Next Article in Journal
Impacts of Socio-Psychological Factors on Actual Adoption of Sustainable Land Management Practices in Dryland and Water Stressed Areas
Next Article in Special Issue
Sexism and Aggression in Adolescence—How Do They Relate to Perceived Academic Achievement?
Previous Article in Journal
Towards a Common European Space for Asylum
Previous Article in Special Issue
Leisure-Time Physical Activity in Relation to the Stages of Changes and Achievement Goals in Adolescents: Comparative Study of Students in Spain, Costa Rica, and Mexico
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Family Socialisation Styles and Hostility in the Adolescent Population

by
María Del Milagro Aymerich
1,*,
Gonzalo Musitu
2 and
Francisco Palmero
1,*
1
Department of Basic Psychology, Clinical and Psychobiology, Faculty of Health Sciences, Universitat Jaume I, 12071 Castellón de la Plana, Castellón, Spain
2
Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Universidad Pablo de Olavide, 41013 Seville, Spain
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2018, 10(9), 2962; https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/su10092962
Submission received: 11 July 2018 / Revised: 8 August 2018 / Accepted: 16 August 2018 / Published: 21 August 2018
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sustainability of Children’s Psychological Health)

Abstract

:
The affective involvement of parents in the socialization of their children is fundamental for the proper psychological and emotional adjustment of adolescents, although we know that it is difficult to study. In this research, the relationship between parenting style and hostility was analysed in Spanish adolescents. Five-hundred and thirty-six adolescents participated in this study (53.7% males and 46.3% females), between the ages of 12 and 18 (M = 15.76, SD = 1.43), enrolled in 4 compulsory Secondary Education centers of the Castellon province. Family socialization was evaluated through the parental styles: authoritative, indulgent, authoritarian and negligent, and hostility, through the Cook–Medley Hostility Scale Criteria assessment. A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was carried out with multiple regression analysis. The results showed that hostility and compound hostility are higher in adolescents from authoritarian and authoritative families than in adolescents from indulgent families. It was also observed that the Mother’s Strictness/Imposition had the greatest effect on hostility, as well as a low acceptance/involvement of the father. Regarding parental styles, it was observed that mother’s deprivation, physical coercion, and mother’s verbal coercion were the greatest predictors of hostility. No differences were observed depending on the gender.

1. Introduction

Current studies emphasize that family environment and parental styles are the main tools available to young people to achieve a correct social adaptation. Regarding the social climate of the family, effective communication allows an appropriate adjustment of adolescents [1,2]. From the Pinquart study [3], we know that the authoritarian, permissive, and neglectful parenting styles are associated with higher levels of externalizing problems, and that victimization and deviant behavior are influenced by negative control [4].
Parenting style can be defined as “… a constellation of attitudes towards the child that, considered jointly, create an emotional climate in which the parents’ behaviours are expressed” [5] (p. 116). Parental support and control are the two factors that explain most of the variability in the disciplinary behaviour of parents. Two dimensions have traditionally been considered in parental socialization: responsiveness and demand, also called acceptance/implication and severity/imposition [6,7,8] to explain the parental styles. This classic two-dimensional model is one that best explains the parental actions of socialization, despite the different names that different authors use. Thus, acceptance terms [9], attachment [10], love [11,12,13] have been used for the acceptance/implication axis, and for severity/imposition, those of domination, hostility, rigidity, control, or restriction are used [10,11,12,13]. The acceptance/implication dimension refers to the degree to which parents show affection to their children, giving them support, and communicating with them through reasoning. The dimension of severity/imposition refers to the degree to which parents use firmness to impose their authority, demand maturity, and place clear limits on the actions of their children [14,15]. A review of the scientific literature [16] showed that importance is given to combining two dimensions, acceptance/involvement and strictness/imposition (support and control, respectively), to explain the way parents bring up their children. The acceptance/involvement dimension refers to the degree of affective involvement of the parents in the socialising process, that is, endearment, support, and reasoned communication in response to their children’s inappropriate behaviour. The strictness/imposition dimension refers to the degree of imposition and strictness employed to establish the limits of children’s behaviour and thus impose parental authority. Combining the two dimensions results in the four-component parental socialisation model, which offers four orthogonal typologies of parenting style to represent all the theoretical features of the model. These four styles are: authoritative (high acceptance and involvement/high strictness and imposition); authoritarian (low acceptance and involvement/high strictness and imposition); indulgent (high acceptance and involvement/low strictness and imposition); and neglectful (low acceptance and involvement/low strictness and imposition).
In general terms, research on the different outcomes of these disciplinary styles in adolescents shows that the authoritative style (in the English-speaking context) and the indulgent style (Spanish population) are related to the following correlates: adjustment [17,18], psychosocial maturity [19], self-esteem [20], academic success [21], altruistic behaviour [22], physical activity [23], and ecological behaviour [24]. Although there have been some investigations in Spain: Murcia and Valencia [1] Valencia [25], Sevilla [26], we do not know of any in Castellón.
Nevertheless, several studies suggest the adequacy of the authoritarian style (low implication and high imposition) in contexts other than the English-speaking one [27], in ethnic minority groups [28] and even in families with a low sociocultural level [29]. On the other hand, in non-English-speaking cultures, the indulgent style fosters less sexist attitudes in children [30], a better use of learning strategies and higher academic achievement [31], and fewer problems in terms of behaviour and substance abuse [32]. Recently, evidence has also been found of a predominance of the indulgent parenting style among children who are not involved in bullying, of the neglectful parenting style in the bullies, and the authoritarian or authoritative parenting style among children who are victims [1], as well as the association between higher frequency and pathological use of the Internet and low family communication [33]. An instrument based on the theoretical model of socialisation has been used [7] which allows both a general analysis of the relations between the two main dimensions with the criteria evaluated and a more specific analysis of the parenting styles (authoritative, authoritarian, neglectful, and indulgent). Other studies, however, only analyse the relations between parenting practices in isolation, different adjustment criteria [34], or use instruments that give rise to typologies that are not in agreement with the theoretical model of socialisation described in this article [35].
In the research carried out to date, the relations between parenting styles and children’s psychosocial adjustment have been analysed [16,36,37], but very few studies analyse hostility as an adjustment variable in adolescence. For this reason, this study aims to analyse the relationships between parental socialisation styles and hostility in adolescents who have attended school. Hostility is considered as a mechanism that can trigger difficulties in psychological and social adjustment [38]. A review of the experimental research conducted to date has highlighted the importance of hostility as a risk factor for the development of difficulties to engage in interrelationships and even for cardiovascular diseases [39,40]. Yet, very few studies have been conducted on hostility in the teenage population.
The term hostility can be conceptualised as a cynical attitude towards human nature in general, which can lead to resentment and violence in certain situations, although it is usually expressed in very subtle ways so as to not violate social norms. Hostility is made up of a cognitive core of negative beliefs and attitudes that are destructive towards others, such as hate, malice, and resentment, which is considered as its key component. According to the AHA syndrome theory, the constructs of Anger, Hostility, and Aggression constitute a continuum. Thus, a certain event can give rise to an emotion (anger), which is influenced by a negative attitude towards others (hostility) and results in violent behaviour or aggression.
Nevertheless, Barefoot [41] had already suggested that, given that hostility often (but not always) involves affective, behavioural, and cognitive aspects, the three of them should be articulated and evaluated separately.
The affective component of hostility includes a variety of emotions such as anger, resentment, and annoyance. The cognitive component is made up of negative beliefs about human nature that include cynicism (widespread mistrust of others), hostile attributions (malice, antagonism, and threat), negative expectations (mistrust or suspicion), and a negative attitude (enmity, negativism and resentment). Lastly, the behavioural component includes aggressiveness, antagonism, negativism, and insults.
All these characteristics suggest that hostile individuals have greater difficulty in their interpersonal relationships, a higher number of conflicts and less social support, which, generally speaking, results in higher psychosocial vulnerability. In this regard, the absence of social support and excessive hostility seem to increase the risk of heart disease and deaths related to heart disorders. Other studies show that high scores of cynicism maintain and increase cardiovascular and neuroendocrine reactivity towards stressors. This situation also implies a greater predisposition to elicit and exacerbate interpersonal conflict in their lives, which reduces social support and thus maintains their hostile beliefs and behaviour towards others. Lastly, their permanent state of vigilance with respect to their surroundings means that the physiological changes associated to that state can become chronic [42].
It is within this framework of research on hostility and parenting styles that we propose our general aim of analysing the relationships between parental socialisation styles and hostility in teenagers. Previous studies conducted on the family have taken into account the educational dimensions of expression of affection and control, and their relationship with children’s adjustment. The indulgent parenting style is the one that is most closely related to psychosocial adjustment in adolescents [25]. Two hypotheses are proposed: (1) the indulgent parenting style is related to lower scores on hostility, and (2) the authoritarian parenting style is related to higher scores on hostility. In numerous studies it has been found that the most negative style, due to its effects on children, is the negligent one, followed by the authoritarian one. The most functional have been, in this order, the indulgent and the authoritative [32,37,43,44,45]. This research has not formulated this hypothesis related to authoritative or negligent styles because it was not among the objectives of this research to analyse these effects.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

The sample in this study consisted of 536 participants, 53.7% males and 46.3% females (Mean = 15.14 years old, and SD = of 1.95). The age range was 12–18. In Table 1, the number of subjects by age is shown and in Table 2, the number of subjects per age group.
The study was conducted in four public secondary schools in the province of Castellón, Spain. To this end, permission was obtained from the Headteacher and the Regional Secretary of Education. This research was authorized by the ethics committee of the University of Castellón, dated 30 March 2015. All students took part in the study on a voluntary basis and all parents signed informed consent. The questionnaires were answered individually and anonymously in a series of tutorial sessions each lasting around 50 min, under the supervision of two specially trained professionals. The schedule for the groups was designed according to terms throughout the academic year 2015–2016, depending on the timetable of each tutorial, by levels and educational stages, that is, ESO (compulsory secondary education), high school, and middle- and upper-level Vocational Training.

2.2. Instruments

Cook–Medley Hostility Scale [46]. Ho was validated for the Spanish population [47], and Cronbach’s alpha value for this scale was 0.80, This scale consists of 50 items taken from the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), which must be answered with a True-False response, grouped in six subscales: cynicism, hostile feelings, aggressive answers, hostile attribution, social avoidance, and others. The following are examples of the questions that measure hostile responses: I have often been given orders by someone who didn’t know as much as me. People often disappoint me. I think I’ve often been punished without reason, etc. The cynicism, hostile feelings, and aggressive answers scales make up the Composite Hostility (CHOST) measure or the more cognitive dimension of the inventory [48], which alone or in combination with defensiveness (that is to say, the tendency to suppress socially unacceptable aspects of oneself), is considered as the most important criterion for predicting cardiovascular reactivity.
The internal consistency for the hostility questionnaire was calculated by means of Cronbach’s α, which yielded a value of 0.791 for the full scale; 0.730 for the Compound Hostility (CHOST) scale (Subscales: cynicism, hostile feelings, aggressive answers); and 0.696 for the cynicism scale.
Parental Socialisation Scale (ESPA29) [49]. This scale is based on the bi-dimensional theoretical model of parental socialization [7]. ESPA29 was validated for the Spanish population [50], and Cronbach’s alpha value for this scale was 0.95 and 0.93 for this study. The reliabilities of the dimensions ranged between 0.90 and 0.93 for both parents. It evaluates the parents’ socialisation styles in different natural scenarios that represent the daily life of families in western culture. It consists of 232 items, 116 devoted to the father figure’s way of parenting and another 116 referring to that of the mother figure. The child rates the parenting of his or her father and mother separately in 29 situations (with four possible answers: 1—never to 4—always), 13 of these situations referring to the obeying of family rules and 16 concerned with disobedience. A global measure is obtained in two dimensions: acceptance/involvement and coercion/imposition, which are similar to the responsiveness and demandingness previously proposed [7]. The family score on the acceptance/involvement dimension was obtained by averaging the answers on affection (He/she is affectionate with me), dialogue (He/she talks to me), indifference (He/she appears indifferent), and apathetic (He/she doesn’t care). The scores on the last two subscales were obtained by inverting the answers, as they were negatively related to the dimension. The family score on the coercion/imposition dimension was obtained by averaging the answers on deprival (He/she deprives me of something), verbal coercion (He/she scolds me), and physical coercion (He/she hits me). The scores on the two dimensions were used to classify the parenting style of each parent as authoritative, indulgent, authoritarian, or neglectful, the sample being divided by the median on each axis of the bi-dimensional model. An average reliability of 0.90 was obtained for the scales of the instrument, namely: overall scale, scale for the father, and scale for the mother, as well as for their dimensions: acceptance/involvement and coercion/imposition. Acceptance/involvement dimension: CR = 0.971. In both instruments, a back translation was used. Our scores are similar to the normative samples of both measures.

2.3. Data Analysis

First of all, all scales’ reliability was analysed from Cronbach’s α. Secondly, the Parental Socialisation Questionnaire (ESPA29) was categorised by classifying the families according to their parenting style (simultaneously bearing in mind two dimensions of the socialisation model, Acceptance/Involvement and Strictness/Imposition) by dichotomising the sample by the median (percentile 50) in the two dimensions. Thus, authoritative parental style scored above the median on both dimensions; indulgent parental style scored above the median on Acceptance/Involvement and below it on Strictness/Imposition; and authoritarian parental style scored below the median on Acceptance/Involvement and above it on Strictness/Imposition. The Neglectful parental style scored below the median on both dimensions. Thirdly, a multivariate factorial analysis (MANOVAs) were applied with the criterion variable hostility, the parenting style (authoritative, indulgent, authoritarian, and neglectful) and sex of adolescents (male and female) being considered as independent variables to test for possible interaction effects. Univariate F tests were later carried out to examine the differences in the variables analysed and the Bonferroni post-hoc test was applied. Finally, a multiple regression analysis was carried out.

3. Results

A first approach to the data was to calculate the number of fathers and mothers integrated in each style of socialization based on the perceptions of the children. In Table 3, the distribution of mothers according to socialization styles is shown. Although the distribution is relatively homogeneous in all styles, it is striking that the predominant styles are negligent and authoritative, in this order.
The same analysis was carried out with respect to the father and a distribution similar to that of the mother was observed with a minimal differences (Table 4).
MANOVA (4 × 2) design, considering the Parenting Styles (A), (a1, indulgent; a2, authoritative; a3, authoritarian; and a4, neglectful), and the Sex of the adolescent (B), (b1, male; b2, female) as the independent variable, and hostility as the dependent variable, the results showed statistically significant differences in the main effect of the parenting style, Λ = 0.958, F(9, 1280.296) = 2.540, p < 0.01. The main effect of sex did not show any statistically significant differences, Λ = 0.998, F(3, 526.000) = 0.390, p > 0.05, and neither did the interaction effect between parenting style and sex, Λ = 0.976, F(9, 1280.296) = 1.405, p > 0.05. Table 5 shows data.
Moreover, ANOVA was performed to analyse the differences in each of the dimensions of hostility (Hostility, Composite Hostility and Cynicism) taking into account the four parenting styles described above. The ANOVA showed statistically significant differences in the dimensions of Hostility, F(3,532) = 4.464, p < 0.01, and Composite Hostility, F(3,532) = 5.140, p < 0.01 (See Table 6). An analysis was then performed to determine which parenting styles had significant differences between them by applying the Bonferroni post-hoc test to compare the means. The results indicated that the children from authoritarian families obtained higher scores on the hostility dimension than children from indulgent families (M = 0.531, SD = 0.126 vs. M = 0.481, SD = 0.154). In the composite hostility dimension, children from authoritative and authoritarian families obtained higher scores than children with indulgent parents (M = 0.623, SD = 0.142, M = 0.619, SD = 0.121 vs. M = 0.563, SD = 0.162), respectively.
A separate regression analysis examining the relative contributions of the parental dimensions (as continued measures rather than dichotomized, separately for mothers and fathers) on hostility has been carried out. The Strictness/Imposition and Acceptance/Involvement from both parents was introduced in the analysis as predictive variables and hostility as the criterion variable. The multiple linear regression analysis, step by step, whose results are presented in Table 7, showed that, in the predictive variables of hostility, two were statistically significant: mother Strictness/Imposition (β = 0.238) and father acceptance/involvement (β = −0.095). The standardized regression coefficients β indicate that all the variables have a certain weight on the criterion variable. According to this statement, the percentages of variance explained (adjusted coefficients of determination) for each of these predictor variables were of average magnitude for the two variables, respectively: 4.6% and 5.5%. High Strictness/Imposition of the mother and low acceptance/involvement of the father were predictive variables of hostility, with its explanatory power being medium, since they explain 5.5% of the variance. The interpretation of the β of each predictor variable on the criterion variable, hostility, for example, would be the following: for each unit added to the variable mother Strictness/Imposition, 0.238 points the hostility.
The multiple linear regression analysis, step by step, whose results are presented in Table 8, showed that in the predictive variables of hostility, four were statistically significant: mother deprivation (β = 0.161), father dialogue (β = −0.187), mother physical coercion (β = 0.132), and father verbal coercion (β = 0.107). The standardized regression coefficients β indicate that all the variables have a certain weight on the criterion variable. According to this statement, the explained variance percentages (adjusted coefficients of determination) for each one of these predictor variables were of average magnitude for the four variables respectively: 3.6%, 6.3%, 8.1%, and 8.8%. High mother deprivation, low father dialogue, high mother physical coercion, and high verbal coercion father were predictive variables of hostility, with their explanatory power being medium, since they explain 8.8% of the variance.
The interpretation of the β of each predictor variable on the criterion variable, hostility, for example, would be the following: for each unit added to the variable mother deprivation, 0.161 points go into hostility.
It was also observed that the Mother’s Strictness/Imposition had the greatest effect on hostility, as well as a low acceptance/involvement of the father. Regarding parental styles, it was observed that mother’s deprivation, physical coercion, and mother’s verbal coercion were the greatest predictors of hostility. No differences were observed depending on the gender.

4. Discussion

The aim of this research was to analyse the relationships between parenting styles and hostility in adolescents. It has been observed that adolescents from indulgent homes (affectionate and not very imposing parents) are more secure, trusting, open to dialogue, and tolerant of their peers (less hostile), which confirms the first hypothesis of this study. As has been highlighted earlier, very little research has been conducted in the field of hostility and parenting styles. The results are also consistent with previous studies which have highlighted the relationships between the different parenting styles and adjustment in teenagers [17,51]. Additionally, in previous studies, it has been observed that the indulgent style is also predominant in other non-English-speaking countries, such as Brazil [52], Italy [53], Portugal [54], which is, in our opinion, very interesting insofar as it incorporates a relatively new dimension into these results: hostility.
In addition, regression analysis provides some nuances that we find very interesting, such as the fact that the Mother’s Strictness/Imposition had the greatest effect on the hostility of the children as well as low acceptance/involvement of the father, which is a novelty in this field of study. Similar results had already been obtained in the area of peer violence in the adolescent period [2,17,25,55], but not in the hostility field. Regarding parental styles, it was observed that mother’s deprivation, physical coercion, and mother’s verbal coercion were the greatest predictors of hostility. These data enrich the previous results that refer to the Mother’s Strictness/Imposition. Regarding gender, although no differences were found in the MANOVA, the results obtained from the regression allow us to start, or move forward, in a very suggestive line of work that can offer excellent results. This would be related to the role that the mother, and not the father, at least not so much, have in hostility and violence. New studies are necessary and if these results are confirmed, it would entail a great enrichment of the prevention and intervention programs, because what they point to is the role of the mother in the prevention of hostility and violence.
Secondly, the hostility and cynicism dimensions have been seen to be the highest in adolescents brought up in families in which the authoritarian style predominates, which therefore confirms part of the second hypothesis in that anger and aggression are higher in the authoritarian style. The third dimension, composite hostility, is however similar in teenagers brought up both in families in which the authoritative style predominates and in those where the authoritarian style prevails. Therefore, children from authoritarian families and from authoritative families, respectively, are the ones who were seen to be more mistrustful and hostile than adolescents from indulgent families. An upbringing that is based on patterns of an imposing nature and the absence of dialogue or the chance to reason fosters hostile attitudes in children, which are related, either implicitly or explicitly, with aggressive behaviour. When children perceive irritability, greater control, and an attitude of rejection in their parents, their aggressive behaviours and their frustration increase because they feel their freedom is being restricted. In the case of authoritative families, expressing affection reduces the unease associated with a high level of control imposed by the parents, although it results in high levels of composite hostility in their children. An authoritarian upbringing is a risk factor for hostile behaviours in youngsters due to the possibility of them reproducing the patterns of irritability, intolerance, and lack of empathy existing within the family context in other contexts, such as at school and/or with a partner [26,56,57].
These results are also consistent with those previously obtained [58] on the importance of family support and protection to foster safe and trusting relationships in children instead of rejection and hostility as promoters of insecure interaction based on expectations of mistrust, aggression, and hostility in their relationships with others. They are also in line with the other results [59] on the direct relationship between hostile behaviour and the use of punitive strategies in controlling the child’s behaviour, and other study [60] about negative communication and mistrust. We believe these results are interesting and deserve to be explored in greater depth due to their theoretical and practical implications.
Our results, however, do not support the findings of other studies carried out in English-speaking contexts, in which it is concluded that the authoritative parenting style (high affection and high imposition) is related to better psychological and social adjustment of one’s children [17,61]. In this regard, according to our results, Spanish adolescents from authoritative families are the ones that obtain the highest scores on composite hostility, and therefore the binomial high control–high affection does not appear to work properly in the Spanish context, contrary to what occurs in the English-speaking context. In this respect, it is observed that the negative effects of control are not mitigated by affection and responsiveness, so that strictness and excessive control, although accompanied by affection, appear to encourage hostile and malicious attitudes, which could undermine their socio-moral and affective development and give rise to forms of relationships based on mistrust and a lack of empathy from an early age. This is a result that should be explored further from an intercultural perspective and taking into account, more specifically, the variable hostility, because in previous studies mentioned earlier hostility is a collateral dimension.

5. Limitations

One of the limitations of this study could be the fact that families are classified according to their parenting style, based exclusively on the answers given by their children. Nevertheless, there is empirical evidence showing that children’s answers tend to be less biased due to social desirability than the answers given by parents [62]. Finally, the cross-cutting nature of the study does not allow conclusions to be drawn about the causal relationships between the study variables and therefore, as noted earlier, we suggest that longitudinal study designs should be used to test the relationship with, first, cardiovascular pathology, and second, the transmission of violence from one context to another. The ultimate purpose would be to design health-promoting preventive strategies.
Despite these limitations, the results of this study highlight the importance of parenting in adolescents’ psychological and emotional adjustment, while also providing empirical evidence for the incipient research being carried out in our country in the field of parenting. The family matters; it is an asset for the adolescent’s positive development if it provides support and affection, resulting in a reduction in the frequency of emotional and behavioural disorders [63,64]. In this regard, there is clearly a need to consider family assets in our cultural setting when designing intervention programmes [65] that are aimed at improving educational practices, the processes of parental communication, familial acceptance, and involvement. They should also include, in addition to positive parenting, the development of school and community resources, with the aim of providing the support and experiences needed to foster the full development and adjustment of our teenagers.

Author Contributions

G.M. and F.P. designed the research. All authors performed the research. M.d.M.A. collected data, and G.M. analyzed the data. All authors wrote the paper and read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Cerezo, F.; Sánchez, C.; Ruiz, C.; Arense, J.J. Adolescents and preadolescents’ Roles on Bullying, and Its Relation with Social Climate and Parenting Styles. Rev. Psicodidáct. 2015, 20, 139–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Cerezo, F.; Ruiz, C.; Sánchez, C.; Arense, J.J. Dimensions of parenting styles, social climate, and bullying victims in primary and secondary education. Psicothema 2018, 30, 59–65. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  3. Pinquart, M. Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: An updated meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 2017, 53, 873–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Samper-García, P.; Mestre-Escrivá, V.; Malonda, E.; Mesurado, B. Victimización en la escuela: relación de la crianza y variables funcionales disfuncionales del desarrollo. Anales de Psicología 2015, 31, 849–858. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar]
  5. Musitu, G.; Cava, M.J. El rol del apoyo social en el ajuste de los adolescentes. Interv. Psicosoc. 2002, 11, 1–14. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar]
  6. Lamborn, S.D.; Mounts, N.S.; Steinberg, L.; Dornbusch, S.M. Patterns of competence and adjustment among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Dev. 1991, 62, 1049–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Maccoby, E.; Martin, J. Socialization in the context of the family: parent-child interaction. In Handbook of Child Psychology; Hetherington, E.M., Mussen, P.H., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1983; Volume 4, pp. 1–101. [Google Scholar]
  8. Steinberg, L.; Lamborn, S.D.; Darling, N.; Mounts, N.S.; Dornbusch, S.M. Over-time changes in adjustment and competence among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 754–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Symonds, P.M. The Psychology of Parent-Child Relationships; Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, NY, USA, 1939. [Google Scholar]
  10. Baldwin, A.L. Behavior and Development in Childhood; Dryden Press: New York, NY, USA, 1955. [Google Scholar]
  11. Becker, W.C. Consequences of different kinds of parental discipline. In Review of Child Development Research; Hoffman, M.L., Hoffman, W., Eds.; Russell Sage Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 1964; Volume 1, pp. 169–208. [Google Scholar]
  12. Schaefer, E.S. A circumflex model for maternal behavior. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1959, 59, 226–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Sears, R.R.; MacCoby, E.; Levin, H. Patterns of Child Rearing; Stanford University Press: Redwood City, CA, USA, 1957. [Google Scholar]
  14. Baumrind, D. Child cares practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 1967, 75, 43–88. [Google Scholar]
  15. Baumrind, D. Current patterns of parental authority. Dev. Psychol. 1971, 4, 1–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. García, F.; Gracia, E. ¿Qué estilo de socialización parental es el idóneo en España? Un estudio con niños y adolescentes de 10 a 14 años. Infanc. Aprendiz. 2010, 33, 365–384. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Fuentes, M.C.; García, F.; Gracia, E.; Alarcón, A. Los estilos parentales de socialización y el ajuste psicológico. Un estudio con adolescentes españoles. Rev. Psicodidáct. 2015, 20, 117–138. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Yazdani, S.; Daryei, G. Parenting styles and psychosocial adjustment of gifted and normal adolescents. Pacific Science Review. Hum. Soc. Sci. 2016, 2, 100–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Oliva, A.; Palacios, J. Familia y escuela: Padres y profesores. In Familia y Desarrollo Humano; Rodrigo, M., Palacios, J., Eds.; Alianza Editorial: Madrid, Spain, 1998; pp. 333–350. [Google Scholar]
  20. Aihie, O.N. Perceived Parenting Styles as Correlates of Self-Esteem among Adolescents in Secondary Schools. IJEB 2016, 4, 1. [Google Scholar]
  21. Biirah, J.; Anika, A.A. Perception of Learners on Pareting System and ITS Influence on Academic Performace among Secondary Schools Students of Bukonzo country, Uganda. WJER 2018, 5, 1. [Google Scholar]
  22. Ali, I.; Ali, M.; Badghish, S.; Baazeem, T.A.S. Examining the role of childhood experiences in developing altruistic and knowledge sharing behaviors among children in their later life: A Partial Least Squares (PLS) PathModeling Approach. Sustainability 2018, 10, 292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Smith, L.; López Sánchez, G.F.; Díaz, A.; Stubbs, B.; Dowling, M.; Scruton, A.; Roberts, J.; Johnstone, J.; Pardhan, S. Barriers and Facilitators of Physical Activity in Children of a South Asian Ethnicity. Sustainability 2018, 10, 761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Gentina, E.; Singh, P. How national culture and parental style affect the process of adolescents ecological resocialization. Sustainability 2015, 7, 7581–7603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Fuentes, M.C.; Alarcón, A.; Gracia, E.; García, F. El ajuste escolar en los adolescentes españoles: Influencia de la socialización parental. Cult. Educ. 2015, 27, 1–32. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Crespo-Ramos, S.; Romero-Abrio, A.; Martínez-Ferrer, B.; Musitu, G. Variables psicosociales y violencia escolar en la adolescencia. Psychosoc. Interv. 2017, 26, 125–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Dwairy, M.A.; Achoui, M. Introduction to three cross-regional research studies on parenting styles, individuation, and mental health in Arab societies. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2006, 37, 221–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Chao, R.K. Extending research on the consequences of parenting style for Chinese Americans and European Americans. Child Dev. 2001, 72, 1832–1843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Hoff, E.; Laursen, B.; Tardif, T. Socioeconomic status and parenting. In Handbook of Parenting: Biology and Ecology of Parenting; Bornstein, M.H., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2002; pp. 231–252. [Google Scholar]
  30. Garaigordobil, M.; Aliri, J. Parental socialization styles, parents educational level, and sexist attitudes in adolescence. Span J. Psychol. 2012, 15, 592–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Cerezo, M.T.; Casanova, P.F.; de la Torre, M.J.; Carpio, M.V. Estilos educativos paternos y estrategias de aprendizaje en alumnos de educación secundaria. EJEP 2011, 4, 51–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Calafat, A.; García, F.; Juan, M.; Becoña, E.; Fernández-Hermida, J.R. Which parenting style is more protective against adolescent substance use? Evidence within the European context. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014, 138, 185–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Caamaño, D.P.; Castro, L.L.; Oviedo, P.O. La prevención e intervención en el cyberbullying: ¿qué papel juegan las familias? Rev. Estudios Investig. Psicol. Educ. 2017, 5, 122–126. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Linares, M.C.; Rusillo, M.T.; Cruz, M.J.T.; Fernández, M.V.C.; Arias, P.F.C. Prácticas educativas paternas y problemas internalizantes y externalizantes en adolescentes españoles. Psicothema 2011, 23, 654–659. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar]
  35. García-Linares, M.C.; de la Torre, M.J.; Carpio, M.V.; Cerezo, M.T.; Casanova, P.F. Consistency/inconsistency in parental and maternal parenting styles and daily stress in adolescence. Rev. Psicodidáct. 2014, 19, 307–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Alonso-Geta, P.M.P. La socialización parental en padres españoles con hijos de 6 a 14 años. Psicothema 2012, 24, 371–376. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  37. Gracia, E.; Fuentes, M.C.; García, F.; Lila, M. Perceived neighborhood violence, parenting styles, and developmental outcomes among Spanish adolescents. J. Community Psychol. 2012, 40, 1004–1021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Smith, T.W.; Pope, M.K. Cynical hostility as a health risk: current status and future directions. Soc. Behav. Pers. 1990, 5, 77–88. [Google Scholar]
  39. Farrell, A.K.; Simpson, J.A. Effects of relationship functioning on the biological experience of stress and physical health. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 13, 49–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Ghobari, B.; Haddadi, A.A. Relation among quality of attachment, hostility and interpersonal sensitivity in college students. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2011, 30, 192–196. [Google Scholar]
  41. Barefoot, J.C. Developments in the measurement of Hostility. In Hostility, Coping and Health; Friedman, H.S., Ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, WA, USA, 1992; pp. 13–21. [Google Scholar]
  42. Palmero, F. La ira y la agresión. In En Psicología Experimental; Violencia y Sociedad Llar Digital: Castellón, Spain, 2008; pp. 13–42. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar]
  43. Ahn, J.; Lee, S. Peer attachment, perceived parenting style, self-concept, and school adjustments in adolescents with chronic illness. Asian Nurs. Res. 2016, 10, 300–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Lund, I.; Scheffels, J. Adolescent perceptions of dissuasive sticks: A web survey among 16–20 year olds in Norway. BMC Public Health 2018, 18, 974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Valente, J.Y.; Cogo-Moreira, H.; Sanchez, Z.M. Gradient of association between parenting styles and patterns of drug use in adolescence: A latent class analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2017, 180, 272–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Cook, W.W.U.; Medley, M. Proposed hostility and pharisaic—Virtue scales for the MMPI. J. Appl Psychol. 1954, 38, 414–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bermúdez, J.; Sánchez-Elvira, A.; Fernández, E. Contenido del Inventario de Hostilidad de Cook y Medley (ICM): Implicaciones pronocoronarias. In Proceedings of the IV Congreso de Evaluación Psicológica, Santiago de Compostela, Spain, 24 September 1994. (In Spanish). [Google Scholar]
  48. Boyle, S.H.; Williams, R.B.; Mark, D.B.; Brummett, B.H.; Siegler, I.C.; Helms, M.J.; Barefoot, J.C. Hostility as a predictor of survival in patients with coronary artery disease. Psychosom. Med. 2004, 66, 629–632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Musitu, G.; García, F. ESPA29: Escala de Estilos de Socialización Parental en la Adolescencia, 2nd ed.; TEA: Madrid, Spain, 2001. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar]
  50. Musitu, G.; García, F. Consecuencias de la socialización familiar en la cultura española. Psicothema 2004, 16, 297–302. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar]
  51. Gracia, E.; Herrero, J. Is it considered violence? The acceptability of physical punishment of children in Europe: A multilevel analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 2008, 70, 210–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Martínez, I.; García, J.F. Internalization of values and self-esteem among Brazilian teenagers form authoritative, indulgent, authoritarian, and neglectful homes. Adolescence 2008, 43, 13–29. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  53. Di Maggio, R.; Zappulla, C. Mothering, fathering, and Italian adolescents’ problem behaviors and life satisfaction: Dimensional and typological approach. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2014, 23, 567–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Rodrigues, Y.; Veiga, F.; Fuentes, M.C.; García, F. Parenting and adolescent’s self-esteem: The Portuguese context. Rev. Psicodidáct. 2013, 18, 395–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Varela, R.M.; Elena, M.; Martínez, B. Violencia escolar: Un análisis desde los diferentes contextos de interacción. Psychosoc. Interven. 2013, 22, 25–32. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Menesini, E.; Nocentini, A.; Camodeca, M. Bullying among siblings: The role of personality and relational variables. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2010, 28, 921–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Ortega, R.; Mora-Merchan, J.A. Social Networks, ICTs and cyberbullying. In Cyberbullying: A Cross-National Comparison; Verlag Empirische Padagogik: Landau, Germany, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  58. Olweus, D. Familial and temperamental determinants of aggressive behavior in adolescent boys: A causal analysis. Dev. Psychol. 1980, 16, 644–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Paschall, M.J.; Ringwalt, C.L.; Flewelling, R. Effects of parenting, father absence and affiliation with delinquent peers on delinquent behavior among African American male adolescents. Adolescence 2003, 38, 15–43. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  60. O’Connor, T. The effects of parenting reconsidered: findings, challenges and applications. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2002, 43, 555–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Steinberg, L.; Morris, A.S. Adolescent Development. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2001, 52, 83–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Barry, C.T.; Frick, J.P.; Grafeman, S.J. Child versus parent reports of parenting practices: Implications for the conceptualization of child behavioral and emotional problems. Assessment 2008, 15, 294–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Bowers, E.P.; Von Eye, A.; Lerner, J.V.; Arbeit, M.R.; Weiner, M.B.; Chase, P.; Agans, J.P. The role of ecological assets in positive and problematic developmental trajectories. J. Adolesc. 2011, 34, 1151–1165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Parra, A.; Oliva, A. La familia como contexto positivo para el desarrollo adolescente. In Desarrollo Positivo Adolescente; Oliva, A., Ed.; Síntesis: Madrid, Spain, 2015; pp. 41–58. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar]
  65. Capano, A.; Ubach, A. Estilos parentales, parentalidad positiva y formación de padres. Ciencias Psicol. 2013, 7, 83–95. (In Spanish) [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Participants’ age and frequency.
Table 1. Participants’ age and frequency.
FrequencyPercentageValid PercentageAccumulated Percentage
Valid12 years478.88.88.8
13 years8916.616.625.4
14 years9417.517.542.9
15 years7313.613.656.5
16 years7714.414.470.9
17 years5911.011.081.9
18 or more year9718.118.1100.0
Total536100.0100.0
Table 2. Number of subjects, frequency, and percentage per age group.
Table 2. Number of subjects, frequency, and percentage per age group.
FrequencyPercentageValid PercentageAccumulated Percentage
ValidFrom 12 to 14 years old23042.942.942.9
From 15 to 16 years old15028.028.070.9
From 17 to 18 or more years old15629.129.1100.0
Total536100.0100.0
Table 3. Distribution of mothers according to the Parental Style.
Table 3. Distribution of mothers according to the Parental Style.
AuthoritativeIndulgentAuthoritarianNeglectfulTotal
Frequency156111107162536
Percentage29.120.72030.2100
Acceptance/Involvement
M3.593.522.842.793.18
SD0.200.200.320.360.47
Strictness/Imposition
M2.071.492.031.461.76
SD0.250.190.250.180.36
Note: The scores on acceptance/involvement and strictness/imposition range from 1 to 4.
Table 4. Distribution of mothers according to the Parental Style.
Table 4. Distribution of mothers according to the Parental Style.
AuthoritativeIndulgentAuthoritarianNeglectfulTotal
Frequency15898103153512
Percentage29.518.319.228.5100
Acceptance/Involvement
M3.503.412.672.513.02
DT0.250.230.330.450.56
Strictness /Imposition
M2.061.462.061.391.74
DT0.270.190.300.200.40
Note: The scores on acceptance/involvement and strictness/imposition range from 1 to 4.
Table 5. MANOVA (4a × 2b Parenting Styles and Sex) and Hostility.
Table 5. MANOVA (4a × 2b Parenting Styles and Sex) and Hostility.
VariablesHostility
ΛFglentreglerrorp
(A) Parenting Style a0.9582.54091280.2960.007 **
(B) Sex b0.9980.39035260.760
A × B0.9761.40591280.2960.181
aa1, indulgent, a2, authoritative, a3, authoritarian, a4, neglectful; bb1, male, b2, female; ** p < 0.01.
Table 6. Mean (Standard deviation), F values, and Bonferroni post hoc test# between Parenting Styles and Dimensions of Hostility.
Table 6. Mean (Standard deviation), F values, and Bonferroni post hoc test# between Parenting Styles and Dimensions of Hostility.
Parenting Style
AuthoritativeIndulgentAuthoritarianNeglectfulF(3,532)
Hostility
Hostility0.526
(0.133)
0.481
(0.154)
0.531
(0.126)
0.487
(0.139)
4.464 **
Composite Hostility0.623
(0.142)
0.563
(0.162)
0.619
(0.121)
0.583
(0.152)
5.140 **
Cynicism0.628
(0.187)
0.575
(0.208)
0.636
(0.176)
0.613
(0.192)
2.333
#α = 0.05, 1 > 2; ** p < 0.01.
Table 7. Stepwise multiple regression analysis for predictive variables of hostility for the sample as a whole.
Table 7. Stepwise multiple regression analysis for predictive variables of hostility for the sample as a whole.
Non-Typified CoefficientsTypified Coefficientstp
FactorsBTypical errorβ
1(Constant)0.3610.030 12.180.000R2 = 4.3%
Mother Strictness/Imposition0.0820.0160.2154.970.000F = 24.68
2(Constant)0.4170.039 10.580.000R2 = 5.5%
Father Strictness/Imposition0.0910.0170.2385.360.000F = 14.70
Father Acceptance/Involvement−0.0240.011−0.095−2.140.033
Note: p < 0.05.
Table 8. Stepwise multiple regression analysis for predictive variables of hostility for the sample as a whole.
Table 8. Stepwise multiple regression analysis for predictive variables of hostility for the sample as a whole.
No Typified CoefficientsTypified Coefficientstp
FactorsBTypical errorβ
1(Constant)0.4240.019 21.750.000R2 = 3.6%
Mother deprivation0.0470.0110.1914.390.000F = 19.24
2(Constant)0.4840.025 19.520.000R2 = 6.3%
Mother deprivation0.0590.0110.2395.350.000F = 17.23
Father dialogue−0.0320.008−0.172−3.840.000
3(Constant)0.4040.036 11.310.000R2 = 8.1%
Mother deprivation0.0520.0110.2114.660.000F = 14.85
Father dialogue−0.0300.008−0.164−3.690.000
Mother physical coercion0.0830.0270.1343.090.002
4(Constant)0.3830.037 10.320.000R2 = 8.8%
Mother deprivation0.0400.0130.1613.160.002F = 12.27
Father dialogue−0.0350.008−0.187−4.100.000
Mother physical coercion0.0810.0270.1323.040.002
Father verbal coercion0.0230.0110.1072.060.040
Note: p < 0.05.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Aymerich, M.D.M.; Musitu, G.; Palmero, F. Family Socialisation Styles and Hostility in the Adolescent Population. Sustainability 2018, 10, 2962. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/su10092962

AMA Style

Aymerich MDM, Musitu G, Palmero F. Family Socialisation Styles and Hostility in the Adolescent Population. Sustainability. 2018; 10(9):2962. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/su10092962

Chicago/Turabian Style

Aymerich, María Del Milagro, Gonzalo Musitu, and Francisco Palmero. 2018. "Family Socialisation Styles and Hostility in the Adolescent Population" Sustainability 10, no. 9: 2962. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3390/su10092962

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop