Next Article in Journal
Effect of Changes in Surgical Strategies for the Treatment of Primary Rhegmatogenous Retinal Detachment on Functional and Anatomical Outcomes: A Retrospective Analysis of 812 Cases from the Years 2004 to 2012
Next Article in Special Issue
App-Based Mindfulness Meditation Training and an Audiobook Intervention Reduce Symptom Severity but Do Not Modify Backward Inhibition in Adolescent Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Evidence from an EEG Study
Previous Article in Journal
Behavioral Interventions on Periodontitis Patients to Improve Oral Hygiene: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Dysfunctional Beliefs and Cognitive Performance across Symptom Dimensions in Childhood and Adolescent OCD
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Impulsivity and Attention in Obsessive Compulsive and Tic Disorders: Mismatch in Self-Report and Behavioural Data

1
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Hospital Cologne, University of Cologne, 50923 Cologne, Germany
2
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic Medicine, Johanniter Hospital Oberhausen, 46145 Oberhausen, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Submission received: 15 January 2023 / Revised: 5 March 2023 / Accepted: 7 March 2023 / Published: 15 March 2023

Abstract

:
Impulsivity is a multidimensional, cross-diagnostic behavioural construct that has been described in various psychiatric disorders including obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and Tourette syndrome (TS). Different interpretations of results in the past have raised the question of heightened impulsivity as an explanatory model for self-described impulsive behaviour, especially in OCD. Our study included 16 patients with OCD, 14 patients with TS, and 28 healthy control subjects (HC). Self-assessed impulsivity was examined by the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11), and the behavioural test used was the immediate and delayed memory task (IMT/DMT). Significantly heightened self-assessed impulsivity of the patient collective compared to HC could be observed in in only one dimension: lack of attention (χ2 (2) = 24.910, p < 0.001). Post-hoc tests were performed using Bonferroni adjusted alpha levels of 0.0167 per test (0.05/3) and revealed significantly higher scores in patients with OCD (M = 19.57, SD = 2.82), z = 4.292, p < 0.001 as with TS (M = 19.38, SD = 3.62), z = 3.832, p < 0.001 compared to HC (M = 13.78, SD = 3.18). In patients with OCD, correlations between the dimension of obsessive thoughts with a lack of attention in the form of first-order factor cognitive instability could be shown (n = 14, p = 0.024, rs = 0.599) while in patients with TS, tic symptomatology correlated significantly with second-order factor attentional impulsivity (n = 12, p = 0.027, rs = 0.635). In behavioural testing, no significant group differences could be observed either in impulsive behaviour (IMT: χ2 (2) = 4.709, p = 0.824; DMT: χ2 (2) = 0.126, p = 0.939) or in sustained attention (IMT: χ2 (2) = 0.388, p = 0.095; DMT: χ2 (2) = 0.663, p = 0.718). Heightened impulsivity as an explanatory model for the observed lack of attention, especially in patients with OCD, should be questioned and interpretation biases considered in the future. The necessity of a multidimensional approach to the research of impulsivity is underscored by our results.

1. Introduction

In day-to-day life, we are confronted with a myriad of stimuli that influence our decisions and behaviour. Inhibitory control mechanisms allow us to act flexibly and consider ourselves in complex situations. Impaired inhibitory control mechanisms often entail pathological impulsivity [1]. Impulsivity represents a complex multidimensional behavioural construct with varying definitions (e.g., tending to act thoughtlessly and inconsiderately; an insensitivity to consequences; the preference of a smaller, immediate, as opposed to a larger reward at a later point of time; or the inability to suppress motor impulses) [2,3,4]. Usually, pathological impulsivity has negative consequences for the affected person or their environment [5]. In everyday life, these consequences can be as heterogenous as its dimensions: deficits in communicating with fellow human beings without being able to control your emotions [6] (e.g., an outburst of anger towards colleagues). A rash, unpredictable action (e.g., risky driving behaviours [7]) can lead to health or financial consequences for those acting impulsively as well as for others, problematic Internet use, nicotine smoking, or excessive use of alcohol [8]. Impulsivity is also discussed as a risk factor for non-suicidal self-injuries [9] and seems to be an important factor of lower quality of life in different psychiatric disorders [10,11].
Negative consequences of a lack of attention as a dimension of impulsivity include problems in staying concentrated and not being distracted from an important task [12] (e.g., writing an important term paper or when giving a presentation). Impulsivity in the form of careless actions may represent overlooking a component of a cooking recipe with moderate consequence, but when crossing a road inattentively, it can become dangerous.
It must be mentioned that the focus of research on impulsive behaviour has been on its negative aspects. However impulsive behaviour can also be helpful and advantageous in some situations, particularly when a fast and spontaneous reaction is essential [13] (e.g., by seizing the moment). Impulsive decision making can prevent thinking circumstances apart and thus missing opportunities. This type of impulsivity is called functional impulsivity [14]. An important example from an evolutionary point of view could be an impulsive fight or flight behaviour as a response to an unpredictable attack [15].
It has become clear that impulsivity is a diverse construct with advantageous as well as disadvantageous aspects and multiple dimensions. Depending on the focus of the investigation of past work regarding effect and dimensionality, various test methods have been created by factor analysis and used in studies including self-assessment as well as behavioural examinations [12,16,17,18,19,20,21].
Impulsivity has been observed in various psychiatric disorders [22]. Patients with bipolar disorder, for example, show more inattentive behaviour [23] and the extent of impulsive behaviour seems to correlate with poorer outcome and heightened suicidality in bipolar disorder [24]. In patients with substance use disorder, it has been described as a risk factor for the age of onset, treatment outcome, and relapse risk in various types of self-assessed and behavioural tests [25,26,27]. In patients with borderline personality disorders, impulsivity is considered as a key feature with a wide range of clinical manifestations (e.g., avoiding the feeling of emptiness through impulsive actions or self-harm). Impulsivity is argued to be a crucial target in psychotherapeutical treatment [28].
Attention plays a central role in the study of behaviour and is a relevant cross-diagnostic factor. While it can be seen as an independent, multi-layered construct with different dimensions and definitions [29], a lack of attention is also described in the context of impulsive behaviour [12]. This study focused on the research of impulsive behaviour in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) and patients with Tourette syndrome (TS) in the most-used questionnaire, the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11). The questionnaire meets the dimensional approach of impulsivity and includes deficits of attention, which was of particular interest to us as there has been ongoing discussion in the past regarding its interpretation. Attentional deficits have been observed in patients with OCD in the past, and authors have drawn different conclusions regarding heightened impulsivity as an explanatory model. This was due to evidence of non-increased impulsive behaviour in the other dimensions of motor and non-planning as well as evidence of possible confounding factors such as obsessive thoughts [30,31,32]. Patients with TS have also been assessed for impulsive behaviour using the BIS-11 in the past. However, due to the different hypotheses to be tested, multidimensionality was not always considered [33,34], or if so, a direct comparison between TS and healthy control subjects was not made [30,35].
It has become clear that due to the diverse influence of impulsivity on our everyday behaviour and the resulting consequences in dealing with ourselves and our fellow human beings, it is of great interest in research. This study seeks to better understand the subtle distinctions and overlaps of its multifaceted constructs and dimensions by comparing patients with OCD, patients with TS, and healthy control subjects (HC). A central feature of this study is the detailed consideration of each of the dimensions of impulsive behaviour, the comparison between the patient collective and HC as well as the use of an additional behavioural test in the form of the Immediate and Delayed Memory Task (IMT/DMT).

2. Materials and Methods

Patients were recruited from the interdisciplinary outpatient clinic for obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders at University Hospital Cologne. Handouts and information about the study were placed in the waiting area. If patients showed interest in the study, scientific staff, not involved in the treatment of the patients, would provide them with further information. All participants provided written informed consent for the procedure and participation in the study. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University Hospital Cologne. The recruitment of HC was conducted by telephone contact with persons in a database who had already participated in previous studies at the Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy and had consented to further contact for study purposes. The study included 16 patients with OCD, 14 patients with TS, and 28 HC. Patients were pre-diagnosed in our outpatient clinic based on a semi-structured clinical interview by an experienced clinician. In our study protocol, pre-diagnosed OCD patients were tested with the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R) and patients with pre-diagnosed TS with the Adult Tic Questionnaire (ATQ) to evaluate the current severity of the respective symptoms. The OCI-R is a theoretically driven and brief self-report measure for the assessment of obsessive-compulsive symptoms [36] and captures the full range of OCD symptoms with excellent psychometric properties [37]. Healthy control participants had screenings for mental disorders before as they had participated in previous studies. In our study, they were tested with the OCI-R and the ATQ to screen for subclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms or tics.
The subjects completed a behavioural test as well as various questionnaires that were performed in randomised order. The behavioural test used was the IMT/DMT. It is a continuous performance test to study sustained attention, working memory, and impulsivity [21,38]. The response of the subjects to target-directed and non-target-directed stimuli was distinguished, which differed only marginally. The more frequent the response to target-directed stimuli (correct responses), the more attentive the patient. The more frequent the response to non-target stimuli (commission errors), the more impulsive the patient’s behaviour [39,40]. The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) is the most widely used questionnaire to assess impulsivity by self-assessment and contains 30 items in total [12]. Each of these items is assigned to one of six different first-order factor dimensions. Two first-order factor dimensions in turn form one second-order factor dimension, leaving three second-order factor dimensions (attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsivity) in total (see Table 1).
The OCI-R is an 18-item questionnaire for the self-assessment of obsessive-compulsive symptoms that are divided into six dimensions and a total score [41]. The ATQ is designed to examine tic symptoms by self-assessment. Tic symptoms are divided into 14 items for motor and 13 items for phonic tics [42]. The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) is one of the most commonly used questionnaires to assess depressive symptomatology and includes 21 items [43]. The Wender Utah Rating Scale (WURS) retrospectively allows for the assessment of the severity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in an adult’s childhood. The German-language short form (WURS-K) was used in this study and contains 25 items [44]. All statistical analyses were performed with commercially available statistical software (IBM SPSS, version 26). The collected data were subdivided into patients with OCD, patients with TS and HC based on the diagnoses, and the resulting group assignment. The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test did not show a normal distribution in all parameters of the data used (see Table A1 and Table A2). This necessitated the use of nonparametric statistical procedures. First, a descriptive review of the collected data was performed. Then, to test for differences in the central tendencies between the groups, the Kruskal–Wallis test was performed. If the results of the Kruskal–Wallis test were significant, additional post-hoc tests were performed using Bonferroni adjusted alpha levels of 0.0167 per test (0.05/3). In order to draw conclusions about the correlations between the collected data of different tests, the Spearman rank correlation coefficient rs was also determined. According to Cohen, correlations of rs <0.3 are considered weak, rs = 0.3–0.5 are moderately strong, and rs >0.5 are strong [45]. The significance level was defined as 5%.

3. Results

3.1. Patient Collective

Demographic Data

Included in the study were 16 patients with OCD (10 male, six female) with a mean age of 36.31 years (±13.65), 14 patients with TS (11 male, three female) with a mean age of 28.79 (±4.89), and 28 HC with a mean age of 38.11 (±13.84).

3.2. Reliability Analysis

Cronbach′s α for the BIS-11 total score was 0.77 in the overall sample and ranged from 0.72 to 0.81 across the three groups (OCD, TS, and HC). Examining the ATQ Cronbach’s α in patients with TS was 0.89 for the total score, 0.79for motor tics, and 0.83 for vocal tics. Patients with OCD showed Cronbach’s α in the OCI-R total score of 0.87 and ranged from 0.76 to 0.98 in the subscales. The WURS-K showed a Cronbach’s α of 0.75 in patients with TS, 0.85 in patients with OCD, and 0.79 in HC. Cronbach’s α for the BDI-2 total score was 0.88 in patients with TS, 0.95 in patients with OCD, and 0.75 in HC.

3.3. BIS-11

3.3.1. Kruskal–Wallis Test

Results from the Kruskal-Wallis Test are shown in Table 2.

3.3.2. Bonferroni Post-Hoc Test

Results from the Bonferroni Post-Hoc Test are shown in Table 3.

3.4. IMT/DMT

Kruskal–Wallis Test

In the evaluation of the Correct Responses objectifying sustained attention, no significant group differences in either the IMT (χ2 (2) = 0.388, p = 0.095) or the DMT (χ2 (2) = 0.663, p = 0.718) were found. Commission Errors account for impulsivity and showed no significant group differences in either the IMT (χ2 (2) = 4.709, p = 0.824) or the DMT (χ2 (2) = 0.126, p = 0.939). The Commission Errors/Correct Responses ratio, also used for the measurement of impulsivity, did not show significant group differences in either the IMT (χ2 (2) = 0.411, p = 0.814) or the DMT (χ2 (2) = 0.474, p = 0.789).

3.5. Correlations

3.5.1. Patients with TS

In patients with TS, tic severity correlated strongly positively in the ATQ total score with the second-order factor attentional impulsivity of the BIS-11 questionnaire (n = 12, p = 0.027, rs = 0.635).

3.5.2. Patients with OCD

In patients with OCD, the symptom dimension obsessive thoughts of the OCI-R correlated strongly positively with the dimension cognitive instability (first-order factor) of the BIS-11 (n = 14, p = 0.024, rs = 0.599). Moreover, the symptom dimension controlling of the OCI-R showed a strong positive correlation with the dimension cognitive instability (first-order factor) of the BIS-11 (n = 14, p = 0.001, rs = 0.758).

3.6. Comorbidities

Depressive symptoms were examined using the BDI-II (for results, see Table A4). ADHD symptoms in childhood and adolescence were recorded by the WURS-K (for results, see Table A5). Statistical analysis regarding the group differences in the severity of depressive or ADHD symptoms were conducted (Table A6 and Table A7). No correlations were performed to test for a possible influence of underlying comorbidities on impulsive behaviours; this was also discussed in the limitations of the study.

4. Discussion

The complexity of impulsivity underlines the meaningfulness of its detailed consideration. In our study, the inclusion of different research approaches using self-assessment and behavioural testing and the multidimensional approach in their evaluations proved revealing. Patients with OCD as well as patients with TS showed significant deficits in attention via self-assessment. Our data revealed a direct correlation: the stronger the obsessive-compulsive or tic symptoms, the less attentive the patients were. To be able to classify these results in the best possible way, it is interesting to observe that other dimensions of self-assessed impulsivity (motor and non-planning) remained without significant differences compared to HC. Different causes of this influence on attention are conceivable, and according to our results, an increased impulsivity, especially in patients with OCD, as the sole explanatory model is not obvious. Moreover, a lack of attention could not be objectified in patients either with OCD or in patients with TS.
Tic symptomatology correlated with the phenomenon of a lack of attention in the form of focus on current tasks (first-order factor attention), and as a result, with poorer second-order factor attentional impulsivity. A comparison with the current literature is difficult as the studies concerning the self-assessment of impulsivity in patients with TS by means of the BIS-11 or its evaluation are expandable. For example, the studies of Atkinson-Clement et al. showed a more impulsive behaviour in patients with TS compared to HC [33], while Delorme et al. found no significant differences [34], but none of the studies considered the individual dimensions beyond the total score of the questionnaire. Thus, a specific conclusion regarding a lack of attention in the context of impulsive behaviour cannot be drawn. Further studies did justice to the multidimensional approach and examined clinical pictures such as patients with OCD and comorbid tic disorders [30] or addictive disorders, which additionally included a group of patients with TS [35], however, the results were not tested for significant differences between patients with TS and HC, as this was not the focus of these studies. Including different test methods, the evidence for reduced sustained attention (the capacity to maintain attention over a period of time, to react to desired stimuli, and to ignore faulty ones [46]), in childhood patients with TS is predominant, especially in the case of comorbid ADHD [47,48,49,50]. In adulthood, however, the data are less clear: in some cases, reduced attention could be demonstrated [51,52], in other studies, however, not [53,54]. Overall, the field of research of attention deficits in patients with TS in adulthood is expandable and is handled differently by different authors in the evaluation of the results. Our study underlines the assumption that multidimensionality should be considered when evaluating the BIS-11. Our results do not indicate a cross-dimensionally heightened impulsivity; more studies in the future with larger cohorts would be necessary to investigate this hypothesis further.
The existing literature examining impulsive behaviour in patients with OCD using the BIS-11 is more extensive. Increased scores in the cross-dimensional total score could be shown, while individual dimensions were also considered separately. The dimension that was decisive for the significant differences was usually attentional impulsivity [55,56,57,58]. The dimensions motor and non-planning often remained without significant group differences [30,31,58,59]. Stein et al. even found lower motor impulsivity in OCD patients compared to HC in a large cohort [60]. In congruence with these results, we found increased impulsivity in patients with OCD compared to HC in the dimension of a lack of attention; remarkably, in all first- and second-order factors, there was a poorer ability to concentrate on current tasks as well as increased racing thoughts and thought jumps. Resulting from this, an overall heightened attentional impulsivity was found. No significant group differences in the dimension of motor and non-planning or the total scores could be shown (see Table 2).
The fact that other dimensions (motor and non-planning) often remain without significant results has already caused discussion in the past. Some authors argue that an elevated score in a questionnaire measuring impulsivity is directly linked to increased impulsive behaviour [57,58]. However, other authors have questioned this interpretation and state that increased scores in the total score need to be interpreted with caution if attentional impulsivity is the only dimension with a stronger expression. At this point, increased cross-dimensional impulsive behaviour in patients with OCD is still a matter of debate [32,61].
Given the above consideration, it is highly interesting to discuss hypothesis of the relation between OCD and lack of attention. Interestingly, our data showed a correlation where the more pronounced the obsessive-compulsive symptoms (controlling and obsessive thoughts), the more frequently patients with OCD reported a lack of attention (in form of first-order factor cognitive instability, e.g., racing thoughts and thought jumps). Similar to our findings, past studies have found a direct correlation between obsessive-compulsive symptoms and decreased attention [30,31,55]. As a result, a lack of attention has been discussed as a consequence of increased obsessive thoughts and not necessarily in the context of increased impulsivity [30,31]. Obsessive thoughts are often fearful and worrying, and patients try unsuccessfully to resist these intrusions. Therefore, patients with OCD in our study reported difficulties in resisting “unpleasant” and “repulsive” thoughts and in “controlling their thoughts” in the OCI-R questionnaire. Accordingly, it would be conceivable that the simultaneously observable lack of attention in the BIS-11 (e.g., in the scoring of the statements “I concentrate easily” or “I have extraneous thoughts when thinking”) arose due to the obsessive-compulsive symptomatology in the form of uncontrollable intrusions and not necessarily as an independent phenomenon in the context of increased impulsivity.
Summerfeldt et al. took this consideration one step further [30]. The authors argued that patients with OCD would not necessarily have reduced attentional impulsivity despite elevated scores on the BIS-11. It would be possible that the wording chosen to describe impulsive behaviour could be understood, at least in part, as describing obsessive thoughts. The subjects would thus confirm that while thinking “incidental thoughts come into their mind” or “thoughts race through their head”, referring perhaps, even exclusively, to their existing obsessive thoughts. Hence, whether patients refer to a disorder immanent phenomenon (intrusive thoughts) or to their process of thinking in general cannot be uncovered by the BIS-11.
In summary, the assessment of an increased total score in the BIS-11 must always be considered in relation to the dimensions. Impulsivity could still be a reason for the described lack of attention, but it is not obvious as the sole explanatory model based on our results and in the synopsis with the described previous literature. An alternative conceivable explanatory model according to our results as well as previous studies is shown in Figure 1.
In this context, the evaluation of objectifiable attention is exciting. As described, attention can present itself in different ways. The IMT/DMT, as a behavioural test, captures “sustained attention”: the capacity to maintain attention over a period of time, to react to desired stimuli, and to ignore faulty ones [46]. In the BIS-11, a lack of attention is captured in the context of impulsive behaviour (e.g., in the scoring of the statements “I don’t pay attention”, “I can concentrate easily”, or “I am a persistent thinker.”). Despite the different perspectives on the lack of attention of the two test options, overlaps can be identified. Whilst the BIS-11 patients with OCD showed poorer attention, the results of the IMT/DMT remained without significant group differences. One explanatory approach would support the hypothesis that the self-perceived decreased attention in the BIS-11 might have been obsessive thoughts misinterpreted. Despite the overlap of the definitions of attention in IMT/DMT and BIS-11, they are, of course, not equivalent, and the inference cannot be drawn without bias. For example, different parameters are used to capture sustained attention and impulsivity in the IMT/DMT [21]. Even if overlaps of sustained attention with the dimension of impulsivity of the BIS-11 are assumed, it remains an explanatory possibility that different results could also be due to the type of testing. A large study by Cyders et al. showed that although self-perceived cognitive impulsivity overlaps with impulsivity at the behavioural level, these two test methods appear to examine different constructs [62]. Congruent to this, in our study, subjective impulsivity examined by self-assessment questionnaires showed no correlation with impulsivity objectifiable by behavioural testing in patients with OCD (see Table A3). In fact, there are even indications that subjective approaches by self-assessment show a higher reliability than objective ones by behavioural testing [63]. The results of the IMT/DMT would therefore not exclude an actual lack of attention in patients with OCD.
Just as complex and multi-layered as the behavioural phenomena under consideration are the individuals studied with their respective diagnoses and clinical manifestations. Despite the small group of patients, the frequent occurrence and degree of expression of depressive symptoms [64,65,66,67] (see Table A4) as comorbid ADHD [68,69,70,71] (see Table A5) could be seen in our patient collective. Thus 80% of the patients with OCD and 46.2% of patients with Tourette’s syndrome suffered from depressive syndromes of different degrees, and in each case, showed significantly more pronounced depressive symptoms than HC (see Table A6 and Table A7). A total of 46.7% of patients with OCD and 53.8% of patients with TS could be diagnosed with ADHD in retrospect. Patients with TS showed significantly more pronounced ADHD symptoms in childhood and adolescence in the Kruskal–Wallis test and post-hoc analysis compared to HC (see Table A8 and Table A9).
Depressiveness [72,73,74] as well as ADHD [75,76,77] have shown potential influences of the observed behaviours in the past, and this circumstance should be considered in the assessment of our results as we did not examine the correlations between the underlying comorbid symptomatology and therefore its possible influence on the observed impulsive behaviour. Future studies should not only describe the occurrence of, but also its influence on impulsiveness. It would also be of great interest to also use further diagnostic groups that suffer from impulsivity.
Symptom severities of obsessive-compulsive symptomatology in patients with OCD and tic symptomatology in patients with TS can be found in Table A10 and Table A11. Results from screening HC for obsessive compulsive or tic symptoms can be found in Table A12 and Table A13.
Other limitations of the study that should be mentioned are the small patient collective and the heterogeneity in the severity of the different diseases and their treatment status.
In summary, our results suggest that multidimensionality should always be considered when examining impulsivity. Even when a study design does not allow for various types of testing, the dimensional evaluation of single tests like the BIS-11 can provide relevant information about impulsive behaviours regarding its different aspects. In our study, the question of heightened impulsivity in patients with OCD as well as TS would have been denied, without further consideration of the dimensional construct, missing to discuss the self-reported lack of attention. Therefore, our study underscores the necessity of a detailed interpretation of the various measuring tools for impulsive behaviour with its overlaps and subtle differences regarding their different parameters. Impulsivity represents an important building block in our understanding of behaviour beyond defined psychiatric diagnoses. Despite the information already acquired, there is often still no consensus regarding its exact dimensional classification [78,79,80,81]. Further studies with larger cohorts are therefore of great importance for a detailed understanding of this behavioural phenomenon.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, S.K. and J.K.; Methodology, S.K. and J.K.; Software, S.K. and J.K.; Validation, L.G.K., S.S. and S.K.; Formal analysis, L.G.K.; Investigation, L.G.K. and S.S.; Resources, J.K.; Data curation, L.G.K. and S.K.; Writing—original draft preparation, L.G.K. and S.K.; Writing—review and editing, S.S. and J.K.; Supervision, J.K.; Project administration, S.K.; Funding acquisition, S.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Marga and Walter Boll Foundation, grant number 210-02.1-15.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University Hospital Cologne (protocol code 16-120, 23 May 2016).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on reasonable request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to missing consent.

Acknowledgments

We thank all of the participants for their time and effort in our study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Testing for parametric distribution in the BIS-11 using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test with significance correction according to Lillefors. Abbreviations: df, degrees of freedom; p, significance.
Table A1. Testing for parametric distribution in the BIS-11 using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test with significance correction according to Lillefors. Abbreviations: df, degrees of freedom; p, significance.
DimensionStatisticdfp
First-order factors
Attention0.143540.008
Cognitive Instability0.124540.038
Motor0.119540.053
Perseverance0.16654<0.001
Self-Control0.122540.044
Cognitive Complexity0.107540.187
Second-order factors
Attentional0.076540.200
Motor0.129540.025
Non-planning0.091540.200
Total Score0.078540.200
Table A2. Testing for parametric distribution in the IMT/DMT using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test with significance correction according to Lillefors. Abbreviations: CE, commission errors; CR, correct responses; df, degrees of freedom; p, significance.
Table A2. Testing for parametric distribution in the IMT/DMT using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test with significance correction according to Lillefors. Abbreviations: CE, commission errors; CR, correct responses; df, degrees of freedom; p, significance.
ParametersStatisticdfp
Correct Responses
IMT0.101530.200
DMT0.166530.001
Commission Errors
IMT0.104530.200
DMT0.088530.200
CE/CR
IMT0.121530.049
DMT0.066530.200
Table A3. Testing for Spearman correlations between dimensions of the BIS-11 questionnaire and IMT/DMT in patients with OCD (n = 13). Abbreviations: Com., commission errors; Corr., correct responses; DMT, delayed memory task; IMT, immediate memory task; p, significance (2-sided); rs, Spearman correlation coefficient.
Table A3. Testing for Spearman correlations between dimensions of the BIS-11 questionnaire and IMT/DMT in patients with OCD (n = 13). Abbreviations: Com., commission errors; Corr., correct responses; DMT, delayed memory task; IMT, immediate memory task; p, significance (2-sided); rs, Spearman correlation coefficient.
BIS-11
Dimension
Spearman IMT
Corr.
DMT
Corr.
IMT
Com.
DMT
Com.
IMT
Com./Cor.
DMT
Com./Cor.
Attention
first-order factor
rs0.3090.1410.069−0.1820.039−0.157
p0.3040.6460.8230.5510.9000.607
Cognitive Instability
first-order factor
rs−0.041−0.1080.0700.0000.076−0.015
p0.8950.7260.8211.0000.8060.962
Motor
first-order factor
rs−0.2020.163−0.0280.0360.0640.080
p0.5090.5950.9290.9070.8370.795
Perseverance
first-order factor
rs0.1790.065−0.080−0.344−0.213−0.324
p0.5580.8320.7960.2500.4840.280
Self-Control
first-order factor
rs−0.2600.050−0.202−0.155−0.113−0.105
p0.3910.8720.5080.6130.7120.733
Cognitive Complexity
first-order factor
rs0.0170.394−0.305−0.508−0.293−0.531
p0.9570.1830.3120.0760.3310.062
Attentional
second-order factor
rs0.2410.0690.202−0.0220.1940.003
p0.4280.8220.5080.9430.5260.993
Motor
Second-order factor
rs−0.1990.180−0.058−0.119−0.006−0.061
p0.5150.5570.8510.6990.9860.844
Non-planning
second-order factor
rs−0.0990.226−0.253−0.399−0.226−0.369
p0.7470.4580.4030.1760.4580.214
Total Scorers−0.1810.110−0.038−0.192−0.016−0.137
p0.5530.7210.9010.5290.9570.655
Table A4. Scores in the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) in groups. Abbreviations: HC, healthy control subjects; IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; SD standard deviation; TS, Tourette syndrome.
Table A4. Scores in the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) in groups. Abbreviations: HC, healthy control subjects; IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; SD standard deviation; TS, Tourette syndrome.
GroupnMSDMdnIQRMin–Max
TS1310.317.353104.5–140–28
OCD1514.3813.9122618–332–59
HC273.153.38221–60–15
Table A5. Scores in the German short version of the Wender Utah Rating Scale (WURS-K) in groups. Abbreviations: HC, healthy control subjects; IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; SD standard deviation; TS, Tourette syndrome.
Table A5. Scores in the German short version of the Wender Utah Rating Scale (WURS-K) in groups. Abbreviations: HC, healthy control subjects; IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; SD standard deviation; TS, Tourette syndrome.
GroupnMSDMdnIQRMin–Max
TS1333.6911.81433.0026–4017–61
OCD1528.1313.91228.0015–4010–51
HC2723.2610.20820.0015–297–47
Table A6. Testing for significant group differences in the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) using the Kruskal–Wallis test. Abbreviations: BDI-II, Beck Depressions Inventory-II; df, degree of freedom; p, asymptotic significance; χ2, chi-square.
Table A6. Testing for significant group differences in the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) using the Kruskal–Wallis test. Abbreviations: BDI-II, Beck Depressions Inventory-II; df, degree of freedom; p, asymptotic significance; χ2, chi-square.
Kruskal–WallisBDI-II
χ228.431
df 2
p<0.001
Table A7. Testing for significant group differences using the Bonferroni post-hoc test in the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). Abbreviations: Adj. Sig., adjusted significance; HC, healthy control subjects; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; p, asymptotic significance; Sig., significance; Std., standard; TS, Tourette syndrome.
Table A7. Testing for significant group differences using the Bonferroni post-hoc test in the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). Abbreviations: Adj. Sig., adjusted significance; HC, healthy control subjects; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; p, asymptotic significance; Sig., significance; Std., standard; TS, Tourette syndrome.
Group 1–Group 2Test StatisticStd. ErrorStd. Test StatisticSig.Adj. Sig.
HC–TS 14.7565.3932.7360.0060.019
HC–OCD 26.9335.1455.235<0.001<0.001
TS–OCD −12.1776.054−2.0110.0440.133
Table A8. Testing for significant group differences in the Wender Utah Rating Scale-Kurzform (WURS-K) using the Kruskal–Wallis test. Abbreviations: WURS-K, Wender Utah Rating Scale-Kurzform; df, degree of freedom; p, asymptotic significance; χ2, chi-square.
Table A8. Testing for significant group differences in the Wender Utah Rating Scale-Kurzform (WURS-K) using the Kruskal–Wallis test. Abbreviations: WURS-K, Wender Utah Rating Scale-Kurzform; df, degree of freedom; p, asymptotic significance; χ2, chi-square.
Kruskal–WallisWURS-K
χ26.133
df 2
p0.047
Table A9. Testing for significant group differences using the Bonferroni post-hoc test in the Wender Utah Rating Scale-Kurzform (WURS-K). Abbreviations: Adj. Sig., adjusted significance; HC, healthy control subjects; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; p, asymptotic significance; Sig., significance; Std., standard; TS, Tourette syndrome.
Table A9. Testing for significant group differences using the Bonferroni post-hoc test in the Wender Utah Rating Scale-Kurzform (WURS-K). Abbreviations: Adj. Sig., adjusted significance; HC, healthy control subjects; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; p, asymptotic significance; Sig., significance; Std., standard; TS, Tourette syndrome.
Group 1–Group 2Test StatisticStd. ErrorStd. Test StatisticSig.Adj. Sig.
HC–TS 13.2815.4012.4590.0140.042
HC–OCD 5.7375.1521.1140.2650.796
OCD–TS 7.5446.0621.2440.2130.640
Table A10. Scores in the Adult Tic Questionnaire (ATQ) of patients with TS (n = 12). Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; SD, standard deviation.
Table A10. Scores in the Adult Tic Questionnaire (ATQ) of patients with TS (n = 12). Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; SD, standard deviation.
DimensionMSDMdnIQRMin–Max
Motor36.0017.6331.0026.75–47.009–76
Phonic20.5818.4214.5010–21.256–66
Total56.5834.0345.5039–68.515–142
Table A11. Scores in the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R) of patients with OCD (n = 15). Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; SD, standard deviation.
Table A11. Scores in the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R) of patients with OCD (n = 15). Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; SD, standard deviation.
DimensionMSDMdnIQRMin–Max
Washing4.534.452.000–90–12
Controlling7.273.566.006–121–12
Ordering5.474.636.000–90–12
Obsessive Thoughts8.803.3211.005–123–12
Hoarding3.202.463.000–60–7
Neutralising4.004.003.000–70–11
Table A12. Scores in the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R) of healthy control subjects (n = 27). Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; SD, standard deviation.
Table A12. Scores in the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R) of healthy control subjects (n = 27). Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, minimum; SD, standard deviation.
DimensionMSDMdnIQRMin–Max
Washing0.931.4290.000–10–5
Controlling1.591.8031.000–21–6
Ordering2.632.4362.001–40–9
Obsessive Thoughts0.851.4600.000–10–7
Hoarding1.671.3301.001–30–4
Neutralising0.410.8880.000–10–4
Table A13. Scores in the Adult Tic Questionnaire (ATQ) of healthy control subjects (n = 28). Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, Minimum; SD, standard deviation.
Table A13. Scores in the Adult Tic Questionnaire (ATQ) of healthy control subjects (n = 28). Abbreviations: IQR, interquartile range; M, mean; Max, maximum; Mdn, median; Min, Minimum; SD, standard deviation.
DimensionMSDMdnIQRMin–Max
Motor0.180.9450.000–00–5
Phonic0.000.000.000–00–0
Total0.180.9450.000–00–5

References

  1. Berlin, G.S.; Hollander, E. Compulsivity, impulsivity, and the DSM-5 process. CNS Spectr. 2014, 19, 62–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Evenden, J.L. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology 1999, 146, 348–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Bari, A.; Robbins, T.W. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol. 2013, 108, 44–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Dalley, J.W.; Robbins, T.W. Fractionating impulsivity: Neuropsychiatric implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 158–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Robbins, T.W.; Gillan, C.M.; Smith, D.G.; de Wit, S.; Ersche, K.D. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: Towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2012, 16, 81–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Blair, R.J. The Neurobiology of Impulsive Aggression. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2016, 26, 4–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Luk, J.W.; Trim, R.S.; Karyadi, K.A.; Curry, I.; Hopfer, C.J.; Hewitt, J.K.; Stallings, M.C.; Brown, S.A.; Wall, T.L. Unique and interactive effects of impulsivity facets on reckless driving and driving under the influence in a high-risk young adult sample. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2017, 114, 42–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Liu, S.J.; Lan, Y.; Wu, L.; Yan, W.S. Profiles of Impulsivity in Problematic Internet Users and Cigarette Smokers. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Hamza, C.A.; Willoughby, T.; Heffer, T. Impulsivity and nonsuicidal self-injury: A review and meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2015, 38, 13–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chamberlain, S.R.; Grant, J.E. Relationship between quality of life in young adults and impulsivity/compulsivity. Psychiatry Res. 2019, 271, 253–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Victor, S.E.; Johnson, S.L.; Gotlib, I.H. Quality of life and impulsivity in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord. 2011, 13, 303–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Patton, J.H.; Stanford, M.S.; Barratt, E.S. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 1995, 51, 768–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Dalley, J.W.; Everitt, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 2011, 69, 680–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Dickman, S.J. Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: Personality and cognitive correlates. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 58, 95–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Fenneman, J.; Fraukenhuis, W.E. Is impulsive behavior adaptive in harsh and unpredictable environments? A formal model. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2020, 41, 261–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Fineberg, N.A.; Chamberlain, S.R.; Goudriaan, A.E.; Stein, D.J.; Vanderschuren, L.J.; Gillan, C.M.; Shekar, S.; Gorwood, P.A.; Voon, V.; Morein-Zamir, S.; et al. New developments in human neurocognition: Clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity. CNS Spectr. 2014, 19, 69–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Gullo, M.J.; Loxton, N.J.; Dawe, S. Impulsivity: Four ways five factors are not basic to addiction. Addict. Behav. 2014, 39, 1547–1556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Teese, R.; Willie, C.; Jago, A.; Gill, P.R. An Investigation of Alternative Factor Models of Impulsivity using the UPPS-P. J. Pers. Assess. 2021, 103, 324–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Lynam, D.R.; Smith, G.T.; Cyders, M.A.; Fischer, S.; Whiteside, S.A. The UPPS-P: A Multidimensional Measure of Risk for Impulsive Behavior. 2007; unpublished technical report. [Google Scholar]
  20. Carver, C.S.; White, T.L. Behavioural Inhibition, behavioural acitvation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 67, 319–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Dougherty, D.M.; Marsh, D.M.; Mathias, C.W. Immediate and delayed memory tasks: A computerized behavioral measure of memory, attention, and impulsivity. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 2002, 34, 391–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Berlin, H.A. Antiepileptic Drugs for the Treatment of Impulsivity. Curr. Psychiatry Rev. 2008, 4, 114–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Santana, R.P.; Kerr-Gaffney, J.; Ancane, A.; Young, A.H. Impulsivity in Bipolar Disorder: State or Trait? Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Watkins, H.B.; Meyer, T.D. Is there an empirical link between impulsivity and suicidality in bipolar disorders? A review of the current literature and the potential psychological implications of the relationship. Bipolar Disord. 2015, 15, 542–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Charney, D.A.; Zikos, E.; Gill, K.J. Early recovery from alcohol dependence: Factors that promote or impede abstinence. JSAT J. Subst. Abus. Treat. 2010, 38, 42–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Moeller, F.G.; Dougherty, D.M.; Barratt, E.S.; Schmitz, J.M.; Swann, A.C.; Grabowski, J. The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment. J. Subst. Abus. Treat. 2001, 21, 193–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Verdejo-García, A.; Lawrence, A.J.; Clark, L. Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2008, 32, 777–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Mungo, A.; Hein, M.; Hubain, P.; Loas, G.; Fontaine, P. Impulsivity and its Therapeutic Management in Borderline Personality Disorder: A Systematic Review. Psychiatr. Q. 2020, 91, 1333–1362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ocasio, W. Attention to Attention. Organ. Sci. 2010, 22, 1121–1367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Summerfeldt, L.J.; Hood, K.; Antony, M.M.; Richter, M.A.; Swinson, R.P. Impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Comparisons with other anxiety disorders and within tic-related subgroups. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2004, 36, 539–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Boisseau, C.L.; Thompson-Brenner, H.; Caldwell-Harris, C.; Pratt, E.; Farchione, T.; Barlow, D.H. Behavioral and cognitive impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder and eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2012, 200, 1062–1066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Abramovitch, A.; McKay, D. Behavioral Impulsivity in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 2016, 5, 395–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Atkinson-Clement, C.; Porte, C.A.; de Liege, A.; Klein, Y.; Delorme, C.; Beranger, B.; Valabregue, R.; Gallea, C.; Robbins, T.W.; Hartmann, A.; et al. Impulsive prepotent actions and tics in Tourette disorder underpinned by a common neural network. Mol. Psychiatry 2020, 26, 3548–3557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Delorme, C.; Salvador, A.; Valabregue, R.; Roze, E.; Palminteri, S.; Vidailhet, M.; de Wit, S.; Robbins, T.; Hartmann, A.; Worbe, Y. Enhanced habit formation in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Brain 2016, 139, 605–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Kräplin, A.; Bühringer, G.; Oosterlaan, J.; van den Brink, W.; Goschke, T.; Goudriaan, A. Dimensions and disorder specificity of impulsivity in pathological gambling. Addict. Behav. 2014, 39, 1646–1651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Abramovitch, A.; Abramovitch, J.S.; Riemann, B.C.; McKay, D. Severity benchmarks and contemporary clinical norms for the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R). JOCRD J. Obs.-Compuls. Relat. Disord. 2020, 27, 100557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Foa, E.B.; Huppert, J.D.; Leiberg, S.; Langner, R.; Kichic, R.; Hajcak, G.; Salkovskis, P.M. The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: Development and validation of a short version. Psychol. Assess. 2002, 14, 485–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Mathias, C.W.; Marsh, D.M.; Dougherty, D.M. Reliability estimates for the immediate and delayed memory tasks. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2002, 95, 559–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Dougherty, D.M.; Moeller, F.G.; Steinberg, J.L.; Marsh, D.M.; Hines, S.E.; Bjork, J.M. Alcohol increases commission error rates for a continuous performance test. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 1999, 23, 1342–1351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Dougherty, D.M.; Marsh, D.M.; Moeller, F.G.; Chokshi, R.V.; Rosen, V.C. Effects of moderate and high doses of alcohol on attention, impulsivity, discriminability, and response bias in immediate and delayed memory task performance. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2000, 24, 1702–1711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gonner, S.; Leonhart, R.; Ecker, W. The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory-Revised (OCI-R): Validation of the German version in a sample of patients with OCD, anxiety disorders, and depressive disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 2008, 22, 734–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Abramovitch, A.; Reese, H.; Woods, D.W.; Peterson, A.; Deckersbach, T.; Piacentini, J.; Scahill, L.; Wilhelm, S. Psychometric Properties of a Self-Report Instrument for the Assessment of Tic Severity in Adults With Tic Disorders. Behav. Ther. 2015, 46, 786–796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Beck, A.T.; Steer, R.A.; Brown, G.K. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  44. Retz-Jungingner, P.; Retz, W.; Blocher, D.; Wieijers, H.-G.; Trott, G.-E.; Wender, P.H.; Rössler, M. Wender Utah Rating Scale (WURS-k); Die deutsche Kurzform zur retrospektiven Erfassung des hyperkinetischen Syndroms bei Erwachsenen. Der Nervenarzt 2002, 73, 830–838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; L. Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  46. Tsal, Y.; Shalev, L.; Mevorach, C. The Diversity of Attention Deficits in ADHD. J. Learn. Disab. 2005, 38, 142–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Sherman, E.M.; Shepard, L.; Joschko, M.; Freeman, R.D. Sustained attention and impulsivity in children with Tourette syndrome: Comorbidity and confounds. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1998, 20, 644–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Sukhodolsky, D.G.; Landeros-Weisenberger, A.; Scahill, L.; Leckman, J.F.; Schultz, R.T. Neuropsychological functioning in children with Tourette syndrome with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2010, 49, 1155–1164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Lin, Y.; Lai, M.-C.; Gau, S. Youths with ADHD with and without tic disorders: Comorbid psychopathology, executive function and social adjustment. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 951–963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Rasmussen, C.; Soleimani, M.; Carroll, A.; Holdlevskyy, O. Neuropsychological Functioning in Children with Tourette Syndrome (TS). J. Can. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2009, 18, 307–315. [Google Scholar]
  51. Channon, S.; Flynn, D.; Robertson, M.M. Attentional deficits in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 1992, 5, 170–177. [Google Scholar]
  52. Georgiou, N.; Bradshaw, J.L.; Phillips, J.G. Directed attention in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Behav. Neurol. 1998, 11, 85–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Matsuda, N.; Kono, T.; Nonaka, M.; Shishikura, K.; Konno, C.; Kuwabara, H.; Shimada, T.; Kano, Y. Impact of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in Tourette’s syndrome on neuropsychological performance. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2012, 66, 195–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Müller, S.V.; Johnannes, S.; Wieringa, B.; Weber, A.; Müller-Vahl, K.; Matzke, M.; Kolbe, H.; Dengler, R.; Münte, T.F. Disturbed monitoring and response inhibition in patients with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome and co-morbid obsessive compulsive disorder. Behav. Neurol. 2003, 14, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Onur, O.S.; Tabo, A.; Aydin, E.; Tuna, O.; Maner, A.F.; Yildrim, E.A.; Carpar, E. Relationship between impulsivity and obsession types in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 2016, 20, 218–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Shan, H.D.; Liu, Y.F.; Zhao, Q.; Wang, Y.; Wang, Y.M.; Cheung, E.F.; Chan, R.C.; Wang, Z. Distinct clinical manifestations of obsessive-compulsive disorder are associated with cortical thickness alteration. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2021, 56, 186–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Grassi, G.; Pallanti, S.; Righi, L.; Figee, M.; Mantione, M.; Denys, D.; Piccagliani, D.; Rossi, A.; Stratta, P. Think twice: Impulsivity and decision making in obsessive–compulsive disorder. J. Behav. Addict. 2015, 4, 263–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Benatti, B.; Dell’Osso, B.; Arici, C.; Hollander, E.; Altamura, A.C. Characterizing impulsivity profile in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Int. J. Psychiatry Clin. Pract. 2014, 18, 156–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Ettelt, S.; Ruhrmann, S.; Barnow, S.; Buthz, F.; Hochrein, A.; Meyer, K.; Kraft, S.; Reck, C.; Pukrop, R.; Klosterkoetter, J.; et al. Impulsiveness in obsessive–compulsive disorder: Results from a family study. Acta. Psychiatr. Scand. 2007, 115, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Stein, D.J.; Hollander, E.; Simeon, D.; Cohen, L. Impulsivity scores in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1994, 182, 240–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Frydman, I.; Mattos, P.; de Oliveira-Souza, R.; Yucel, M.; Chamberlain, S.R.; Moll, J.; Fontenelle, L.F. Self-reported and neurocognitive impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 2020, 97, 152155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Cyders, M.A.; Coskunpinar, A. Measurement of constructs using self-report and behavioral lab tasks: Is there overlap in nomothetic span and construct representation for impulsivity? Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2011, 31, 965–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Enkavi, A.Z.; Eisenberg, I.W.; Bissett, P.G.; Mazza, G.L.; MacKinnon, D.P.; Marsch, L.A.; Poldrack, R.A. Large-scale analysis of test-retest reliabilities of self-regulation measures. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 5472–5477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Pinto, A.; Mancebo, M.C.; Eisen, J.L.; Pagano, M.E.; Rasmussen, S.A. The Brown Longitudinal Obsessive Compulsive Study: Clinical features and symptoms of the sample at intake. J. Clin. Psychiatry 2006, 67, 703–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Brakoulias, V.; Starcevic, V.; Belloch, A.; Brown, C.; Ferrao, Y.A.; Fontenelle, L.F.; Lochner, C.; Marazziti, D.; Matsunaga, H.; Miguel, E.C.; et al. Comorbidity, age of onset and suicidality in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): An international collaboration. Compr. Psychiatry 2017, 76, 79–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Rizzo, R.; Gulisano, M.; Martino, D.; Robertson, M.M. Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome, Depression, Depressive Illness, and Correlates in a Child and Adolescent Population. J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 2017, 27, 243–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Piedad, J.C.; Cavanna, A.E. Depression in Tourette syndrome: A controlled and comparison study. J. Neurol. Sci. 2016, 364, 128–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Hirschtritt, M.E.; Lee, P.C.; Pauls, D.L.; Dion, Y.; Grados, M.A.; Illmann, C.; King, R.A.; Sandor, P.; McMahon, W.M.; Lyon, G.J.; et al. Lifetime prevalence, age of risk, and genetic relationships of comorbid psychiatric disorders in Tourette syndrome. JAMA Psychiatry 2015, 72, 325–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. Robertson, M.M. Corrections. A personal 35 year perspective on Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: Prevalence, phenomenology, comorbidities, and coexistent psychopathologies. Lancet Psychiatry 2015, 2, 291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Sheppard, B.; Chavira, D.; Azzam, A.; Grados, M.A.; Umana, P.; Garrido, H.; Mathews, C.A. ADHD prevalence and association with hoarding behaviors in childhood-onset OCD. Depress. Anxiety 2010, 27, 667–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Masi, G.; Millepiedi, S.; Mucci, M.; Bertini, N.; Pfanner, C.; Arcangeli, F. Comorbidity of obsessive-compulsive disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in referred children and adolescents. Compr. Psychiatry 2006, 47, 42–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Swann, A.C.; Steinberg, J.L.; Lijffijt, M.; Moeller, F.G. Impulsivity: Differential relationship to depression and mania in bipolar disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2008, 106, 241–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. Saddichha, S.; Schuetz, C. Impulsivity in remitted depression: A meta-analytical review. Asian J. Psychiatr. 2014, 9, 13–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Khemakhem, K.; Boudabous, J.; Cherif, L.; Ayadi, H.; Walha, A.; Moalla, Y.; Hadjkacem, I.; Ghribi, F. Impulsivity in adolescents with major depressive disorder: A comparative tunisian study. Asian J. Psychiatr. 2017, 28, 183–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Morand-Beaulieu, S.; Leclerc, J.B.; Valois, P.; Lavoie, M.E.; O’Connor, K.P.; Gauthier, B. A Review of the Neuropsychological Dimensions of Tourette Syndrome. Brain Sci. 2017, 7, 106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Tan, O.; Metin, B.; Metin, S. Obsessive-compulsive adults with and without childhood ADHD symptoms. Atten. Deficit Hyperact. Disord. 2016, 8, 131–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Mersin Kilic, S.; Dondu, A.; Memis, C.O.; Ozdemiroglu, F.; Sevincok, L. The Clinical Characteristics of ADHD and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder Comorbidity. J. Atten. Disord. 2020, 24, 1757–1763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  78. Kahn, J.P.; Cohen, R.F.; Etain, B.; Aubin, V.; Bellivier, F.; Belzeaux, R.; Bougerol, T.; Courtet, P.; Dubertret, C.; Gard, S.; et al. Reconsideration of the factorial structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11): Assessment of impulsivity in a large population of euthymic bipolar patients. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 253, 203–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Reise, S.P.; Moore, T.M.; Sabb, F.W.; Brown, A.K.; London, E.D. The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11: Reassessment of its Structure in a Community Sample. Psychol. Assess. 2013, 25, 631–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Ros, L.; Zabala, C.; Romero-Ayuso, D.; Jimeno, V.; Ricarte, J.J. The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 in Community and Incarcerated Samples: Motor and Nonplanning Impulsivity Effects on Criminal Recidivism. Crim. Justice Behav. 2020, 47, 996–1013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Lau, J.H.; Jeyagurunathan, A.; Shafie, S. The factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) and correlates of impulsivity among outpatients with schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders in Singapore. BMC Psychiatry 2022, 22, 226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceivable explanatory model for a self-reported lack of attention in the BIS-11 questionnaire in patients with OCD.
Figure 1. Conceivable explanatory model for a self-reported lack of attention in the BIS-11 questionnaire in patients with OCD.
Jcm 12 02277 g001
Table 1. Factor structure of the BIS-11 questionnaire according to Patton et al. [12].
Table 1. Factor structure of the BIS-11 questionnaire according to Patton et al. [12].
Second-Order FactorFirst-Order FactorDescription
Attentional ImpulsivityAttentionFocusing on current task
Cognitive InstabilityIntruding thoughts
Motor ImpulsivityMotorActing quickly
PerseveranceStable lifestyle
Non-planning ImpulsivitySelf-ControlEnjoys mental challenges
Cognitive ComplexityPlans and thinks deliberatively
Table 2. Testing for significant group differences in the second-order factors attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsivity and the total score of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 using the Kruskal–Wallis Test. Abbreviations: df, degree of freedom; p, asymptotic significance; χ2, chi-square.
Table 2. Testing for significant group differences in the second-order factors attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsivity and the total score of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 using the Kruskal–Wallis Test. Abbreviations: df, degree of freedom; p, asymptotic significance; χ2, chi-square.
Kruskal–WallisAttentionalMotorNon-PlanningTotal
χ224.9105.791.0544.825
df 2222
p<0.0010.0560.5900.090
Table 3. Th post-hoc Bonferroni test of the first-order factors attention and cognitive insatiability (belonging to the second-order factor attentional impulsivity) of the BIS-11 questionnaire. Abbreviations: Adj., Adjusted; HC, healthy control subjects; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; Sig., Significance; Std., Standard; TS, Tourette syndrome.
Table 3. Th post-hoc Bonferroni test of the first-order factors attention and cognitive insatiability (belonging to the second-order factor attentional impulsivity) of the BIS-11 questionnaire. Abbreviations: Adj., Adjusted; HC, healthy control subjects; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; Sig., Significance; Std., Standard; TS, Tourette syndrome.
Group 1–Group 2BIS-11 First-Order FactorTest StatisticStd. ErrorStd. Test StatisticSig.Adj. Sig.
HC–TS Attention20.2835.2943.832<0.001<0.001
Cognitive Instability16.8195.2223.2210.0010.004
HC–OCD Attention22.1655.1644.292<0.001<0.001
Cognitive Instability19.8825.0953.902<0.001<0.001
TS–OCD Attention−1.8826.040−0.3120.7551.000
Cognitive Instability−3.0635.959−0.5140.6071.000
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Koenn, L.G.; Kohl, S.; Schleyken, S.; Kuhn, J. Impulsivity and Attention in Obsessive Compulsive and Tic Disorders: Mismatch in Self-Report and Behavioural Data. J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12, 2277. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jcm12062277

AMA Style

Koenn LG, Kohl S, Schleyken S, Kuhn J. Impulsivity and Attention in Obsessive Compulsive and Tic Disorders: Mismatch in Self-Report and Behavioural Data. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2023; 12(6):2277. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jcm12062277

Chicago/Turabian Style

Koenn, Leonard Guenter, Sina Kohl, Sophia Schleyken, and Jens Kuhn. 2023. "Impulsivity and Attention in Obsessive Compulsive and Tic Disorders: Mismatch in Self-Report and Behavioural Data" Journal of Clinical Medicine 12, no. 6: 2277. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/jcm12062277

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop