740.86
p0.170.840.39
BS 3U529.5526.5517
Z1.631.671.76
p0.100.940.08
1 LBS = Locomotor and ball skills subgroup. 2 LS = Locomotor skills subgroup. 3 BS = Ball skills subgroup.
Table 9. TGMD-3 scores 1 for all grades and all subgroups of gross motor skills training.
Table 9. TGMD-3 scores 1 for all grades and all subgroups of gross motor skills training.
LBS 2 SubgroupLS 3 SubgroupBS 4 Subgroup
SessionsSessionsSessions
Grade1st2nd3rdGrade1st2nd3rdGrade1st2nd3rd
1st grade74.5
(8.5)
75.5
(10)
78
(12.25)
1st grade33
(6.75)
34.5
(7.5)
36
(3.25)
1st grade40.5
(8.5)
39.5
(8.25)
40
(7.5)
2nd grade75
(12.5)
75
(16)
77
(15.5)
2nd grade36
(4.25)
37.5
(8.75)
37
(6.75)
2nd grade45
(11.5)
45
(9)
43
(10)
3rd grade82
(11.5)
82
(12.5)
84
(11.5)
3rd grade37
(3.75)
38
(4.75)
37.5
(5.75)
3rd grade45
(8)
44
(7)
45
(6)
4th grade87
(11)
86
(9)
87
(9)
4th grade38
(8)
40
(5)
39
(7)
4th grade47
(6)
45
(3)
48
(6)
5th grade87
(9)
90
(9)
91
(10)
5th grade39
(5)
40
(5)
38
(3.5)
5th grade51
(5)
51
(3)
50
(2)
1 Median (interquartile range), 2 LBS = Locomotor and ball skills subgroup, 3 LS = Locomotor skills subgroup, 4 BS = Ball skills subgroup.
Table 10. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient between TGMD-3 scores and grades.
Table 10. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient between TGMD-3 scores and grades.
Subgroup1st Session2nd Session3rd Session
LBS 1R = 0.52,R = 0.59,R = 0.53,
p < 0.001p < 0.001p < 0.001
LS 2R = 0.39,R = 0.42,R = 0.33,
p < 0.001p < 0.001p < 0.001
BS 3R = 0.55,R = 0.50,R = 0.54,
p < 0.001p < 0.001p < 0.001
1 LBS = Locomotor and ball skills subgroup, 2 LS = Locomotor skills subgroup, 3 BS = Ball skills subgroup.
Table 11. Kruskal-Wallis test followed by the Dunn-Bonferroni post-hoc between TGMD-3 scores and grades.
Table 11. Kruskal-Wallis test followed by the Dunn-Bonferroni post-hoc between TGMD-3 scores and grades.
SubgroupData1st Session2nd Session3rd Session
LBS 1Chi Sq33.5329.8424.00
d.f. 4444
p<0.001<0.001<0.001
Post-hoc 51 vs.4; 1 vs.5;
2 vs.4; 2 vs.5
1 vs.4; 1 vs.5;
2 vs.4; 2 vs.5
1 vs.4; 1 vs.5;
2 vs.4; 2 vs.5
LS 2Chi Sq13.4113.538.94
d.f. 4444
p<0.05<0.05=0.06
Post-hoc 51 vs.51 vs.4; 1 vs.5//
BS 3Chi Sq26.827.8926.69
d.f. 4444
p<0.001<0.001<0.001
Post-hoc 51 vs.4; 1 vs.5; 2 vs.51 vs.5; 2 vs.5;
3 vs.5; 4 vs.5
1 vs.4; 1 vs.5; 2 vs.5
1 LBS = Locomotor and ball skills subgroup. 2 LS = Locomotor skills subgroup. 3 BS = Ball skills subgroup. 4 d.f. = degrees of freedom. 5 Refers to the significant difference (p < 0.05) between the single grades.
Table 12. BBT scores1 comparison between the baseline and post-gross motor skills training assessments.
Table 12. BBT scores1 comparison between the baseline and post-gross motor skills training assessments.
Dominant HandNon-Dominant Hand
GradeBaselinePostBaselinePost
1st grade50 (3)52 (2)45 (2)46 (1)
2nd grade53 (2)54 (2)49.5 (2.25)50 (2.25)
3rd grade56 (1.75)57 (2.25)53.5 (3)54 (2)
4th grade57 (2)59 (3)54 (3)55 (2)
5th grade62 (3)63 (4)56 (3)57 (3)
1 Median (interquartile range).
Table 13. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient between BBT scores and grades.
Table 13. Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient between BBT scores and grades.
Dominant HandNon-Dominant Hand
BaselinePostBaselinePost
R = 0.95,
p < 0.001
R = 0.94,
p < 0.001
R = 0.94,
p < 0.001
R = 0.92,
p < 0.001
Table 14. Kruskal-Wallis test followed by the Dunn-Bonferroni post-hoc between BBT scores and grades.
Table 14. Kruskal-Wallis test followed by the Dunn-Bonferroni post-hoc between BBT scores and grades.
DataBaselinePost
Dominant handChi Sq206.8203.48
d.f. 144
p<0.001<0.001
Post-hoc exceptions 23 vs.4; 4 vs.53 vs.4; 4 vs.5
Non-dominant handChi Sq203.48200.48
d.f. 144
p<0.001<0.001
Post-hoc exceptions 23 vs.4; 4 vs.53 vs.4; 4 vs.5
1 d.f. = degrees of freedom. 2 Refers to the non-significant differences (p > 0.05) between single grades.
Table 15. Spearman correlation between BBT scores and TGMD-3 scores.
Table 15. Spearman correlation between BBT scores and TGMD-3 scores.
LBS 1
BBT Evaluations1st Session2nd Session3rd Session
BaselineDominant handR = 0.56
p < 0.001
R = 0.56
p < 0.001
R = 0.50
p < 0.001
Non-dominant handR = 0.58
p < 0.001
R = 0.57
p < 0.001
R = 0.53
p < 0.001
PostDominant handR = 0.57
p < 0.001
R = 0.55
p < 0.001
R = 0.48
p < 0.001
Non-dominant handR = 0.62
p < 0.001
R = 0.61
p < 0.001
R = 0.56
p < 0.001
LS 2
BaselineDominant handR = 0.39
p < 0.001
R = 0.40
p < 0.001
R = 0.33
p < 0.05
Non-dominant handR = 0.33
p < 0.05
R = 0.38
p < 0.001
R = 0.24
p < 0.05
PostDominant handR = 0.40
p < 0.001
R = 0.43
p < 0.001
R = 0.36
p < 0.05
Non-dominant handR = 0.36
p < 0.05
R=0.40
p < 0.001
R = 0.27
p < 0.05
BS 3
BaselineDominant handR = 0.55
p < 0.001
R = 0.51
p < 0.001
R = 0.57
p < 0.001
Non-dominant handR = 0.54
p < 0.001
R = 0.47
p < 0.001
R = 0.50
p < 0.001
PostDominant handR = 0.56
p < 0.001
R = 0.51
p < 0.001
R = 0.55
p < 0.001
Non-dominant handR = 0.48
p < 0.001
R = 0.40
p < 0.001
R = 0.45
p < 0.001
1 LBS = Locomotor and ball skills subgroup. 2 LS = Locomotor skills subgroup. 3 BS = Ball skills subgroup.
Table 16. Statistical significance (p-value) of the changes in gross motor performance during the intervention.
Table 16. Statistical significance (p-value) of the changes in gross motor performance during the intervention.
LBS 1 Subgroup LS 2 SubgroupBS 3 Subgroup
Sessions SessionsSessions
Grade1st vs. 2nd2nd vs. 3rd1st vs. 3rdGrade1st vs. 2nd2nd vs. 3rd 1st vs. 3rd Grade1st vs. 2nd 2nd vs. 3rd 1st vs. 3rd
1st gradep = 0.16p = 0.82p = 0.061st gradep = 0.08p = 0.13p = 0.651st gradep = 0.52p = 0.28p = 0.15
2nd gradep = 0.24p = 0.25p = 0.362nd gradep = 0.19p = 0.91p = 0.312nd gradep = 0.38p = 0.77p = 0.82
3rd gradep = 0.31p = 0.69p = 0.453rd gradep = 0.11p = 0.72p = 0.593rd gradep = 0.26p = 0.54p = 0.69
4th gradep = 0.22p = 0.18p = 0.344th gradep = 0.85p = 0.45p = 0.534th gradep = 0.96p = 0.39p = 0.14
5th gradep = 0.09p = 0.45p = 0.215th gradep = 0.68p = 0.78p = 0.565th gradep = 0.17p = 0.16p = 0.33
1 LBS = Locomotor and ball skills subgroup, 2 LS = Locomotor skills subgroup, 3 BS = Ball skills subgroup.
Table 17. Friedman’s test between BBT scores at the baseline and BBT scores post gross motor skills training.
Table 17. Friedman’s test between BBT scores at the baseline and BBT scores post gross motor skills training.
Dominant HandNon-Dominant Hand
Gradep-ValueChi-Sqd.f. 1p-ValueChi-Sqd.f.
1st grade0.430.6810.740.631
2nd grade0.110.5010.880.411
3rd grade0.240.8610.660.741
4th grade0.090.2510.130.131
5th grade0.080.1910.250.361
1 d.f. = degrees of freedom.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Sorgente, V.; Cohen, E.J.; Bravi, R.; Minciacchi, D. Crosstalk between Gross and Fine Motor Domains during Late Childhood: The Influence of Gross Motor Training on Fine Motor Performances in Primary School Children. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 11387. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph182111387

AMA Style

Sorgente V, Cohen EJ, Bravi R, Minciacchi D. Crosstalk between Gross and Fine Motor Domains during Late Childhood: The Influence of Gross Motor Training on Fine Motor Performances in Primary School Children. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2021; 18(21):11387. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph182111387

Chicago/Turabian Style

Sorgente, Vincenzo, Erez James Cohen, Riccardo Bravi, and Diego Minciacchi. 2021. "Crosstalk between Gross and Fine Motor Domains during Late Childhood: The Influence of Gross Motor Training on Fine Motor Performances in Primary School Children" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18, no. 21: 11387. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/ijerph182111387

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop